Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory...

51
Copyright © 2020 Wutz et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed. Research Articles: Behavioral/Cognitive Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic attention between multiple objects and ensembles https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0231-20.2020 Cite as: J. Neurosci 2020; 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0231-20.2020 Received: 30 January 2020 Revised: 24 June 2020 Accepted: 29 June 2020 This Early Release article has been peer-reviewed and accepted, but has not been through the composition and copyediting processes. The final version may differ slightly in style or formatting and will contain links to any extended data. Alerts: Sign up at www.jneurosci.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fully formatted version of this article is published.

Transcript of Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory...

Page 1: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

Copyright © 2020 Wutz et al.This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in anymedium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

Research Articles: Behavioral/Cognitive

Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflectdynamic attention between multiple objectsand ensembles

https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0231-20.2020

Cite as: J. Neurosci 2020; 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0231-20.2020

Received: 30 January 2020Revised: 24 June 2020Accepted: 29 June 2020

This Early Release article has been peer-reviewed and accepted, but has not been throughthe composition and copyediting processes. The final version may differ slightly in style orformatting and will contain links to any extended data.

Alerts: Sign up at www.jneurosci.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fullyformatted version of this article is published.

Page 2: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

1

Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic attention between multiple 1 objects and ensembles 2 3 Abbreviated title: Bursts during object-ensemble attention 4 5 Andreas Wutz1,2, Agnese Zazio3 and Nathan Weisz1 6 7 1 Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstrasse 34, 8 5020 Salzburg, Austria 9 2 The Picower Institute for Learning & Memory, Massachusetts Institute of 10 Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139; 11 3 Cognitive Neuroscience Section, IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni di Dio 12 Fatebenefratelli, Via Pilastroni 4, 25125 Brescia, Italy 13 14 Corresponding author: 15 Andreas Wutz 16 Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstrasse 34, 17 5020 Salzburg, Austria 18 +43 (0) 662 / 8044 – 5100 19 [email protected] 20 21 Conflict of interest statement: 22 “The authors declare no competing financial interests.” 23 24 Acknowledgements: 25 This research was supported by a Lise-Meitner grant from the Austrian Science Fund 26 (FWF) awarded to A.W. (grant agreement number: M2496). 27

Page 3: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

2

Abstract 28 The visual system employs two complimentary strategies to process multiple objects 29 simultaneously within a scene and update their spatial positions in real-time. It either 30 uses selective attention to individuate a complex, dynamic scene into a few focal 31 objects (i.e. object individuation) or it represents multiple objects as an ensemble by 32 distributing attention more globally across the scene (i.e. ensemble grouping). Neural 33 oscillations may be a key signature for focal object individuation vs. distributed 34 ensemble grouping, because they are thought to regulate neural excitability over 35 visual areas through inhibitory control mechanisms. We recorded whole-head 36 magneto-encephalography data (MEG) during a multiple-object tracking paradigm 37 (MOT), in which human participants (13 female, 11 male) switched between different 38 instructions for object individuation and ensemble grouping on different trials. The 39 stimuli, responses and the demand to keep track of multiple spatial locations over 40 time were held constant between the two conditions. We observed increased alpha-41 band power (9-13 Hz) packed into oscillatory bursts in bilateral inferior parietal cortex 42 during multiple-object processing. Single-trial analysis revealed greater burst 43 occurrences on object individuation vs. ensemble grouping trials. By contrast, we 44 found no differences using standard analyses on across-trials averaged alpha-band 45 power. Moreover, the bursting effects occurred only below/at - but not above - the 46 typical capacity limits for multiple-object processing (at ~4 objects). Our findings 47 reveal the real-time neural correlates underlying the dynamic processing of multiple-48 object scenarios, which are modulated by grouping strategies and capacity. They 49 support a rhythmic, alpha-pulsed organization of dynamic attention to multiple 50 objects and ensembles. 51 52

Page 4: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

3

Significance statement 53 Dynamic multiple-object scenarios are an important problem in real-world and 54 computer vision. They require keeping track of multiple objects as they move through 55 space and time. Such problems can be solved in two ways: One can individuate a 56 scene object by object, or alternatively group objects into ensembles. We observed 57 greater occurrences of alpha-oscillatory burst events in parietal cortex for processing 58 objects vs. ensembles and below/at vs. above processing capacity. These results 59 demonstrate a unique top-down mechanism by which the brain dynamically adjusts 60 its computational level between objects and ensembles. They help to explain how the 61 brain copes with its capacity limitations in real-time environments and may lead the 62 way to technological innovations for time-critical video analysis in computer vision. 63

Page 5: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

4

Introduction 64 Dynamic perceptual experiences require the visual system to process multiple 65 objects simultaneously within a scene and update them from moment to moment. 66 Such situations provide a high degree of ecological validity for real-world visual 67 cognition, because we often encounter them in everyday-life, for example in traffic 68 situations or in team sports (Scholl, 2009). Moreover, dynamic multiple-object 69 scenarios are an important problem in computer vision with wide applications in the 70 time-critical video analysis for robotics or autonomous driving (Kazanovich and 71 Borisyuk, 2006; Xiang, Alahi and Savarese, 2015). An ideal task to investigate this 72 ability in experimental settings is multiple-object tracking (MOT, Pylyshyn and Storm, 73 1988). In a typical MOT trial, participants need to keep track of multiple, randomly 74 moving target-objects amongst distractors for a period of several seconds (Figure 75 1A). 76 77 Previous research has identified two distinct key strategies for the visual system to 78 deal with dynamic multiple-object problems. A first important mechanism for MOT is 79 object individuation, which involves selecting features from a crowded scene, binding 80 them into an object and individuating it from other objects (for reviews see Xu and 81 Chun, 2009; Wutz and Melcher, 2014). Individuation provides local detail about 82 specific objects in a scene, but only a few objects (typically around 4) can be 83 individuated at once (Cowan, 2010) due to limited selective attention (Cavanagh and 84 Alvarez, 2005). Indeed, object-based attention and individuation is a central 85 component to visual cognition, since its capacity limitations set the bounds for many 86 visual tasks (e.g. enumeration (Jevons, 1871), visual working memory (Luck and 87 Vogel, 1997), MOT (Trick and Pylyshyn, 1994), for review see Piazza et al., 2011). 88 For MOT, individuation capacity is often probed by indicating the spatial location of 89 one object location from the pool of target-objects at trial end (i.e. partial report, 90 Figure 1B). 91

Page 6: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

5

92 A second core mechanism for MOT is to represent multiple objects as an ensemble 93 or group (Yantis, 1992; Merkel et al., 2014, 2015; Merkel, Hopf and Schoenfeld, 94 2017). In contrast to focal attention to individual objects, recent reports have 95 highlighted the ability for information integration and compression in the visual scene 96 analysis by computing group-level ensemble statistics across multiple objects (for 97 reviews see Alvarez, 2011; Whitney and Yamanashi Leib, 2018). For example, the 98 average size (Ariely, 2001; Chong and Treisman, 2003), orientation (Parkes et al., 99 2001) and location (Alvarez and Oliva, 2008) of many objects can be reported with 100 high precision and velocity, and often better compared to the properties of individual 101 objects. Critically, ensemble computations exploit higher-order regularities diagnostic 102 of the large-scale scene-layout (its ‘gist’, (Torralba and Oliva 2003;, Greene and 103 Oliva, 2009), which can be computed even under conditions of reduced or withdrawn 104 attention to individual, focal objects and thus overcome its typical capacity limitations 105 (Alvarez and Oliva, 2009; Corbett and Oriet, 2011). A typical ensemble-statistic for 106 MOT is the average location of all target-objects (i.e. their centroid, Figure 1B). 107 108 Currently, there is no clear consensus about the neural correlates underlying 109 dynamic multiple-object problems (like MOT) and its processing differences in terms 110 of focal object-level vs. distributed ensemble-level attention. Likewise, it is not clear 111 how its different processing capacity limitations are implemented in neural function 112 and whether they are due to “online” perceptual or “offline” memory-related 113 mechanisms. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and magneto-114 encephalography (MEG) work on multiple-object attention on the level of object 115 individuation suggests neural substrates in parietal cortex (Todd and Marois, 2004; 116 Xu and Chun, 2009; Rouhinen et al., 2013). In terms of temporal dynamics, neural 117 oscillations in the alpha-frequency band (9-13 Hz) are a key candidate because they 118 are associated with the functional inhibition of neural processes (Klimesch, Sauseng 119

Page 7: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

6

and Hanslmayr, 2007; Haegens et al., 2011). In the case of dynamic object 120 individuation, neural excitation-inhibition needs to be regulated focally, in order to 121 carve-out discrete objects from continuous visual inputs (e.g. by modulating signal-122 to-noise in saliency maps, (Quiles et al., 2011; Jensen, Bonnefond and VanRullen, 123 2012; Wutz et al., 2014) and keep distinct objects separate in spatial working 124 memory (Palva and Palva, 2007; Jensen et al., 2014). By contrast ensemble 125 computations require more global inhibition, because attention is distributed across 126 the visual scene and objects are represented as a group (Torralba and Oliva 2003; 127 Alvarez, 2011). 128 129 We aimed to identify the neural dynamics underlying object- and ensemble-level 130 computations during MOT by means of MEG recordings. We focused on single-trial 131 activity changes in neural oscillations, e.g. oscillatory burst events (Lundqvist et al., 132 2016, for review see van Ede et al., 2018), because they account for relevant trial-by-133 trial variability in neural timing expected in time-critical tasks, such as MOT. The 134 participants switched between instructions to perform object individuation (i.e. partial 135 report) or ensemble processing (i.e. centroid / averaging task, Figure 1 A, B) in 136 counter-balanced blocks and in a dual-task condition, in which the task demands 137 were randomly intermixed in each block and cued after each trial, in order to control 138 for top-down influences. This strategy allowed us to match processing in the two 139 tasks in visual stimuli, responses and the requirement to keep track of multiple 140 spatial locations over time, while preserving critical differences in the dynamic 141 deployment of attention to individual objects vs. group-level ensembles during 142 dynamic multiple-object problems. 143 144 Materials and Methods 145 All procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg. 146

Page 8: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

7

Participants 147 24 participants (13 female; mean age ± SD, 25.9 ± 5.3 y; 20 right-handed) took part 148 in the experiment. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, gave 149 written informed consent before the experiment, and received a monetary 150 compensation or course credits. 151 Apparatus 152 MEG Data Acquisition 153 Electrophysiological activity was recorded with a whole-head MEG system with 102 154 magnetometers and 204 planar gradiometers (Neuromag306; Elekta), sampled at 155 1,000 Hz in a magnetically shielded room. For each participant, a head frame 156 coordinate reference was defined before the experiment by digitizing the cardinal 157 points of the head (nasion and left and right preauricular points), the location of five 158 head position indicator (HPI) coils, and a minimum of 200 other head shape samples 159 (3Space Fastrack; Polhemus). The head position within the MEG helmet (relative to 160 the HPI coils and the MEG sensors) was controlled before each block to ensure that 161 no large movements occurred during the data acquisition. 162 Stimulus Presentation 163 Stimuli were generated using MATLAB 8.5 (MathWorks) and Psychophysics 164 Toolbox, version 3 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). A DLP projector (PROPixx; VPixx 165 Technologies) showed the stimuli at a refresh rate of 120 Hz centered onto a 166 translucent screen (25 horizontal × 16 vertical DVA). The screen was located in front 167 of the participant (viewing distance, 125 cm) within the dimly lit, magnetically 168 shielded MEG room. Stimulus timing was controlled with a data and video processing 169 peripheral (DATAPixx; VPixx Technologies) and monitored via a photo diode placed 170 at the upper left corner of the projection screen. The delay between trigger and 171 stimulation onset was corrected with this method. 172

Page 9: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

8

Stimuli and Experimental Design 173 Each trial started with the central presentation of a red fixation cross (0.25 visual 174 angle, DVA) on a gray background, of a red frame indicating the bounds of the 175 tracking display (6 x 6 DVA) and of 12 white dots (0.25 DVA) presented at random, 176 non-overlapping locations within the inner 3 x 3 DVA of the display for 1 s duration. 177 Then, a subset of the white dots (2, 4 or 8 depending on the target-object pool per 178 trial) was cued as task-relevant objects by presenting a green circle (0.5 DVA) 179 around them for 1-s duration. The total number of presented items was held 180 constant, in order to control for visual load and track set size effects based 181 exclusively on the number of task-relevant items. Subsequently, the cue was 182 removed, rendering task-relevant and -irrelevant objects indistinguishable from each 183 other, and the dots started to move with 1 DVA/s speed in random directions on 184 linear paths. The motion paths were generated offline before the experiment such 185 that the moving dots did not collide with each other (0.5 DVA minimal center-to-186 center distance) or with the display edges. The duration of the motion epoch was 187 randomly jittered to last between 2-3 s duration per trial such that the objects’ final 188 spatial positions were unpredictable during tracking. It was followed by a delay epoch 189 (for 1 s duration), during which only the fixation cross and the tracking boundary were 190 presented and the subjects had to remember the target dot locations (Figure 1A). At 191 trial end, a response screen was presented, which depended on the performed task 192 (i.e. object individuation, ensemble processing). On individuation trials, all distractor 193 dots and all except for one, a priori unknown target dot were presented after the 194 delay and the subjects’ task was to indicate the location of the missing target dot 195 from the pool of target-objects via mouse-click (i.e. partial report). On ensemble 196 averaging trials, all distractor dots were reshown and the subject’s task was to 197 indicate the centroid location of the target dots (Figure 1B). The two tasks were run in 198 counter-balanced blocks and in a dual-task condition, which controls for strategic 199 influences, because the task demands were randomly intermixed within a block and 200

Page 10: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

9

cued after each trial. We used two blocks each comprising 120 trials for each 201 condition (240 trials total for the two single-tasks and the dual-task) with all set sizes 202 (2, 4, 8 objects) balanced and randomly intermixed within a block. The experiment 203 lasted about 2.5 h. 204 Behavioral Data Analysis 205 We quantified behavioral performance in terms of percent correct trials, in which the 206 difference between the responded- and the probed screen location (i.e. partial report 207 item or centroid) was below 0.5 DVA. Performance was contrasted between the 208 tasks and across the different object-pools with a two-way, repeated-measures 209 analysis of variance (ANOVA). The performance difference per task between single- 210 vs. dual-task conditions was tested with dependent-samples t-test. All error bars (for 211 both behavioral and MEG measures) show the SEM for repeated measures. The 212 mean between conditions was subtracted from the data in each condition before 213 calculating the SE. The resulting error estimate was bias corrected by the number of 214 conditions [M, multiplied by √(M/(M − 1))] (Morey, 2008). 215 Eye-Movement Data Analysis 216 Eye-movements were recorded with a 2-kHz, binocular eye-tracker (TRACKPixx; 217 VPixx Technologies). We used an automated algorithm for micro-saccade detection 218 that computes thresholds based on velocity changes from the horizontal and vertical 219 eye-tracker components (Engbert and Kliegl, 2003). Micro-saccades were identified 220 as binocular events that exceeded a given duration and velocity threshold. The 221 minimum micro-saccade duration was 12 ms and the velocity threshold was set at 6 222 times the noise level per trial and horizontal / vertical component. Trials that 223 contained blinks were excluded from the analysis prior to micro-saccade detection 224 (on average 44 ± 36 SD trials were excluded). Due to technical difficulties, eye-225 tracking data was only available for 13 out of 24 subjects. On average, the 226 participants performed 209 micro-saccades over the course of the experiment 227

Page 11: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

10

(SD=202, min=12, max=534). For each participant, we calculated the difference in 228 the vertical and horizontal eye-gaze position before and after each micro-saccade 229 during the motion- and jitter epochs, in order to quantify the micro-saccade size per 230 trial. Vertical and horizontal eye gaze components were combined via vector 231 addition. Moreover, we averaged the simultaneously recorded alpha power over the 232 time-samples corresponding to micro-saccades per trial. Then, we correlated alpha 233 power and micro-saccade size across all micro-saccade occurrences for each 234 participant separately. The false-discovery rate procedure by Benjamini and 235 Hochberg (1995) was used to correct for multiple comparisons across participants. 236 MEG Data Analysis 237 Data preprocessing 238 The data were analyzed using custom-built MATLAB code (MATLAB 9.5; 239 MathWorks) and the FieldTrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011)). We use a signal 240 space separation algorithm implemented in the Maxfilter program (version 2.2.15) 241 provided by the MEG manufacturer to remove external noise from the MEG signal 242 (mainly 16.6 Hz, which is the alternating current used by the railway electrification 243 systems in Austria, Germany and Switzerland, and 50 Hz plus harmonics, which is 244 line noise in Europe) and realign data to a common standard head position (trans 245 default Maxfilter parameter) across different blocks based on the measured head 246 position at the beginning of each block. Data were segmented from 1.5 s before to 6 247 s after fixation onset and down-sampled off-line to 250 Hz. In separate analysis, data 248 were time-locked to the motion epoch offset / delay epoch onset and segmented 249 from 3 s before to 3 s after, in order to reveal exclusive delay epoch activity. Data 250 were high-pass filtered at 1 Hz with a FIRWS filter (2 Hz transition width) and band-251 stop filtered between 49-51, 99-101 and 149-151 Hz with a two-pass Butterworth 252 filter (order 4), applied in the forward and reverse directions. The trial average was 253 subtracted from each single trial, to obtain induced activity without the contribution 254

Page 12: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

11

from stimulus-evoked components. For validation purposes, we re-ran the main 255 bursting analyses without subtracting the ERF and obtained very similar results. 256 Unless otherwise indicated, all data shows MEG activity on gradiometer channels. 257 The effects on magnetometer channels were qualitatively and quantitatively similar. 258 Artifact Rejection 259 A set of different summary statistics (variance, maximum absolute amplitude, 260 maximum z value) was used to detect statistical outliers of trials and channels in the 261 datasets. These trials and channels were removed from each dataset (semiautomatic 262 artifact rejection). In addition, the data were visually inspected and any remaining 263 trials and channels with artifacts were removed. On average, 1.3 channels (±1.5 SD) 264 and 9.7% of the trials (±2.6% SD) were rejected. The rejected channels were 265 interpolated with the nearest-neighbors approach for sensor-level analysis. 266 Interpolated channels were not used for source modeling. 267 Time-Frequency Representations 268 Time–frequency representations were calculated on single-trial data using Morlet-269 Wavelets applied to short sliding time windows in steps of 10 ms in the time interval 270 between −0.5 and +5 s relative to fixation onset and in the frequency range between 271 5 and 50 Hz. We used a frequency-dependent window width of five cycles per 272 frequency. The squared absolute value gave the signal power for each MEG sensor 273 across different frequencies and time points. For gradiometer sensors, the power 274 values were calculated for the horizontal and vertical component of the planar 275 gradient and then combined via their vector sum. Peaks in the power spectra during 276 the motion epoch (2-4 s) for each participant were found with an automatic peak-277 detection algorithm (i.e. with the in-built MATLAB function, findpeaks.m). 278 279 Neural activity increase with Wilcoxon sign-rank test 280

Page 13: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

12

MEG signal power during the MOT task (i.e. fixation epoch (0-1 s), cue epoch (1-2 s), 281 motion epoch (2-4 s), jittered motion-delay transition epoch (4-5 s), delay epoch (+1 282 second duration time-locked to the end of the motion epoch on each trial)) was 283 compared to the pre-trial baseline epoch (-0.5 to 0 s relative to fixation onset) by 284 means of a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. This time epoch was chosen as baseline 285 because it was free from stimulus-evoked, eye-movement related and task related 286 activity. Baseline activity for each trial was calculated by averaging power between -287 0.5 to 0 s for every frequency bin on each sensor. Single-trial baseline values were 288 then compared to each time-frequency bin and time sample during the task epochs. 289 The sum of the signed rank difference across trials (Wilcoxon test statistic) was 290 converted into a Z-value for a standard normal distribution. The resulting time-291 frequency Z-value maps for each sensor were averaged over subjects. Gradiometer- 292 and magnetometer sensor systems were analyzed separately. In order to identify 293 sensors with significant activity increases, we averaged the Z-value maps over the 294 alpha-frequency band (9-13 Hz) and the motion epoch (2-4 s). The resulting sensor-295 topographies were Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparison correction (threshold 296 at z= 3.3 corresponding to one-sided p< .05 / 102 sensors, see Figure 2B). Figure 2A 297 shows the time-frequency Z-value map averaged over significant gradiometer 298 sensors and masked at a Bonferroni-corrected threshold (z= 5.48 corresponding to 299 one-sided p< .05 / 46 frequency bins x 500 time samples). 300 Neural activity increase with dependent-samples t-test 301 The Wilcoxon sign-rank test quantifies the alpha power increase from baseline 302 across trials (i.e. a fixed-effects analysis). For validation purposes, we also 303 performed a random-effects analysis to quantify the alpha power increase (i.e. a 304 dependent-samples t-test over participants). For each participant, we computed the 305 average power over all trials, the alpha-frequency band (9-13 Hz) and separately for 306 the baseline (-0.5-0 s) and the motion epochs (2-4 s) per MEG-gradiometer channel. 307

Page 14: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

13

Then, we performed dependent-samples t-tests (BL vs. motion) across participants 308 (df = 23). The two methods (fixed- vs. random-effects) yield similar results with 309 respect to the MEG-sensor selection. 310 Neural activity increase with bursting analysis 311 We followed the approach by Lundqvist et al. (2016) to identify oscillatory bursts in 312 the MEG-signal. Bursts were detected when the power per frequency bin and time 313 sample during the MOT task epochs exceeded its respective pre-trial mean power 314 (averaged between -0.5 to 0 s) by 2 times its trial-specific variability estimate for at 315 least 3 cycles of the corresponding frequency. The variability estimate per trial and 316 frequency was calculated by the standard deviation of the MEG signal during the 317 baseline epoch (-0.5 to 0 s; equation 1). 318 319

Eq 1: burst event = MEG power for 3 cycles > pre-trial mean + 2 * pre-trial SD 320 321 The bursting analysis results in sparse time-frequency maps for each trial and 322 sensor, which separate the occurrence of a supra-threshold burst event from non-323 burst periods. In order to show the topographical distribution of burst events, we 324 averaged the burst event maps over trials (i.e. calculating the burst rate), the alpha-325 frequency band (9-13 Hz) and the motion epoch (2-4 s). Figure 3A shows the time-326 frequency burst rate map averaged over gradiometer sensors with a significant 327 power increase (Wilcoxon Z-statistic; see Figure 2B) and masked above a burst rate 328 of 25% trials. For validation purposes, we also used an alternative algorithm for burst 329 event detection (derived from automatic spike-detection methods, adapted from 330 Quiroga, Nadasdy and Ben-Shaul (2004); Eq 2). 331 332

Eq 2: burst event = MEG power for 3 cycles > median (MEG power / 0.6745) 333

Page 15: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

14

Equation 2 refers to the statistical concept of the probable error, which defines the 334 half-range interval around a distribution’s central point. For symmetrical distributions, 335 it is equivalent to the interquartile range or the Median absolute deviation (MAD, 336 Hoaglin et al., 1983). The probable error can be expressed in terms of standard 337 deviations when scaled with a constant factor that depends on the assumed 338 distribution (i.e. this factor refers to the 75%-quantile for a normal distribution with ϕ-1 339 (0.75) = 0.6745). It is a more robust measure of variability compared to the standard 340 deviation, because it is more resilient to outliers (i.e. it remains largely constant 341 across different burst rate regimes and burst amplitudes). Moreover, because the 342 variability per trial and channel is estimated across the MEG signal of the entire trial, 343 it is independent from the trial-specific baseline epoch. The two methods yielded 344 highly similar results (compare Figures 6 and 7). 345 Comparison between across-trials averaged power and single-trial bursting 346 As outlined above, the bursting analysis provides a single-trial measure of cortical 347 activation. We compared the single-trial bursting findings to a standard approach on 348 across-trials averaged power. For the power calculation, we first subtracted the 349 average power in the baseline epoch (-0.5 to 0 s) from the power maps per condition. 350 Thus, the power analysis accounts for possible average baseline differences 351 between the conditions but, unlike the bursting analysis, it does not relate the single-352 trial MEG activity increases to its trial-specific baseline mean and variability estimate. 353 Source Localization 354 To illustrate the source localization of the observed sensor-level effects, we 355 performed source modeling based on a spatial-filtering algorithm (Beamforming). A 356 structural magnetic resonance image (MRI) was available for 16 of 24 participants. 357 We co-registered the brain surface from their individual segmented MRIs with a 358 single-shell head model (Nolte, 2003). For the remaining subjects, we obtained the 359 canonical cortical anatomy from the affine transformation of a Montreal Neurological 360

Page 16: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

15

Institute (MNI)- template brain (Montreal, Canada; 361 brainweb.bic.mni.mcgill.ca/brainweb/) to the subject’s digitized head shape. Then, we 362 created MNI-aligned grids in each subjects’ individual headspace. To this end, each 363 subject’s head shape was warped to the MNI template brain and the inverse of the 364 transformation matrix was applied onto an MNI template grid with 889 grid points 365 and, on average, 1.5-cm spacing. With this method, we achieved a consistent 366 mapping of the spatial positions of grid points across subjects and to the MNI 367 template. Single-trial, MEG-sensor time courses were projected into source space 368 (individual MNI-aligned grids) using a linear constrained minimum variance (LCMV) 369 beamformer algorithm (Van Veen et al., 1997). We defined a common spatial filter 370 based on the covariance matrix of the band-pass filtered signal (cutoff frequencies, 371 1–30 Hz). The covariance window depended on the locus of the effect as expected 372 from sensor-level analysis (2–4 s). The beamformer filters were computed using the 373 broadband single-trial data. Both the information from the magnetometer and planar 374 gradiometer sensors systems were used. Time-frequency representations (see 375 above) were calculated from single-trial, source-space data, its relative change to the 376 baseline epoch (-0.5-0 s) was calculated (1 – signal / baseline) and then averaged 377 between 9-13 HZ and 2-4 s. The resulting power increase maps (expressed in 378 percent increase from baseline) were interpolated onto a standard MNI brain for 379 illustrative purposes (Figure 2C). Anatomical structures corresponding to localized 380 sources were found using the MNl brain and Talairach atlas (MRC Cognition and 381 Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK; imaging.mrc-382 cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MniTalairach). 383 384 Statistical Analysis 385 Cluster-based permutation statistics 386

Page 17: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

16

The statistical analysis between conditions was done on the sensor-level time 387 courses for power and bursting averaged over the gradiometer sensors with a 388 significant power increase (Wilcoxon Z-statistic; see Figure 2B) and the frequencies 389 between 9-13 Hz (or 20-28 Hz for beta-band bursting). It is important to note that this 390 analysis strategy avoids “double dipping” because the gradiometer sensor selection 391 as a region of interest is based on the power increase from baseline across all 392 conditions (see above). The average alpha-band power and burst rate over trials was 393 compared between conditions (i.e. between tasks, between tasks at different object-394 pools, between different object-pools per task and between correct- vs. error trials 395 per task) across each task epoch separately (i.e. fixation epoch (0-1 s), cue epoch 396 (1-2 s), motion epoch (2-4 s), jitter epoch (4-5 s), delay epoch (0-1 s from delay 397 onset)) using a nonparametric, cluster-based permutation test (Maris and 398 Oostenveld, 2007). The beta-band burst rates were tested with the same method 399 during the cue epoch. The choice of the task epochs was based on the temporal 400 properties of the visual stimulation. Temporal smoothing inherent in the applied 401 sliding Wavelet-window approach can influence the observed effects close to the 402 boundaries of the task epochs. The error bounds are within +/-2.5 cycles per 403 frequency (i.e. for 9-13 Hz: +/-192-277 ms). This effect, however, dampens 404 considerable with increasing temporal distance due to the implicit Gaussian kernel of 405 the Wavelet-window. This nonparametric, cluster-based permutation procedure 406 controls for the type I error accumulation arising from multiple statistical comparisons 407 at multiple time points. First, temporal clusters of adjacent supra-threshold effects 408 (dependent-samples t-statistics exceeding p< 0.05, two-sided) were identified. The t-409 values within a connected cluster were summed up as a cluster-level statistic. Then, 410 random permutations of the data were drawn by exchanging the data between 411 conditions within the participants. After each permutation run, the maximum cluster-412 level statistic was recorded, generating a reference distribution of cluster-level 413 statistics (approximated with a Monte Carlo procedure of 1,000 permutations). The 414

Page 18: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

17

proportion of values in the corresponding reference distribution that exceeded the 415 observed cluster statistic yielded an estimated cluster-level p-value, which is 416 corrected for multiple comparisons. 417 Statistical analysis of neural activity increases integrated across the motion epoch 418 In order to statistically test the observed task- and object-pool specific pattern for 419 main- and interaction effects, we calculated single-trial metrics of the alpha-band 420 burst events (i.e. their count and duration per trial) and for alpha-band power across 421 the motion- and transition epochs (2-5 s). The burst counts were calculated as the 422 number of burst occurrences per trial (see above for the burst detection algorithm) 423 with a minimum of 1 cycle per respective frequency between them. The burst 424 duration was calculated as the average time period over all burst events per trial (in 425 case of multiple burst events). We only considered burst events between 2-5 s and 426 thus excluded those, of which the bigger proportion was outside this time interval. 427 Moreover, we computed the average alpha-band power by first subtracting the power 428 in the baseline epoch (-0.5-0 s) per condition and then averaging the power values 429 between 2-5 s. Power, burst counts and durations were averaged over gradiometer 430 sensors with a significant power increase (Wilcoxon Z-statistic; see Figure 2B) and 431 the frequency bins between 9-13 Hz, and then compared between the conditions by 432 means of repeated-measures ANOVAS and dependent-samples t-tests. 433 Results 434 Behavioral performance was quantified in terms of percent correct trials, in which the 435 difference between the responded- and the probed screen location (i.e. partial report 436 item or centroid) was below 0.5 degrees of visual angle (DVA). As expected, 437 performance for both tasks decreased with more processed objects under both 438 single- and dual-task conditions (single: F(2,46)= 304, p≤ 1x10-323; dual: F(2,46)= 439 231, p≤ 1x10-323; Figure 1C). The main effect between the tasks (single: F(1,23)= 19, 440 p≤ 2.3x10-4; dual: F(1,23)= 48, p≤ 4.3x10-7) is not straightforward to interpret in 441

Page 19: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

18

absolute terms (i.e. whether it is different from zero), because the two tasks differ in 442 chance-level (Alvarez and Oliva, 2008). Moreover, it depended on single- vs. dual-443 task conditions and on the number of processed objects. As expected given their 444 proposed difference in attention (Alvarez and Oliva, 2008; 2009), the dual-task 445 demands impaired individuation (single vs. dual: t(23)= 7.4, p≤ 1.6x10-7), while it had 446 only negligible effects on ensemble-averaging (t(23)= 1.1, p≤ 0.28). Furthermore, 447 individuation- and averaging performance decreased differently with increasing 448 numbers of processed objects (interaction for task x object-pool, single: F(2,46)= 18, 449 p≤ 1.7x10-6; dual: F(2,46)= 10, p≤ 2x10-4). Consistent with different capacity limits for 450 the two tasks, object individuation performance continued to decrease with 451 increasing target-object pools, whereas ensemble-averaging performance remained 452 more stable across many objects (Figure 1C). 453 454 In order to identify neural activity increases during the MOT task, we computed time-455 frequency power maps and used a Wilcoxon sign-rank test to compare the power 456 increase over trials during task epochs against pre-trial baseline epochs (500 ms 457 before fixation onset, see Materials and Methods). Neural activity increased from 458 baseline first during the target-cue epoch in the alpha- (9-13 Hz) and beta frequency 459 bands (20-28 Hz). Then during the motion epoch, power remained exclusively 460 elevated in the alpha-frequency band (Figure 2A). The alpha-band effects increased 461 until about 1 second after motion onset and ceased towards the jittered transition 462 from the motion- into the delay epoch. In separate analyses, we time-locked the 463 power maps to the end of the motion epoch on each trial to test for exclusive delay 464 epoch activity. It showed that alpha-band power was still significantly increased 465 during the memory delay (Figure 2A). The alpha-band power increase had a central-466 parietal topography (8 significant MEG-gradiometer channels; p< .05, one-sided and 467 Bonferroni corrected; Figure 2B). We used an lcmv-beamforming algorithm (Van 468 Veen et al., 1997, see Materials and Methods) to locate the neural generators of the 469

Page 20: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

19

observed alpha-band effects. In line with prior fMRI-work on individuation for static 470 displays (Todd and Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2009), we found the greatest signal 471 change during the motion epoch vs. pre-trial baseline epochs in the inferior parietal 472 cortex (BA 40, Figure 2C). The source modeling revealed bilateral cortical activations 473 but the strongest effects were found in the left hemisphere (max. voxel MNI 474 coordinates in cm = [-50 -50 55]). Similar results were found on MEG-magnetometer 475 channels (10 significant sensors, Figure 3A) and when using a random-effects 476 approach to quantify the alpha-power increase from baseline (dependent-samples t-477 test for BL vs. motion epoch over participants, Figure 3B). On a single-subject-level, 478 22 out of 24 participants showed a peak between 9-13 Hz during the motion epoch 479 (average peak at M=10.5 Hz, SD = +/- 2 Hz; Figure 4). We recorded the participants’ 480 eye gaze to control for micro-saccades during MOT. The long motion epoch with 481 multiple moving objects favors micro-saccades, which can mirror oscillatory neural 482 activity (Yuval-Greenberg, 2008) and alter perception and attentional selection 483 (Hafed, 2013). In our paradigm, however, effects due to micro-saccades were 484 negligible, because they occurred rarely during the motion epoch (0.3-0.5 % trials, 485 Figure 5). Further, there were no significant correlations between the micro-saccade 486 size and the simultaneously recorded alpha power across micro-saccade 487 occurrences for none of the participants (FDR-corrected for multiple-comparison 488 across participants (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995), all absolute r < .39, all FDR-489 adjusted p > .48). 490 491 We used bursting analysis methods (Lundqvist et al., 2016, see Materials and 492 Methods) to compute the observed power increases on the single-trial level and 493 compare it between the different experimental conditions. In contrast to standard 494 across-trials averaged power analyses, the bursting approach isolates time- and 495 band-limited, high-signal epochs (i.e. bursts) during each trial with respect to its 496 respective pre-trial mean and variability estimate. Because bursts can occur at 497

Page 21: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

20

different rates, times and with different durations from trial to trial, we expected them 498 to be more diagnostic of dynamic activity changes in the time-critical MOT task. We 499 found the strongest bursting activity, quantified in the percentage of trials that 500 contained a burst at a particular sensor-time-frequency sample (i.e. burst rate), in the 501 alpha-frequency band during the motion epoch over central-parietal MEG-502 gradiometer sensors (Figures 6A and B). Similar results were found on MEG-503 magnetometer channels (Figure 3C), without subtracting event-related fields from 504 single trials (Figure 3D) and with an alternative algorithm for burst detection 505 (Quiroga, Nadasdy and Ben-Shaul, 2004; see Materials and Methods and Figure 7). 506 On average, one to two alpha-band bursts were detected per trial (M= 1.15; SD= 507 0.22) with a mean duration of 865 ms (SD= 348 ms; Figure 10). 508 509 The single-trial bursting analysis was sensitive to differences in neural activity 510 between the performed tasks during MOT (individuation, averaging, dual-task). We 511 observed greater alpha-band burst rates for individuation vs. averaging during the 512 motion epoch (cluster-corrected p≤ .05), during the jittered transition between 513 motion- and delay epochs (cluster-corrected p≤ .004) and during the delay epoch 514 (cluster-corrected p≤ .006; Figure 8A). Similar results were found for the dual-task 515 condition vs. averaging (motion: all cluster-corrected p≤ .008, jitter: p≤ .006, delay: p≤ 516 .006). By contrast, we found no significant differences for the dual-task condition vs. 517 individuation during any task epoch. Moreover, there were no significant differences 518 in the alpha-band burst rates during the fixation- and cue epochs and in beta-band 519 bursting (averaged between 20-28 Hz) during the cue epoch between any of the 520 tasks. The observed alpha-band effects rooted mainly on single-trial activity changes, 521 because across-trials averaged power was less sensitive to the between-task 522 differences (Figure 8B). We used baseline-corrected power (baseline interval: -0.5 to 523 0 s), which factors out average baseline differences between the conditions but - in 524 contrast to the burst analysis - it does not quantify activity increases from baseline for 525

Page 22: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

21

each single trial. We found no alpha-band power differences for individuation vs. 526 averaging and for individuation vs. the dual-task condition. However, there was 527 greater alpha-band power for the dual-task vs. averaging (motion: all cluster-528 corrected p≤ .022, jitter: p≤ .024, delay: p≤ .034). 529 530 The observed alpha-band bursting effects were strongest up to the typical capacity 531 limitations for multiple-object processing at around 4 objects. We found significant 532 differences between the individuation vs. averaging tasks below / at capacity during 533 the motion epoch (4 objects: cluster-corrected p≤ .038 and p≤ .04), during the 534 motion-delay transition (2 objects: all cluster-corrected p≤ .002, 4 objects: p≤ .004) 535 and during the delay epoch (2 objects: all cluster-corrected p≤ .006, 4 objects: p≤ 536 .002). Above capacity at 8 objects, however, there were only small differences 537 between the two tasks during the delay (cluster-corrected p≤ .044; Figures 9A-C). 538 The dual-task condition showed no significant differences vs. individuation for any 539 task epoch or object pool. However, it showed greater burst rates vs. averaging 540 below / at capacity (2 objects, motion: all cluster-corrected p≤ .01, jitter: p≤ .004, 541 delay: p≤ .008; 4 objects, motion: p≤ .022 and p≤ .042, jitter: p≤ .01, delay: p≤ .004) 542 and smaller effects above capacity (8 objects, motion: all cluster-corrected p≤ .02, 543 delay: p≤ .034; Figures 9A-C). 544 545 Comparing the different object-pools for each task condition separately showed that 546 capacity limitations had a strong impact on the burst rates during the motion epochs 547 for individuation and, in part, under dual-task conditions. We found greater burst 548 rates below vs. above capacity (individuation 2 vs. 8 objects, motion: all cluster-549 corrected p≤ .002; 4 vs. 8 objects, motion: p≤ .008; 4 vs. 8 objects, jitter: p≤ .018; 550 dual task 2 vs. 8 objects, motion: p≤ .02) but no significant differences below/at 551 capacity (i.e. 2 vs. 4 objects, Figures 9D,F). By contrast, we found a different pattern 552 on averaging trials. There were burst rate differences below/at capacity (2 vs. 4 553

Page 23: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

22

objects, motion: cluster-corrected p≤ .022) and more bursting above vs. below 554 capacity (4 vs. 8 objects, jitter: cluster-corrected p≤ .022; Figure 9E). Moreover, the 555 burst rates during the delay epoch were also different for individuation / dual-task 556 conditions vs. averaging. Whereas we found particularly low burst rates on two-557 object trials under individuation- (2 vs. 4 objects: all cluster-corrected p≤ .036; 2 vs. 8 558 objects: p≤ .044;) and dual-task conditions (2 vs. 4 objects: all cluster-corrected p≤ 559 .026; 2 vs. 8 objects: p≤ .012), averaging was mainly characterized by stronger burst 560 rates on eight-object trials (2 vs. 8 objects: all cluster-corrected p≤ .004; 4 vs. 8 561 objects: p≤ .006; Figures 9D-F). 562 563 In order to formally test the observed task- and object-pool specific pattern, we ran 564 two-way, repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) and post-hoc, 565 dependent-samples t-tests on the number of alpha-band bursts per trial, their 566 average duration and the average alpha-band power (detected and integrated from 567 2-5 s after motion onset, Figure 10). For the alpha-band burst count per trial, we 568 found a significant main effect for the individuation vs. averaging tasks (F(1,23)= 5.9, 569 p≤ .023) and an interaction between the performed tasks across different numbers of 570 processed objects (F(2,46)= 3.9, p≤ .028). There were more bursts on individuation 571 vs. averaging trials for two- (t(23)= 3, p≤ .007) and four objects (t(23)= 3, p≤ .007) but 572 not for eight objects (t(23)= 0.5, p> .62). By contrast, there were no significant main- 573 and interaction effects for the alpha-band burst duration and power. Comparing 574 individuation vs. the dual-task condition, we found a significant interaction over 575 object-pools for the burst counts (F(2,46)= 3.9, p≤ .028) and no significant effects for 576 the burst duration and power. Moreover for averaging vs. the dual-task condition, 577 there were main effects on the burst counts (F(1,23)= 6.9, p≤ .016) and on power 578 (F(1,23)= 5.6, p≤ .027) but no significant effects for the burst duration (Figure 10). 579 580

Page 24: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

23

Finally, we investigated possible links between bursting and behavioral performance, 581 in order to add support for the behavioral relevance of our results. To this end, we 582 split correct- and error trials separately for individuation and averaging conditions and 583 contrasted their alpha-band burst rates over each task epoch. For individuation, we 584 found greater burst rates on correct trials during the motion epoch (cluster-corrected 585 p ≤ .05) and greater burst rates on error trials during the delay epoch (cluster-586 corrected p ≤ .02, Figure 11). For averaging, there were greater burst rates on error 587 trials during both the jitter epoch (cluster-corrected p ≤ .018) and during the delay 588 epoch (cluster-corrected p ≤ .008). There were no significant differences during the 589 other task epochs (Figure 11). These results suggest that the observed bursting 590 effects were relevant for behavior in different ways for the two tasks. 591 592 Discussion 593 We observed oscillatory bursts over inferior parietal cortex during dynamic multiple-594 object situations. Confirming previous work on dynamic multiple-object processing, 595 we observed effects in the alpha-frequency band (Rouhinen et al., 2013, Jia et al., 596 2017). Advancing on previous reports, however, the burst occurrences were 597 diagnostic for the task demands for object individuation vs. ensemble / average 598 processing, the number of processed objects during MOT and its behavioral 599 outcome. On individuation trials, more bursts were detected below/at vs. above the 600 typical capacity limit. By contrast on ensemble trials, we found more stable burst 601 rates across different object-pools, which were lower compared to those found on 602 individuation trials up to capacity. The observed bursting effects occurred both 603 “online” during object-motion tracking and “offline” during the memory delay. 604 Moreover under dual-task conditions, we found greater burst rates compared to 605 single-task ensemble averaging and similar burst rates compared to single-task 606 individuation, which suggests strategic influences per task on the observed effects. 607 Finally, the observed bursting showed behavioral relevance, because greater 608

Page 25: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

24

bursting was found on correct individuation trials and on error trials for averaging. 609 Overall, our findings identify the neural correlates for object- vs. ensemble- and 610 below- vs. above capacity processing during time-critical, multiple-object scenarios. 611 612 Alpha-band oscillations are thought to regulate neural excitability via inhibitory-613 control (Klimesch, Sauseng and Hanslmayr, 2007; Haegens et al., 2011). Along 614 these lines, two possible functional interpretations for the observed dynamic alpha-615 band increases stand out: On the one hand, they could reflect the inhibition of task-616 irrelevant inputs (Bonnefond and Jensen, 2012; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014). Previous 617 work on event-related potentials (ERPs) has suggested that both effects due to 618 target enhancement and distractor suppression play a role during MOT (Drew et al., 619 2009; Doran and Hoffman, 2010). Less target-objects meant also more distractor-620 objects, respectively, in our paradigm. Thus, in line with an interpretation based on 621 distractor suppression, we found greater alpha-band burst rates with greater 622 numbers of task-irrelevant objects. Alternatively, the observed alpha-band effects 623 might signal inhibitory control needed to prioritize, select and maintain task-relevant 624 objects carved out as individuals from visual inputs (Palva and Palva, 2007; Jensen 625 et al., 2014; Wutz et al., 2014). In this perspective, complex visual scenes are 626 segmented into local objects by regulating neural excitability focally according to their 627 alpha-timed release from inhibition. By contrast, inhibition is more globally distributed 628 across many objects in the scene for ensemble computations. Consistent with this 629 view, we found greater alpha-band burst rates up to the typical capacity limits for 630 object individuation and similar burst rates below capacity. Above capacity, however, 631 when individuation via alpha-timed inhibitory control is insufficient, we observed 632 lower alpha-band bursting down to the level for global ensemble processing. The 633 observed effects during the delay epoch further support the interpretation based on 634 task-relevant object individuation. No task-irrelevant distractors were present during 635 the memory delay and thus effects due to distractor suppression were not expected. 636

Page 26: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

25

Instead during the delay, task-relevant objects need to be kept separate via inhibitory 637 control to perform partial report on individuation trials. By contrast on ensemble trials, 638 the centroid task allowed for integration and maintenance in a compressed format 639 (i.e. their average) without explicitly representing the spatial positions of the 640 individual objects during the delay (Greene and Oliva, 2009; Alvarez, 2011; Corbett 641 and Oriet, 2011). Importantly, we also found alpha-band bursting differences 642 between the two tasks during the motion epoch. In addition to its effects on memory 643 maintenance, this suggests that alpha-timed inhibitory control can also be used 644 dynamically to switch between input individuation and compression “online” during 645 dynamic multiple-object perception. 646 647 Alternatively, the observed alpha-band bursting effects reflect rather differences 648 between the two tasks in terms of difficulty or effort. Three lines of evidence render 649 this explanation inadequate in our paradigm. First, we found the greatest burst rate 650 differences when performance was most similar (below/at capacity) and the smallest 651 burst rate differences when the performance difference was greatest between the 652 two tasks (above capacity, see behavioral data, Figure 1C). Second, behavioral 653 outcome impacted bursting differently for the two tasks with more bursting on correct 654 individuation trials and on incorrect averaging trials. Third, whereas the dual-task 655 demands worsened individuation performance probably due to higher difficulty, it did 656 not impact the burst rates under dual-task conditions vs. single-task individuation. 657 Instead, we found burst rate differences between the dual-task condition vs. single-658 task averaging. This pattern suggests that the observed effects were due to top-659 down strategies during single-task ensemble-averaging, which could not be applied 660 under single-task individuation and dual-task conditions. The investigation of top-661 down grouping strategies during multiple-object processing has a long history in 662 experimental and cognitive psychology (Wertheimer, 1912; Yantis, 1992; Merkel et 663 al., 2014, 2015; Merkel, Hopf and Schoenfeld, 2017). The observed alpha-band 664

Page 27: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

26

bursting effects signal a potential implementation of top-down principles during 665 dynamic multiple-object scenarios into neural function by grouping objects into 666 ensemble representations. 667 668 Our findings queue up with recent reports from neurophysiology about oscillatory 669 burst events underlying cognitive and motor operations (Feingold et al., 2015; 670 Lundqvist et al., 2016; Sherman et al., 2016; Shin et al., 2017). These observations 671 have sparked a debate on principles about the sustained vs. transient nature of 672 oscillatory dynamics (for a discussion see Van Ede et al., 2018). The key aspect is 673 that in many cases sustained oscillatory activity represents an analysis artifact from 674 averaging over trials and it may instead be better captured by transient high-signal 675 burst events that happen at different rates and times, and with different durations 676 from trial to trial. Here, we detected bursts in single-trial MEG activity. It is thus not 677 clear whether and how our findings relate to the oscillatory burst events found with 678 single-cell neurophysiology. Critically, however, the bursting analysis served as a 679 sensitive tool to capture single-trial differences in our paradigm, which would have 680 gone unnoticed with a standard approach on across-trials averaged power. 681 Comparing the dynamic mode of attention to multiple moving objects studied here vs. 682 classic “sustained attention” situations, in which typically more sustained power 683 effects are found, buttresses the potential functional significance of the observed 684 bursting effects. The following three aspects stand out: First, in a classic “sustained-685 attention” task participants typically need to constantly monitor a static stimulus at a 686 particular fixed spatial location. By contrast, our MOT task requires keeping track of 687 dynamic visual objects, which continuously change their spatial-temporal positions in 688 random directions throughout the trial. Second in contrast to “sustained-attention” 689 over time to a single, isolated object or spatial location, the participants need to 690 dynamically shift their attention-focus multiple times per trial between many objects / 691 locations during MOT. Third, a typical attention experiment is built on a repetitive trial 692

Page 28: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

27

structure that reiterates the same sequence of temporal events often with the same 693 constant latencies over and over (e.g. “Cue” – “Focus” – “Response”). In our MOT 694 task, however, not one single trial is like the next one, because the spatial-temporal 695 coordinates of each object’s motion path were randomly generated for each trial. 696 Consequently, there is a high degree of inter-trial variability in the timing of the 697 relevant perceptual / cognitive processes, because critical situations might happen at 698 different times on different trials (e.g. when an object moves to the display edges or 699 two objects cross in close proximity). 700 701 All three aspects (static vs. motion, one vs. many objects, inter-trial variability) favor 702 sparse bursts over sustained activity. Moreover, they suggest that MOT requires a 703 more dynamic mode of attention compared to the typical “sustained attention” 704 situation. Importantly, these three aspects are also more pronounced on individuation 705 vs. averaging trials. For individual objects, there is (1) “more” motion (i.e. a higher 706 dynamic range in the spatial-temporal coordinates vs. a more stable centroid), (2) 707 “more” objects (i.e. processing is split between multiple objects vs. one average 708 object) and (3) “more” inter-trial variability (i.e. in timing for independent objects vs. 709 averages). Because MOT for individual objects vs. ensembles requires more 710 dynamic and flexible attention, it is better captured by sparsely timed bursts on 711 individual trials compared to across-trials averaged, sustained activity. 712 713 This view is in line with recent perspectives from neurophysiology and computational 714 modeling on dynamic processing and its burst-like neural signatures. For example, in 715 the Working Memory 2.0 – Model (Miller, Lundqvist and Bastos, 2018), brief bursts of 716 oscillatory activity reflect reactivations of attractor states for new mental content (i.e. 717 new attention foci or working memory items), beyond the established view based on 718 sustained and persistent activity. Neural ensemble-communication in alpha and beta 719 oscillations (there is explicitly no distinction between the two in the theory) acts as a 720

Page 29: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

28

default state and implements top-down information and inhibition. Excitation at 721 sensory read-in kicks the rhythms up to gamma frequencies in short discrete bursts 722 supporting the encoding of new content and refreshing previous impressions. Alpha / 723 Beta- and Gamma-frequency bursts serve antagonistic roles on inhibition-excitation. 724 Its interplay enables the exertion of the volitional, executive control needed to act 725 flexibly in dynamic situations. Alpha / Beta-inhibition is integral in the model, because 726 it allows for selective and timed routing of sensory flow into objects. Consequently, 727 more alpha / beta bursting would be expected for many individual objects vs. one 728 average ensemble. In principle, attractor networks are capable of processing multiple 729 items simultaneously with minimal interference between them through temporal 730 multiplexing their reactivations in time. However, the refresh-rate of successive 731 reactivations sets the system’s capacity limitations. Consistent with our observations, 732 exceeding capacity would then lead to a break down of bursting, because too many 733 items have to be reactivated within the limited time available for the refresh 734 735 Our results constitute an important step toward understanding how dynamic multiple-736 object processing is implemented in brain function. Consistent with our source 737 modeling results, most functional explanations for object- and scene processing thus 738 far revolve around spatially defined saliency maps in parietal cortex (Todd and 739 Marois, 2004; Xu and Chun, 2009). Our findings augment these theoretical 740 perspectives by capturing its temporal dynamics in neural alpha-band oscillations. 741 They may thus lead the way to novel conceptions about how dynamic multiple-object 742 situations are solved in real-time in the visual world and in computer vision 743 (supporting recent developments in computational modeling and engineering, 744 Mahadevan and Vasconcelos, 2009; Nguyen et al., 2013). Alpha oscillations are 745 thought to coordinate the timing of brain processes (Klimesch, Sauseng and 746 Hanslmayr, 2007, Klimesch, 2012) and to underlie the temporal resolution of visual 747 perception and attention (Gho and Varela, 1988; VanRullen, Reddy and Koch, 2005; 748

Page 30: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

29

Samaha and Postle, 2015; Wutz, Samha and Melcher, 2018). They structure visual 749 processing into discrete temporal events (for review see VanRullen and Koch, 2003), 750 within which scenes are decomposed into objects via the timed release from 751 inhibition (for reviews see Jensen et al., 2014; Wutz and Melcher, 2014). Our findings 752 reveal that this spatiotemporal event structure can be dynamically regulated between 753 object- and ensemble-analysis levels, to overcome capacity limitations due to 754 selective attention and to master the real-time requirements of visual cognition. 755

Page 31: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

30

References 756 Alvarez GA, Oliva A (2008) The representation of simple ensemble visual features 757 outside the focus of attention. Psychol Sci 19(4):392-398. 758 Alvarez GA, Oliva A (2009) Spatial ensemble statistics are efficient codes that can be 759 represented with reduced attention. P Natl Acad Sci USA 106(18):7345-7350. 760 Alvarez GA (2011) Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual 761

cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 15(3):122-131. 762 Ariely D (2001) Seeing sets: Representation by statistical properties. Psychol Sci 763

12(2):157-162. 764 Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical 765

and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal statistical 766 society: series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289-300. 767

Bonnefond M, Jensen O (2012) Alpha oscillations serve to protect working memory 768 maintenance against anticipated distracters. Curr Biol 22(20):1969-1974. 769

Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision 10:433-436. 770 Cavanagh P, Alvarez GA (2005) Tracking multiple targets with multifocal 771

attention. Trends Cogn Sci 9(7):349-354. 772 Chong SC, Treisman A (2003) Representation of statistical properties. Vision Res 773

43(4):393-404. 774 Corbett JE, & Oriet, C (2011) The whole is indeed more than the sum of its parts: 775

Perceptual averaging in the absence of individual item representation. Acta 776 Psychol, 138(2):289-301. 777

Cowan N (2010) The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, 778 and why?. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19(1): 51-57. 779

Doran MM, Hoffman JE (2010) The role of visual attention in multiple object tracking: 780 Evidence from ERPs. Atten Percept Psycho 72(1):33-52. 781

Drew T, McCollough AW, Horowitz TS, Vogel EK (2009). Attentional enhancement 782 during multiple-object tracking. Psychon B Rev 16(2):411-417. 783

Page 32: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

31

Engbert R, Kliegl R (2003) Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert 784 attention. Vision Res 43(9):1035-1045. 785

Feingold J, Gibson DJ, DePasquale B, Graybiel AM (2015) Bursts of beta oscillation 786 differentiate postperformance activity in the striatum and motor cortex of 787 monkeys performing movement tasks. P Natl Acad Sci USA 112(44):13687-788 13692. 789

Gho M, Varela FJ (1988) A quantitative assessment of the dependency of the visual 790 temporal frame upon the cortical rhythm. J Physiol-Paris 83(2):95-101. 791

Greene MR, Oliva A (2009) Recognition of natural scenes from global properties: 792 Seeing the forest without representing the trees. Cogn Psychol 58(2):137-793 176. 794

Haegens S, Nácher, V, Luna R, Romo R, Jensen O (2011) α-Oscillations in the 795 monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by 796 rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108(48): 797 19377-19382. 798

Hafed ZM (2013) Alteration of visual perception prior to microsaccades. Neuron 799 77(4):775-786. 800

Hoaglin, David C.; Frederick Mosteller; John W. Tukey (1983). Understanding 801 Robust and Exploratory Data Analysis. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 404–414. 802

Jensen O, Bonnefond M, VanRullen R (2012) An oscillatory mechanism for 803 prioritizing salient unattended stimuli. Trends Cogn Sci 16(4):200-206. 804

Jensen O, Gips B, Bergmann TO, Bonnefond M (2014) Temporal coding organized 805 by coupled alpha and gamma oscillations prioritize visual processing. Trends 806 Neurosci 37(7): 357-369. 807

Jevons WS (1871) The power of numerical discrimination. Nature 3:363-372. 808 Jia J, Liu L, Fang F, Luo H (2017) Sequential sampling of visual objects during 809

sustained attention. PLoS Biol 15(6):e2001903. 810

Page 33: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

32

Kazanovich Y, Borisyuk R (2006) An oscillatory neural model of multiple object 811 tracking. Neural Comput 18(6):1413-1440. 812

Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S (2007) EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–813 timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 53(1):63-88. 814

Klimesch W (2012) Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to 815 stored information. Trends Cogn Sci 16(12):606-617. 816

Luck SJ, Vogel EK (1997) The capacity of visual working memory for features and 817 conjunctions. Nature 390(6657):279. 818

Lundqvist M, Rose J, Herman P, Brincat SL, Buschman TJ, Miller EK (2016) Gamma 819 and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron 90(1):152-164. 820

Mahadevan, V, Vasconcelos, N (2009) Spatiotemporal saliency in dynamic 821 scenes. IEEE T Pattern Anal 32(1):171-177. 822

Maris E, Oostenveld R (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-823 data. J Neurosci Methods 164:177–190. 824

Merkel C, Stoppel CM, Hillyard SA, Heinze HJ, Hopf JM, Schoenfeld MA (2014) 825 Spatio-temporal patterns of brain activity distinguish strategies of multiple-826 object tracking. J Cogn Neurosci 26(1):28-40. 827

Merkel C, Hopf JM, Heinze HJ, Schoenfeld MA (2015) Neural correlates of multiple 828 object tracking strategies. Neuroimage 118:63-73. 829

Merkel C, Hopf JM, Schoenfeld MA (2017) Spatio-temporal dynamics of attentional 830 selection stages during multiple object tracking. NeuroImage 146:484-491. 831

Miller, E. K., Lundqvist, M., & Bastos, A. M. (2018). Working Memory 832 2.0. Neuron, 100(2), 463-475. 833

Morey RD (2008) Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to 834 Cousineau (2005). Reason 4(2):61-64. 835

Nguyen TV, Xu M, Gao G, Kankanhalli M, Tian Q, Yan S (2013) Static saliency vs. 836 dynamic saliency: a comparative study. In Proceedings of the 21st ACM 837 international conference on Multimedia (pp. 987-996). ACM. 838

Page 34: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

33

Nolte G (2003) The magnetic lead field theorem in the quasi-static approximation and 839 its use for magnetoencephalography forward calculation in realistic volume 840 conductors. Phys Med Biol 48(22):3637. 841

Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, & Schoffelen JM (2011) FieldTrip: open source 842 software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive 843 electrophysiological data. Comput Intel Neurosci 2011:1. 844

Palva S, Palva JM (2007) New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends 845 Neurosci 30(4):150-158. 846

Parkes L, Lund J, Angelucci A, Solomon JA, Morgan M (2001) Compulsory 847 averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nature 848 Neurosci 4(7):739. 849

Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming 850 numbers into movies. Spatial Vision 10:437-442. 851

Piazza M, Fumarola A, Chinello A, Melcher D (2011) Subitizing reflects visuo- spatial 852 object individuation capacity. Cognition 121:147–153. 853

Pylyshyn ZW, Storm RW (1988) Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for 854 a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision 3(3):179-197. 855

Quiles MG, Wang D, Zhao L, Romero RA, Huang DS (2011) Selecting salient objects 856 in real scenes: An oscillatory correlation model. Neural Networks 24(1):54-64. 857

Quiroga RQ, Nadasdy Z, Ben-Shaul Y (2004) Unsupervised spike detection and 858 sorting with wavelets and superparamagnetic clustering. Neural 859 Comput 16(8):1661-1687. 860

Rouhinen S, Panula J, Palva JM, Palva S (2013) Load dependence of β and γ 861 oscillations predicts individual capacity of visual attention. J Neurosci 862 33(48):19023-19033. 863

Roux F, Uhlhaas PJ (2014) Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha–gamma 864 versus theta–gamma codes for distinct WM information?. Trends Cogn 865 Sci 18(1):16-25. 866

Page 35: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

34

Samaha J, Postle BR (2015) The speed of alpha-band oscillations predicts the 867 temporal resolution of visual perception. Curr Biol 25:2985–2990. 868

Scholl BJ (2009) What have we learned about attention from multiple object tracking 869 (and vice versa). Computation, cognition, and Pylyshyn:49-78. 870

Sherman MA, Lee S, Law R, Haegens S, Thorn CA, Hämäläinen MS, Moore CI, 871 Jones SR (2016) Neural mechanisms of transient neocortical beta rhythms: 872 converging evidence from humans, computational modeling, monkeys, and 873 mice. P Natl Acad Sci USA 113(33):E4885-E4894. 874

Shin H, Law R, Tsutsui S, Moore CI, Jones SR (2017) The rate of transient beta 875 frequency events predicts behavior across tasks and species. Elife 6:e29086. 876

Todd JJ, Marois R (2004) Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human 877 posterior parietal cortex. Nature 428(6984):751. 878

Torralba A, Oliva A (2003) Statistics of natural image categories. Network: 879 computation in neural systems 14(3):391-412. 880

Trick LM, Pylyshyn ZW (1994) Why are small and large numbers enumerated 881 differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychol 882 Rev 101(1):80. 883

van Ede F, Quinn AJ, Woolrich MW, Nobre AC (2018) Neural oscillations: Sustained 884 rhythms or transient burst-events?. Trends Neurosci 41(7):415-417. 885

VanRullen R, Reddy L, Koch C (2005) Attention-driven discrete sampling of motion 886 perception. P Natl Acad Sci USA 102(14):5291-5296. 887

VanRullen R, Koch C (2003) Is perception discrete or continuous?. Trends Cogn Sci 888 7(5):207-213. 889

Van Veen BD, van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, Suzuki A (1997) Localization of brain 890 electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. 891 IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 44:867– 880. 892

Wertheimer M (1912) Experimentelle studien uber das Sehen von Bewegung. 893 Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 61:161-265. 894

Page 36: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

35

Whitney D, Yamanashi Leib A (2018) Ensemble perception. Annu Rev 895 Psychol 69:105-129. 896

Wutz A, Melcher D (2014) The temporal window of individuation limits visual 897 capacity. Front Psychol 5:952. 898

Wutz A, Weisz N, Braun C, Melcher D (2014) Temporal windows in visual 899 processing: “Prestimulus brain state” and “poststimulus phase reset” 900 segregate visual transients on different temporal scales. J Neurosci 901 34:1554–1565. 902

Wutz A, Melcher D, Samaha J (2018) Frequency modulation of neural oscillations 903 according to visual task demands. P Natl Acad Sci USA 115(6):1346-1351. 904

Xiang Y, Alahi A, Savarese S (2015) Learning to track: Online multi-object tracking 905 by decision making. IEEE - ICCV 230283:4705-13. 906

Xu Y, Chun MM (2009) Selecting and perceiving multiple visual objects. Trends 907 Cogn Sci 13(4):167-174. 908

Yantis S (1992) Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization. 909 Cogn Psychol 24(3):295-340. 910

Yuval-Greenberg S, Tomer O, Keren AS, Nelken I, Deouell LY (2008) Transient 911 induced gamma-band response in EEG as a manifestation of miniature 912 saccades. Neuron 58(3):429-441. 913

Page 37: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

36

Figure legends 914 Figure 1. Trial sequence, experimental conditions and behavioral data 915 A. A typical trial in the experiment. The black arrows indicate the target-object 916 locations and are only shown here for illustration. B. Response screens for each 917 task. Blue and red circles indicate the probe locations (i.e. the location of the item to 918 be indicated for partial report (in blue) for the individuation condition or the centroid 919 location of the items (in red) for the averaging condition). They are only shown here 920 for illustration. C. Behavioral performance (in percent correct trials) per task and set 921 size under single task- (left panel) and dual task conditions (right panel). Error bars 922 show 1 standard error (SE) for repeated measures (Morey, 2008). Asterisks indicate 923 the significance level with *** p< .001. 924 925 Figure 2. Alpha-band power increase during MOT on the sensor- and source-926 level 927 A. Power increase relative to pre-trial baseline epochs (Wilcoxon z-value) averaged 928 over significant gradiometer sensors (see B). B. MEG-gradiometer topography for 929 the power increase relative to pre-trial baseline epochs (Wilcoxon z-value) averaged 930 between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 s. In A and B, Z-values with p < .05 are shown (Bonferroni-931 corrected). C. Source modeling for the power increase relative to pre-trial baseline 932 epochs (signal change in percent) averaged between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 s. 933 934 Figure 3. MEG-sensor topographies for power and bursting 935 A. Magnetometer topographies for the power increase relative to pre-trial baseline 936 epochs (Wilcoxon z-value) averaged between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 s. B. Gradiometer 937 topographies for the power increase relative to pre-trial baseline epochs with a 938 random-effects approach (dependent-samples t-test over participants) averaged 939 between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 s. For comparison, white dots indicate the sensors of 940 interest resulting from the sensor selection with the fixed-effects approach (Wilcoxon 941

Page 38: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

37

test over trials, see Figure 2B). C. Magnetometer topographies for the burst rate (in 942 percent trials) averaged between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 s. D. Gradiometer topography for 943 the burst rates (in percent trials) without ERF-subtraction averaged between 9-13 Hz 944 and 2-4 s. 945 946 Figure 4. Single-subject power spectra during MOT 947 Power spectra averaged over significant gradiometer sensors (see Figure 2B) and 948 between 2-4 s for each participant separately and for the grand-average. Black 949 horizontal lines denote the alpha-frequency range at 9-13 Hz. 950 951 Figure 5. Micro-saccades during MOT 952 Micro-saccade rate (in percent trials). 953 954 Figure 6. Alpha-band bursting during MOT 955 A. Burst rate (in percent trials) averaged over significant gradiometer sensors (see 956 Figure 2B). Burst rates above 25 percent are shown. B. MEG-gradiometer 957 topography for the burst rates (in percent trials) averaged between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 958 s. 959 960 Figure 7. Alpha-band bursting during MOT with an alternative algorithm 961 A. Burst rate (in percent trials) computed with an alternative algorithm (Quiroga, 962 Nadasdy and Ben-Shaul, 2004) averaged over significant gradiometer sensors (see 963 Figure 2B). Burst rates above 20 percent are shown. B. MEG-gradiometer 964 topography for the burst rates (in percent trials) averaged between 9-13 Hz and 2-4 965 s. 966 967

Page 39: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

38

Figure 8. Alpha-band bursting and power increase separated by experimental 968 task 969 A. Burst rate (in percent trials) per task averaged between 9-13 Hz and over 970 significant gradiometer sensors (see Figure 2B). B. Power per task averaged 971 between 9-13 Hz and over significant gradiometer sensors (see Figure 2B). Shaded 972 regions show 1 standard error (SE) for repeated measures (Morey, 2008). Black 973 lines show significant effects (cluster-corrected). Asterisks indicate the significance 974 level with n.s. not significant, * p< .05, ** p< .01, *** p< .005. 975 976 Figure 9. Alpha-band bursting during MOT separated by task and object-pools 977 A-C. Burst rate (in percent trials) per task averaged between 9-13 Hz and over 978 significant gradiometer sensors (see Figure 2B) for two (A), four (B) and eight 979 objects (C). D-F. Burst rate (in percent trials) per object-pool averaged between 9-13 980 Hz and over significant gradiometer sensors (see Figure 2B) for individuation- (A), 981 averaging- (B) and dual-task conditions (C). Shaded regions show 1 standard error 982 (SE) for repeated measures (Morey, 2008). Black lines show significant effects 983 (cluster-corrected). Asterisks indicate the significance level with n.s. not significant, * 984 p< .05, ** p< .01, *** p< .005. 985 986 Figure 10. Alpha-band burst count, burst duration and power during MOT 987 A-C. Burst count per trial (A), burst duration per trial (B) and power (C) for each task 988 and at different object-pools averaged between 9-13 Hz, 2-5 s and over significant 989 sensors (see topography in Figure 2B). Error bars show 1 standard error (SE) for 990 repeated measures (Morey, 2008). Asterisks indicate the significance level for 991 dependent-samples t-tests between each condition with n.s. not significant, * p< .05, 992 ** p< .01, *** p< .001. 993 994

Page 40: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and

39

Figure 11. Alpha-band bursting during MOT separated by correct- vs. error 995 trials 996 Burst rate (in percent trials) per task averaged between 9-13 Hz and over significant 997 gradiometer sensors (see Figure 2B) separately for correct- and error trials. Shaded 998 regions show 1 standard error (SE) for repeated measures (Morey, 2008). Black 999 lines show significant effects (cluster-corrected). Asterisks indicate the significance 1000 level with * p< .05, ** p< .01. 1001

Page 41: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 42: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 43: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 44: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 45: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 46: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 47: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 48: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 49: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 50: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and
Page 51: Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex reflect dynamic ...€¦ · 4/8/2020  · 1 1 Oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex ref lect dynamic attention between multiple 2 objects and