Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

5
ORIGINAL ARTICLE: EPIDEMIOLOGY, CLINICAL PRACTICE AND HEALTH Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Logical Memory subtest for old-old people Naoko Kawano, 1,2 Shuichi Awata, 1 Mutsuo Ijuin, 1 Kunihiro Iwamoto 2 and Norio Ozaki 2 1 Research Team for Promoting Independence of the Elderly, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology, Tokyo, and 2 Department of Psychiatry, Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya, Japan Aim: Episodic memory is vulnerable to deterioration in people suffering from Alzheimer’s disease. Currently, the Logical Memory (LM) subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) is used internationally as an operational definition to identify people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). However, the Japanese version of the LM has not been adequately normalized for old-old people. Therefore, norms of the LM for people aged 75 years and over are required, and the effects of sex, age and education on performance were evaluated. Methods: A total of 50 (27 female and 23 male) participants without a history of dementia and symptomatic stroke events recruited from the community and hospital populations were investigated using the Mini-Mental State Examination, the LM and some interference tasks. Results: The mean scores (standard deviations) of the sample were 15.5 (5.4) on LM-I and 9.9 (6.6) on LM-II. The distributions of the LM-I and -II scores satisfied the normality assumption. The LM-I and LM-II scores correlated with age and the LM-I score correlated with educational background. Conclusions: For the Japanese version of the LM, the means, standard deviations and distribution features of the old-old sample are presented. Although the normal sample was chosen to closely match the demographic profile of the Japanese population, the present sample might have had a higher educational background than the age-matched population, especially the males. Further study is required to standardize the Japanese version of the LM subtest for each 5-year interval for latter-stage elderly people. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2013; 13: 726–730. Keywords: episodic memory, Logical Memory, mild cognitive impairment, normative data, old-old people. Introduction A mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as a result of Alzhe- imer’s disease (AD) is seen as memory impairment, and this symptom is the key early marker in the prodromal stages of AD. 1 Although the memory deficits in indi- viduals with MCI are clinically discernible, in order to make a diagnosis of MCI, amnesia that does not inter- fere notably with activities of daily life (ADL) must be identified. It is operationally defined as performance 1.0–1.5 standard deviations (SD) below age- and education-adjusted norms on an episodic memory measure of delayed verbal recall. 2 The Logical Memory (LM) subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS), which includes immediate (LM-I) and 30-min delayed (LM-II) trials of prose recall, is a large contributor to discriminating between healthy older adults and individuals with very mild AD. Guillozet et al. reported that AD pathology is more numerous in medial temporal lobe regions asso- ciated with the LM scores of the revised version of WMS (WMS-R), 3 and shows a relationship with LM performance on the WMS-R in individuals in the non- demented stage. 4 Although the relative ability of memory tests to discriminate between the AD conver- tor type of MCI and normal aging has not been well characterized, a previous study reported that the LM-II was one of the best predictors for detecting pro- gression from MCI to AD over a 4-year period. 1 The LM of the WMS-R is one of the standard memory criteria for MCI clinical and research; for example, in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative study. 5 Accepted for publication 16 October 2012. Correspondence: Ms Naoko Kawano PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya University, 65 Tsurumai-cho, Showa-ku, Nagoya 466-8550, Japan. Email: [email protected] Geriatr Gerontol Int 2013; 13: 726–730 726 © 2012 Japan Geriatrics Society doi: 10.1111/ggi.12007

Transcript of Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

Page 1: Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

ORIGINAL ARTICLE: EPIDEMIOLOGY,CLINICAL PRACTICE AND HEALTH

Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of theWechsler Memory Scale-Revised Logical Memory subtest forold-old people

Naoko Kawano,1,2 Shuichi Awata,1 Mutsuo Ijuin,1 Kunihiro Iwamoto2 and Norio Ozaki2

1Research Team for Promoting Independence of the Elderly, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology, Tokyo, and 2Department ofPsychiatry, Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya, Japan

Aim: Episodic memory is vulnerable to deterioration in people suffering from Alzheimer’s disease. Currently, theLogical Memory (LM) subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) is used internationally as anoperational definition to identify people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). However, the Japanese version of theLM has not been adequately normalized for old-old people. Therefore, norms of the LM for people aged 75 years andover are required, and the effects of sex, age and education on performance were evaluated.

Methods: A total of 50 (27 female and 23 male) participants without a history of dementia and symptomatic strokeevents recruited from the community and hospital populations were investigated using the Mini-Mental StateExamination, the LM and some interference tasks.

Results: The mean scores (standard deviations) of the sample were 15.5 (5.4) on LM-I and 9.9 (6.6) on LM-II. Thedistributions of the LM-I and -II scores satisfied the normality assumption. The LM-I and LM-II scores correlatedwith age and the LM-I score correlated with educational background.

Conclusions: For the Japanese version of the LM, the means, standard deviations and distribution features of theold-old sample are presented. Although the normal sample was chosen to closely match the demographic profile ofthe Japanese population, the present sample might have had a higher educational background than the age-matchedpopulation, especially the males. Further study is required to standardize the Japanese version of the LM subtest foreach 5-year interval for latter-stage elderly people. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2013; 13: 726–730.

Keywords: episodic memory, Logical Memory, mild cognitive impairment, normative data, old-old people.

Introduction

A mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as a result of Alzhe-imer’s disease (AD) is seen as memory impairment, andthis symptom is the key early marker in the prodromalstages of AD.1 Although the memory deficits in indi-viduals with MCI are clinically discernible, in order tomake a diagnosis of MCI, amnesia that does not inter-fere notably with activities of daily life (ADL) must beidentified. It is operationally defined as performance1.0–1.5 standard deviations (SD) below age- andeducation-adjusted norms on an episodic memorymeasure of delayed verbal recall.2

The Logical Memory (LM) subtest of the WechslerMemory Scale (WMS), which includes immediate(LM-I) and 30-min delayed (LM-II) trials of proserecall, is a large contributor to discriminating betweenhealthy older adults and individuals with very mildAD. Guillozet et al. reported that AD pathology ismore numerous in medial temporal lobe regions asso-ciated with the LM scores of the revised version ofWMS (WMS-R),3 and shows a relationship with LMperformance on the WMS-R in individuals in the non-demented stage.4 Although the relative ability ofmemory tests to discriminate between the AD conver-tor type of MCI and normal aging has not beenwell characterized, a previous study reported that theLM-II was one of the best predictors for detecting pro-gression from MCI to AD over a 4-year period.1 TheLM of the WMS-R is one of the standard memorycriteria for MCI clinical and research; for example,in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiativestudy.5

Accepted for publication 16 October 2012.

Correspondence: Ms Naoko Kawano PhD, Department ofPsychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, Nagoya University,65 Tsurumai-cho, Showa-ku, Nagoya 466-8550, Japan.Email: [email protected]

bs_bs_banner

Geriatr Gerontol Int 2013; 13: 726–730

726 � © 2012 Japan Geriatrics Societydoi: 10.1111/ggi.12007

Page 2: Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

Various factors have been associated with LM testscore differences. Abikoff et al. have already reportedthat age and education norms are generated for imme-diate, 30-min delayed and 24-h delayed recall in the LMof the WMS, and performance is more closely related toeducational background.6 Their sample ranged in agefrom 18 to 81 years, with a mean educational level of13.96 years (range 6–18 years). The LM performanceincrease is somewhat more common with higher levelsof education.6,7 However, Abikoff et al. noted that“Although education was more highly related to scoresthan was age, small but significant relationshipsbetween age and verbal recall remained over and abovethe influence of education.6“ The impact of age is mostobvious in 24-h delayed recall, and drop-off in perfor-mance occurs over the age of 60 years. Therefore, thelatest version of the WMS has paid attention to elderlyparticipants in the form of advancing an elderly battery.

However, the WMS-R version is the only LM task thathas been standardized for Japanese people, and the nor-mative sample has been limited to the ages of between 16and 74 years. The incidences of AD, combined dementiaand other types of dementia rise with increasing age,particularly after the age of 85 years.8 Although not onlyfor young-old people, but also for old-old or oldest-oldpeople, an amnestic state examination of high accuracy isrequired, because the Japanese versions of the WMS-R,LM-I and LM-II have not been adequately normalizedfor latter-stage elderly people. In the current study, nor-mative data for the LM in Japanese elderly people aged75 years and older were gathered.

Methods

Participants

A total of 50 (27 female and 23 male) participantswithout a history of dementia and symptomatic strokeevents were recruited from the community and hospitalpopulations living in two urban areas. All participantscould attend the trial sites alone. The sample size deter-mination was based on the original version of theWMS-R and the general recommendation on statisticsin psychology and education, taking a sample of 50 andover per age group interval.9 A total of 30 participants(60%) had no history of psychiatric problem as assessedby the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnosticand Statistical Manual of Mental Disoriders, FourthEdition revised (SCID),10 and they did not report clinicalevidence of amnesia and ADL impairment. There isnothing to suggest that participants did not hear some-thing at the time of auditory stimulus presentation inthe study. They ranged in age between 75 and 87 years(mean � SD: 79.3 � 3.6 years), and in educationalbackground between 6 and 18 years (mean � SD:11.7 � 3.1 years).

The ethics committee of the Tokyo MetropolitanInstitute of Gerontology and the Nagoya UniversitySchool of Medicine approved the present study, andeach participant signed a consent form after being suf-ficiently informed about the outline of the study by theprincipal investigator.

Tasks

Logical Memory (LM)-I and -II from the Japaneseversion of the WMS-R11 were carried out. In the LM-I,participants were asked to immediately recall from thenumber of prose units twice: the first trial presentedstory A verbally, and the second trial presented story Bverbally. In the LM-II, participants were asked to recallwords from the two stories 30 min later. During thetime delay, participants were asked to carry out theMini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)12 and someinterference tasks.

In the present study, not all of the participants carriedout every task item, other than LM-I, II and MMSE. Asthe purpose of the present study was to provide norma-tive data for LM-I and -II, the sample size was kept thesame; hence, missing data were not substituted. Theresults are based on the eight task scales.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS 17.0 J.for Windows (SPSS, Chicago. IL, USA). Normative dataare provided in the form of means and standard devia-tions (SD) broken down by sex, age and educationalbackground. Correlation analyses between LM scoresand various factors were carried out using the Pearsonproduct-moment correlation coefficient. A P-value ofless than 0.05 was considered significant. The percentilerank of each LM-I or -II score was calculated, after theShapiro–Wilk test was carried out to check the normal-ity of the sampling distribution.

Results

Sample characteristics

In the present sample, the mean � SD score of MMSE(27.3 � 2.2) reflected the expected distribution ofgeneral cognitive status for aged groups. The normativesample was confirmed to match closely the demo-graphic profile of this population as reported in a recentcensus. Table 1 shows the percentiles of the normaliza-tion sample by age, sex and educational backgroundcompared with these population averages in Japan (Sta-tistics Bureau 2010: Ministry of Internal Affairs andCommunications). The results showed that the samplemight have had a higher educational background thanJapan’s age-matched population.

Norms of LM subtest for old-old Japanese

© 2012 Japan Geriatrics Society � 727

Page 3: Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

Reference data of the normal group

Mean scores (SD) of the sample were 15.5 (5.4) on LM-Iand 9.9 (6.6) on LM-II. Table 2 summarizes the perfor-mance of the sample. To check the normality of thesampling distribution, coefficients of skewness and kur-tosis were calculated for each trial. In the LM-I, theskewness value was 0.19 and the kurtosis value was-0.89; in the LM-II, the skewness value was 0.32 andthe kurtosis value was -0.82. The distributions of theLM-I and -II scores satisfied the normality assumptionusing the Shapiro–Wilk test (P > 0.05).

Characteristics and performances

To examine the effect of sex on performance, unpairedt-tests comparing the LM-I and -II scores in male andfemale participants were carried out. In both the LM-Iand -II, no significant difference was found. The meanscores (SD) of the male group were 15.6 (5.5) on theLM-I and 10.8 (6.4) on the LM-II, compared with 16.3(5.5) on the LM-I and 10.0 (6.8) on the LM-II in thefemale group.

To examine associations between age (years) or edu-cational background (years) and LM scores, correlationanalyses were carried out. The LM-I and LM-II scoreswere moderately correlated with age (r = -0.44, P < 0.01;r = -0.45, P < 0.01), and the LM-I score was moderately

correlated with educational background (r = 0.36,P < 0.05). There was no significant correlation betweenthe LM-II score and educational background (r = 0.23,not significant.). The figures show scatter plots of theWMS-R LM scores and age in years (Fig. 1), or years ofeducation (Fig. 2). Considering that the sample had amoderate to high education, partial correlation analysesbetween age (years) and LM scores were carried out.The LM-I and LM-II scores were moderately correlatedwith age (r = -0.36, P < 0.05; r = -0.40, P < 0.01).

Discussion

In the current study, LM normal performances ofhealthy Japanese people aged 75 years and older weresurveyed, and the effects of sex, age and education onperformance were identified. The means, SD and dis-tribution features of the LM-I and -II of the WMS-R arepresented for Japanese old-old people.

The sample had mean (SD) scores of 15.5 (3.2) onthe LM-I and 9.9 (6.6) on the LM-II. According to

Table 1 Percentiles of the normalization sample by age, sex and educational background

Age (years) Sex Education0–11 (years) 12 (years) >13 (years)Sample Population

(Japan)Sample Population

(Japan)Sample Population

(Japan)

>75 Male 25.9 45.6 18.5 37.6 40.7 16.8Female 63.0 53.3 18.5 41.3 18.5 5.4Total 48.0 50.3 20.0 39.9 32.0 9.8

The estimated population was calculated excluding active students and unknown individuals of education backgrounds fromtotal number.

Table 2 Performance of the sample aged 75 yearsand older

Mean SD (min–max)

LM-I 15.5 � 5.4 (5–8)Story A 8.3 � 3.2 (3–16)Story B 7.4 � 2.8 (2–14)

LM-II 9.9 � 6.6 (0–25)Story A 5.0 � 3.8 (0–13)Story B 4.9 � 3.2 (0–12)

I, immediate recall; II, delayed recall; LM, Wechsler MemoryScale-Revised Logical Memory subtest.

WM

S-R

LM s

core

50

40

30

20

10

0

LM- I

LM- II

(LM- I)

(LM- II)

0 75 80 85 90Age (years)

Figure 1 The scatter plot of Wechsler MemoryScale-Revised Logical Memory (WMS-RLM) subtest scores(y-axis) and age in years (x-axis). I, immediate recall; II,delayed recall; LM, Logical Memory subtest.

N Kawano et al.

728 � © 2012 Japan Geriatrics Society

Page 4: Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

Sugishita,11 people in each age group (16–17 years(n = 50), 20–24 years (n = 54), 35–44 years (n = 56),55–64 years (n = 50), 65–69 years (n = 52) and70–74 years (n = 54)) had the following scores: on theLM-I, 27.7 (7.2), 26.6 (6.4), 25.1 (7.5), 22.0 (7.1), 19.5(6.8) and 18.5 (7.5), respectively; and on the LM-II, 24.9(7.7), 22.8 (6.7), 20.7 (7.6), 16.8 (7.0), 15.3 (7.0) and 13.2(6.8), respectively. These results are consistent with pre-vious data, and indicate an age-related decrease in LMtask performance.6,13 Considering the Flynn effect, thepresent data should not be compared directly with theprevious data by Sugishita.11 It is recommended that alarger study for the normalization of LM in older peoplebe carried out.

Furthermore, the study showed that the LM-I and -IIscores were moderately negatively correlated with age ina healthy sample aged 75 years and older. In particular,the LM-II score reflected the individual difference asso-ciated with age, independent of educational back-ground. The result also confirms the age-relatedchanges in memory functions. This finding, that theLM-II was not correlated with education leaves roomfor interpretation. Although the present sample fromamong community-dwelling older adults had generallybetter health and education, high-risk MCI personsmight have been present in definite proportions, or thenormal population might have individuals who, despiteeducational levels, may have been less able in cognitiveabilities throughout their life.14,15 According to the Mayoclinic’s team, the LM-II data were not correlated witheducation in a community-based healthy sample.15

They noted that the education-WMS performanceassociation in the restricted age range of their oldersample did not reflect true underlying relationshipsbetween the intelligence quotient (IQ) and task perfor-mance, and they recommended that WMS norms be

stratified by IQ. The education-LM performance asso-ciation might reflect these confounding factors.

Although the present sample was chosen to closelymatch the demographic profile of the Japanese popula-tion, the sample might have had a higher educationalbackground than the age-matched population, espe-cially among males. Community-based surveys in ruralareas should also be carried out at the same time assurveys in urban areas. Thus, the sample bias is inap-propriate for determining the “range of normal”memory functioning in an older population.15 Normsstratified to be representative of the general populationhave great diagnostic value. However, the present resultshowed that the aging-related memory decline wasobserved in highly educated people, who had a greaterlikelihood of preserving cognitive function than peoplewith low educational achievement. The result suggeststhat normalization of LM must be carried out forlatter-stage elderly Japanese people. To establish thenorms for the Japanese version of the LM, a furthercommunity-based study using the Intelligence Scale inparallel will be necessary. In addition, it will be necessaryto compare between the LM norms based on theseparately-carried out condition and that based on thecompletely-carried out WMS-R condition, and to nor-malize the latest version of WMS in Japanese people,because the latest version has a short battery for ages65–90 years (the Older Adult Battery), including thenew LM composed of the 14-paragraph-story (story A)and the 25-paragraph-story (story B).

The present estimated values based on LM scores ofpeople aged 75 years and older, which are currentlybased on the population aged less than 75 years, showthat current percentile ranks underestimate the memoryability of people aged 75 years and older. Furthermore,the present study obviously showed that the LM-I andLM-II scores were correlated with age. These resultssuggest the necessity of normative data on the Japaneseversion of the WMS-R LM subtest for each 5-year inter-val for the population aged 75 years and older, like theoriginal version. In the future, for old-old people, it willbe necessary to carry out a survey to establish norms ofthe WMS-R LM for each 5-year interval.

Acknowledgments

Funding for this study was provided by research grantsfrom the following: the Japan Society for the Promotionof Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Number 24730577and the Japan Health Foundation.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.

WM

S-R

LM s

core

50

40

30

20

10

0

LM- I

LM- II

(LM- I)

(LM- II)

0

Education (years)

5 10 15 20

Figure 2 The scatter plot of Wechsler MemoryScale-Revised Logical Memory (WMS-RLM) scores (y-axis)and years of education (x-axis). I, immediate recall; II,delayed recall; LM, Logical Memory subtest.

Norms of LM subtest for old-old Japanese

© 2012 Japan Geriatrics Society � 729

Page 5: Necessity of normative data on the Japanese version of the logical memory.pdf

References

1 Rabin LA, Paré N, Saykin AJ et al. Differential memory testsensitivity for diagnosing amnestic mild cognitive impair-ment and predicting conversion to Alzheimer’s disease.Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2009; 16:357–376.

2 Jack CR Jr, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML et al. 11C PiB and struc-tural MRI provide complementary information in imagingof Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impair-ment. Brain 2008; 131 (Pt 3): 665–880.

3 Wecheler D. Wechsler Memory Scale-reviced: Manual. SanAntonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1987.

4 Guillozet AL, Weintraub S, Mash DC, Mesulam MM.Neurofibrillary tangles, amyloid, and memory in aging andmild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2003; 60: 729–736.

5 Petersen RC, Aisen PS, Beckett LA et al. Alzheimer’sDisease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): clinical charac-terization. Neurology 2010; 74: 201–209.

6 Abikoff H, Alvir J, Hong G et al. Logical Memory subtest ofthe Wechsler Memory Scale: age and education norms andalternate-form reliability of two scoring systems. J Clin ExpNeuropsychol 1987; 9: 435–448.

7 Economou A. Memory score discrepancies by healthymiddle-aged and older individuals: the contributions of ageand education. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009; 15: 963–972.

8 Matsui Y, Tanizaki Y, Arima H et al. Incidence and survivalof dementia in a general population of Japanese elderly: theHisayama Study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2009; 80:366–370.

9 First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. User’sGuide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis IDisorders-clinival Version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC:American Psychiatric Press, 1997.

10 D’elia L, Satz P, Schretlen D. Wechsler Memory Scale: acritical appraisal of the normative studies. J Clin Exp Neu-ropsychol 1989; 11: 551–568.

11 Sugishita M. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (Japanese).Tokyo: Nihonbunkakagakusya, 2001.

12 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mentalstate”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state ofpatients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–198.

13 Drozdick LW, Holdnack JA, Hilsabeck RC. Essentials ofWMS-IV Assessment: Essentials of Psychological Assessment.Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 2009.

14 Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, KokmenE, Kurland LT. Wechsler Memory Scale: IQ-dependentnorms for persons age 65 to 97 years. Psychol Assess 1991; 3:156–161.

15 Malec JF, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Petersen RC, Tangalos EG.Clusters of impaired normal elderly do not decline cogni-tively in 3 to 5 years. Neuropsychology 1996; 10: 66–73.

N Kawano et al.

730 � © 2012 Japan Geriatrics Society