Nasality Malone 652753

download Nasality Malone 652753

of 42

Transcript of Nasality Malone 652753

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    1/42

    Nasality and Nasal Prosody in ChimilaReview by: Terry MaloneInternational Journal of American Linguistics, Vol. 76, No. 1 (January 2010), pp. 1-41Published by: The University of Chicago PressStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/652753.

    Accessed: 24/09/2012 23:23

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    .JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    .

    The University of Chicago Pressis collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to

    International Journal of American Linguistics.

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ucpresshttp://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/652753?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/652753?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ucpress
  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    2/42

    1

    [

    IJAL

    , vol. 76, no. 1, January 2010, pp. 141]

    2010 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved.00207071/2010/76010001$10.0

    NASALITY AND NASAL PROSODY IN CHIMILA

    1

    Terry Malone

    Latin American Mission/FUSBC Medelln

    In Chimila, nasal consonants contrast with voiced stops at the same point of articu-

    lation and no surface phonetic contrast occurs between nasalized and oral vowels in

    core lexemes. Nevertheless, several lines of evidence indicate that the Chimila lexicon

    includes nasal morphemes in contrast with oral morphemes. Evidence for this contrast

    includes morphophonemic alternations of stem-forming verb suffixes, appearance of a

    glottal glide following intransitive verb roots that have been transitivized, allomorphs

    of the intransitive imperative, and a restricted alternation between word-initial voiced

    stops and nasal consonants in roots and some suffixes. A constraint requiring lexical

    nasality to attach to consonants in core lexemes and the interaction of this constraintwith the prosodic system, lexical tone, syllable structure, and the morphology account

    for the varied manifestations of lexical nasality.

    [

    Keywords

    : Chimila, subsegments, nasality, nasal prosody, nasal vowels]

    1. Introduction.

    Chimila is a Chibchan language spoken in northern

    Colombia within a community of 1,600 persons. Most published works on

    Chimila phonology and grammar (Trillos 1997 and Malone 2000; 2001;

    2004) do not mention the existence of nasal prosody or phonetically nasal-

    ized vowels in interjections and onomatopoeia; another paper (Malone

    2006:24, 26) barely mentions the existence of nasal prosody and nasal lexi-cal roots in two footnotes. Nevertheless, evidence exists indicating that na-

    sality is present, and that nasal prosody operates within lexical stems.

    In this paper, I review the evidence and argue that nasality is present in the

    Chimila lexicon, although no phonemic surface contrast exists between oral

    and nasalized vowels, except that of some interjections and onomatopoeia.

    I first present necessary information concerning Chimila phonology (

    2

    ) and

    then describe manifestations of phonetic nasality in core lexemes, interjec-

    tions, and onomatopoeia (

    3

    ). 4

    presents evidence for lexical nasality, includ-

    ing morphophonemic alternations (

    4.1

    ), insertion of syllable-final /h/ (

    4.2

    ),

    1 I would like to express special appreciation to Margrit Hotz, who has provided much logis-

    tical support and thus has made it possible to conduct this analysis and write this paper. The

    Chimila data in this paper come entirely from my field notes. I am grateful to many Chimila

    friends who have generously shared their language; unfortunately, names cannot be mentioned

    here due to political conditions in their homeland. I am grateful to the editors and three referees

    for comments on an earlier draft which have considerably improved this paper. Of course, any

    defects are mine.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    3/42

    international journal of american linguistics

    2

    and insertion of consonants /t/ and /k/ in the intransitive imperative (

    4.3

    ). 5

    presents a unified analysis accounting for these phenomena. In 5.1

    I argue

    that lexical nasality must attach to consonants in core lexemes and that the

    interaction of this constraint with lexical tone, prosody, syllable structure,and the morphology accounts for the phenomena of 4, whereas in onomato-

    poeia and interjections lexical nasality must attach to vowels

    (5.2

    ). In 5.3

    I

    account for the variation in realization between voiced stops and nasals, by

    proposing optional nasalization of the root-initial consonant; this allows one

    to determine underlying forms of suffixes that show this variation. Section

    6

    presents conclusions based on the proposed analysis.

    2. An overview of Chimila phonology.

    This overview is based on the

    analysis presented in Malone (2001; 2006); more detailed information is pre-sented concerning segments and clusters crucial for understanding how lexi-

    cal nasality functions. The inventory of Chimila consonant phonemes is as

    follows:

    (1) Bilabial Coronal Velar Glottal

    Voiceless

    p t k

    ?

    Stops

    Voiced stops

    b d g

    Nasals

    m n

    N

    Fricatives

    s h

    Lateral

    l

    Flap

    \

    Glides

    w y

    Voiceless stops are nonaspirated. All voiced stops are prenasalized. All coro-

    nal segments are dental, except for the flap and the glide. The labial glide is

    preceded by a light nonprenasalized velar voiced stop; the alveopalatal glide

    is preceded by a light nonprenasalized alveopalatal stop. Consonants occurwith three phonetic lengths: short, medium long, and long. Consonant length

    is not phonemic: some cases are analyzed as heteromorphemic geminates,

    some occur in order to fulfill a minimal bimoraic trochaic foot requirement,

    and most occur as the result of the interaction of lexical tone with the met-

    rical system (syllables bearing lexical tone must be bimoraic, i.e., heavy).

    (See Malone 2006 for more details on lexical tone, parsing constraints, and

    their relationship to consonant length and syllable structure.)

    The glottal fricative /h/ occurs in word-initial position in Chimila; it does

    not occur word-medially except in interjections (see 6 in 3.2

    ), compoundwords ([k

    a

    h

    hk

    \

    a

    b

    \

    ] tree trunk (

    ka

    h

    tree, ha

    k

    \

    a

    ?

    head, -b

    \i

    chunk),

    and loanwords ([h

    u

    h] needle, from Spanish aguja

    ). In all such words it oc-

    curs in the syllable onset. Preceding the vowels /o, a/ the fricative exhibits

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    4/42

    nasal prosody in chimila

    3

    the allophone [h]; preceding the vowels /e, i, u/ the stricture of the glottal fri-

    cative narrows, so that it tends to vary freely with a velar fricative [x].

    A glottal off-glide occurs word-finally in verb roots of the syllable struc-

    ture /(C)(C)V/. Its stricture in this position is highly variable from word toword but constant for each word; in syllable-final position the stricture can-

    not be predicted from the height of the preceding vowel. For instance, the

    stricture of the off-glide in [wi

    h

    ~ wi] know, be able to is considerably less

    than that of [si

    h

    ] black or [i

    h

    ] be hard. It is often inaudible and, unlike the

    off-glide of [si

    h

    ] and [i

    h

    ], disappears in compound words or when the root is

    suffixed. Further, light stricture, as in [o

    h

    ~ o] do, contrasts with heavy

    stricture, as in [o

    h

    ] roast.

    The distribution of this off-glide is restricted in syllable-final position,

    much more so than any other consonant occurring in the syllable coda (see

    4.2.2

    ). There is some question about whether it should be analyzed as a syl-

    lable-final occurrence of the phoneme /h/; in fact, Trillos (1997) analyzes it

    as an articulatory modification of the preceding vowel (1997:66), i.e., as

    breathy vowels (/Vh/ in contrast with /V/). Because the glottal off-glide in

    syllable-final position is restricted in distribution, does not behave the same

    as syllable-initial /h/ with respect to stricture, and is associated with lexical

    nasality (

    4.2

    ), the term glottal off-glide ([

    h

    ]) is used here, in order to dis-

    tinguish it from the syllable-initial glottal fricative. Its distribution and pho-

    nemic status is further considered in 4.2

    and 5.1

    , where I conclude that it isbest analyzed as a syllable-final occurrence of the phoneme /h/.

    The following monosyllabic consonant clusters occur in syllable-onset

    position: /p

    \

    /, /b

    \

    /, /k

    \

    /, /g

    \

    /, /kw/, /gw/, /kw

    \

    /, /gw

    \

    /, /w

    \

    /, /ty/, /dy/, and

    /ny/. In syllable-initial position, the last three clusters are manifested respec-

    tively as an alveopalatal affricate preceded by a light voiceless dental stop

    [

    t

    c

    ], a prenasalized alveopalatal glide in which a light alveopalatal stop in-

    tervenes between the dental prenasalization and the glide /

    nd

    y], and an al-

    veopalatal nasal [].

    2

    Articulatory, acoustic, and morphophonemic evidence support this analy-sis of these clusters. The palatalization associated with the phonetic reali-

    zation of the clusters /ty/, /dy/, /ny/ is longer than one would expect for

    palatalization as an articulatory modificationin fact, three times as long

    (Malone 2000:7677). When a CV syllable bearing lexical tone occurs pre-

    ceding the clusters /ty/ and /dy/ in word-medial position, the clusters split

    into a dental syllable coda and an alveopalatal syllable onset:

    (2

    a

    ) [

    n

    dt.

    c

    ata] go in!

    (2

    b

    ) [kn.a] field

    2 I do not reproduce predictable phonetic details in the data of this paper unless they are

    germane to the discussion at hand.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    5/42

    international journal of american linguistics

    4

    In (2

    b

    ) the dental nasal is especially audible and shows clearly in acoustic

    diagrams (Malone 2000:7375). /ty/ occurs in contexts where one must con-

    clude that two segments have combined. For example, the intransitive im-

    perative morpheme [-ya] has the allomorph [-

    c

    a] in contexts where a glottalstop is part of the preceding monosyllabic verb root (3

    c

    ) (note that in 3

    b

    the

    glottal stop occurs at the end of the stem).

    (3

    a

    ) [saa

    \

    a.

    d

    ya] sleep! (/saa

    \

    a/ sleep)

    (3

    b

    ) [kita

    ?

    .

    d

    ya] sing! (/k-ta

    ?

    / sing)

    (3

    c

    ) [yut.

    c

    a] go! (/yu

    ?

    / go)

    Shortened forms in fast speech also suggest that [

    c

    ] represents two com-

    bined segments, for instance, [h

    c

    ] house instead of [ht

    ak] and [Ng\u

    ckw] instead of [Ng\takkw] elders, older person, where two syllablesfuse to form the syllable [ca].

    Lexical syllable structure is summarized by the formulas (X)V (C2),

    (X)V(C2), and (X)VV(C3), where X can be /p/, /t/, /k/, /b/, /d/, /g/, /m/, /n/,

    /N/, /h/, /s /, / l/, /\/, /w/, /y/, or the clusters /p\/, /k\/, /b\/, /g\/, /w\/, /ty/, /dy/,

    /kw/, /gw/, /kw\/, /gw\/; in word-medial position, /?/ can occur as X. C2is

    filled by /m/, /N/, /?/, and in word-medial position /n / also occurs in this set.

    Only /?/ occurs as C3.

    Chimila vowel phonemes are given below (from Malone 2006):

    (4) i u

    e o

    a

    Vowels occur with three contrastive phonetic lengths: short, medium long,

    and long. Medium long vowels are analyzed as canonically long vowels

    (monosyllabic, bearing one single phonetic tone), and long vowels as gem-

    inate vowels (disyllabic, bearing rising or falling phonetic tone) (see Malone

    2001 for details). High tone contrasts with its absence on monosyllabic long

    vowels, as in /k?/ to buy vs. /ki?/ to receive. On disyllabic vowels afour-way contrast occurs, as in /too?/ maraca, /to?/ heart, /t?/ gourd,

    /tii\i/ thin, /ti\o?/ hawk. Canonically long and geminate vowels contrast,

    as in /to?/ to observe, look vs. /too?/ maraca or /k?/ to buy vs. /ki?/

    sap. On CV syllables high tone contrasts with its absence, as in /\ku?/

    species of owl vs. /\uku \uku/ rub, or /ska / ([saka]) someone ground

    vs. /sAka/ ([saka]) someone tamped down earth; in these cases a require-

    ment that syllables bearing lexical tone be heavy results in the lengthening

    of the consonant immediately following the vowel bearing high tone.3

    3 According to the analysis presented in this paper, the vowels of the verb roots grind and

    tamp down earth are lexically nasal. Phonetic nasality is not present in their phonetic forms,

    so I use them here to illustrate the tone contrast. Finding contrasts that do not involve at least

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    6/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 5

    Word-level phonetic stress is predictable: it always occurs on the leftmost

    syllable of the word and therefore is not marked in this paper. One default

    phonetic high tone occurs per word. The syllable locus for the default high

    tone depends on the number of syllables in the word, and whether or not thesyllable bears lexical tone. The leftmost syllable is the unmarked position for

    word-level high tone; in this paper I do not mark default word-level high

    tone. Words parse from left to right into bimoraic trochaic feet, in accor-

    dance with proposals in Hayes (1995); see Malone (2006) for further details

    regarding foot structure and parsing.

    3. Phonetic nasality: an overview.

    3.1. Conditioned nasal vowels. In core lexical vocabulary, phonetic vowelnasalization is conditioned by the presence of contiguous nasal consonants.

    Vowels are nasalized immediately preceding nasal consonants and immedi-

    ately following word-medial nasal consonants. Nasalization varies consider-

    ably preceding nasal consonants; it is most audible on back vowels and less

    audible or inaudible on front vowels. As might be expected, it is heavier in syl-

    lables with nasals in both the onset and the coda, and is usually heavy fol-

    lowing nasal consonants in word-final syllables of the form CV(?). Nasal

    consonants following a glottal off-glide are often partially and sometimes

    completely devoiced, with concomitant heavy nasalization of the immediatelyfollowing vowel; the preceding vowel is nasal, but not as heavy. If the nasal

    consonant devoices completely, as in (5), the preceding vowel is nasalized:4

    (5)

    /ahNa-ta-ke?e-de/

    bring-toward-fut.d.o.-1ev

    Ill bring it back.

    4 Because phonetic devoicing in this environment and nasalization of vowels contiguous to

    nasal consonants are predictable, I do not indicate them elsewhere in this paper, unless germane

    to the discussion. The following abbreviations are used in the glosses of this paper: 1 = first

    person; 2 = second person; 3 = third person; sg = singular; pl = plural; d.o. = direct object; adj

    adjective; benbenefactive; causcausative; condconditional; conjconjunction; contcontin-uative; evevidential; futfuture; gengenitive; impimperative; intinterrogative; invinverse;

    itr intransitive; mid middle voice; neg negative; nomz nominalizer; obj direct or indirect

    object; pl plural; ser serial verb linker; stem transitive stem-forming suffix; pas past; pat

    semantic patient; posspossessive; sg singular; statstative; toptopic.

    [?hN6takE?Ente]

    one lexically nasal vowel (such as /u$ka/ [uka] to drink and /kwa/ [uk

    w

    a] to cut) is diffi-cult. A possibility is /taka?/ with force and /tka?/ turn sour. Speakers perceive the velar

    stop of turn sour as longer than that of with force; acoustic measurements (two tokens of

    each) have been inconclusive. The acute accent indicates lexical tone; I mark phonetic high

    ( ! ), mid (-), and low ( ~) tone on phonetic data where it is pertinent to the discussion. Phonetic

    mid and low tone are inserted by default after the high tone representing word-level tone has

    been assigned and the phonological word has been footed.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    7/42

    international journal of american linguistics6

    Vowel nasalization preceding voiceless nasal consonants in this position is

    not attributable to the off-glide: no nasalization can be heard in vowels pre-

    ceding the off-glide in words such as [kah] tree, stick, [kah mb\nta] piece

    of wood. [ah] hair/to go, or [sihtu?] its black.

    3.2. Oralnasal vowel contrast in interjections and onomatopoeia.

    Nonallophonic nasalized vowels are found in some interjections and ono-

    matopoeia. (6) lists interjections containing nasalized vowels:

    (6) [?], [?h] yes!

    [h ~ hoh] here it is (take it)!

    [h?h?~ h?] right here! (pointing and touching place)

    [hh] no!

    [?E$hE$?E$] ok![h] huh?

    [?ehnI] ok, agreed!

    [ayo] expression of pity

    [mISu] kitty, kitty!

    Note that the nasalization is not likely due to rhinoglottophilia (spontaneous

    nasalization occurring after the phoneme /h/) as described for Iapari

    (Arawakan) in Parker (1999). For one thing, two interjections occur contain-

    ing two oral vowels and one nasal vowel: [?oho?oh] hi!; [?Ehe$e$I] eek!(reaction at being startled). If nasalization were due to rhinoglottophilia,

    one would expect all the vowels to be nasalized in these interjections. Sec-

    ond, other interjections occur in which oral vowels occur contiguous to /h,

    ?/ and which contrast with nasal vowels in the interjections above, for in-

    stance, [?ee] well, well!,5[?ookya] well, ok then!, [ha??] ok!, [hoo\i]

    of course!, [ha?\iyu\i] you bet!. In addition, numerous words beginning

    with /h/ occur in the core lexicon whose vowels are not phonetically nasal-

    ized and which contrast with the nasalized vowels of the interjections in (6)

    above:(7) /oo?/ [?oo?] ax /e/ [?e] to be at

    /h?o/ [ho?o] scrape it! /h?/ [he?] to toast

    /hk\u?/ [hok\u?] housefly /haa ?/ [haa?] shelf

    /hoowa/ [hoowa] to bathe

    Nasalized vowels also occur in onomatopoeia:

    (8a) [isa Ngo$ki Ngki ni huu\a mbuu\u sawi\i]

    3.conj heehaw heehaw ser shout donkey male

    The jack brays heehaw heehaw .

    5 Older speakers pronounce this as [?e$e$e$], with rising intonation.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    8/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 7

    (8b) [isa noNwa we$e$? we$e$? ni noNwa]6

    3.conj hear ribbet ribbet ser hear-mid

    One hears them (frogs), one hears ribbet ribbet .

    (8c) [isa niti? tu\uku]

    3.conj grunt itr hog

    The hog grunts.

    In these and other examples, nasalized vowels can occur following a stop,

    a semivowel, the flap, the liquid, and the sibilant. They contrast with oral

    vowels in other onomatopoeia:

    (9a) [isa niya]

    3.conj clonk clonk say

    It clonks lightly. (dog gnawing a bone)

    (9b) [isa kwe?kwe? niti ni tutikwi tu\uku]

    3.conj squeal itr its little (pl) pig

    Piglets squeal.

    (9c) [isa \u\u? \u\u? niya pek \u isa suh]

    3.conj grrr grrr say dog 3.conj angry

    The dog growls when it is mad.

    As in the case of interjections, the nasalized vowels contrast with oral vow-

    els in core lexemes, for example, /wee?/ be hot, /gota/ to get dressed, and

    /\oo\o/ sp. anteater. In lexemes that are not onomatopoeia or interjections,

    nasalized vowels only occur contiguous to nasal consonants.7

    In spite of this and other phonological differences, onomatopoeia in (8)

    (and elsewhere in the language) behave as normal intransitive verbs: the

    serial verb marker /ni/ follows the onomatopoeia of (8a) and (8b), as if the

    animal sounds were the first verb in a serial verb construction; and in (8c)the intransitive verb suffix /ni?ti?/ is used to indicate sounds or involuntary

    actions, for example, /gim gim ni?ti?/ to blink on and off or /ansi ansi

    ni?ti?/ to sneeze. In other words, the onomatopoeia are inflected as if they

    6 This speaker alternatively pronounces ribbet ribbet as [Nwe$e$?Nwe$e$?], i.e., she optionally

    nasalizes the glide.7 The phonology of onomatopoeia differs in two other respects from that of core lexemes:

    (1) voiced and voiceless stops can appear in syllable-final position, in contrast with nasal con-

    sonants and the glottal stop: [p\?p\?] noise of soldiers marching, [p\k p\k] noise of cartires clicking, [k\?k\?] or [k\?k\?] noise of joints cracking, [sk sk] noise of clock tick-

    ing; [gwp gwp] swish of tail wagging, [k\g ndy] to make a snapping or creaking noise

    (tree branches); and (2) word-final glottal stop does not delete in phrase-internal position.

    \h\h

    Ngoi Ngoi

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    9/42

    international journal of american linguistics8

    were regular verb stems in the language. Furthermore, onomatopoeia and

    other verbs exhibit derivational relationships, for example, [tontn] sound

    of dripping water [tnt nt:?] to drip and [tntoNka] put drops in or

    [tUtUtU] sound of knocking and [tuntntuhNt:i?] to knock on door (eachset of words is from speakers of a distinct dialect).8

    Other evidence suggests that onomatopoeia function as part of the Chim-

    ila lexicon, even if they are phonetically and semantically marked in the

    sense of Klamer (2002). In Chimila onomatopoeia are often a source of lexi-

    cal referents (or vice versa), as we saw with sound of dripping water and

    to knock on door. Other examples abound; a sampling includes [k\m], a

    species of owl that says [k\u\N k\u\N], [gwp gwp] swish of tail wagging,

    and [gwp gwp] to wag tail; the woodpecker makes the sound [ta?ta?ta?

    ta?] and [ta?tahnitaka] means to thunder; a species of crane [k\eu?] calls[k\eu? k\eu? k\eu?], [loN loN] sound of banging on hollow object and

    [lhlond?~ hohllond?] hollow, [k\k k\k niy?] to squeak or make

    a cracking sound (pack saddle) and [k\?k\?] snapping or popping sound

    ( joints). To some degree interjections also appear to migrate back and forth

    between the core and the margins of the lexicon, for instance, [hohkwa] take

    it! ([h ~ hoh] take it! [-kwa] pat) or [ookya] ah, yes! ([oo] ah [-kiya]

    then, consequently). These observations suggest that, just as Klamer (2002)

    assumes for Dutch and Kambera, onomatopoeia and interjections are part of

    the Chimila lexicon, and even though their phonology (and semantics) iscomplex, they must be taken into consideration if one is to adequately

    describe Chimila phonology.

    The most obvious hypothesis to account for this data would be to propose

    that a contrast between nasal and nonnasal vowels existed at one time in core

    lexemes and was manifested by a phonetically audible contrast between oral

    and nasal vowels; synchronically these contrasts have only been preserved

    in interjections and in onomatopoeia.

    3.3. Variation in realization between voiced stops and nasals. As in-dicated in the consonant chart in 2above, nasal consonants and voiced stops

    contrast at the same point of articulation. Examples of these contrasts are:

    (10) /bne?/ [mbn?] lots (mass)

    /mne?e?/ [mne??] to want

    /do/ [nd] it is bad

    /n/ [n] to hear

    8

    The last pair of words suggests that the phonological markedness of onomatopoeia cannotextend to regularly inflected verbs, and that at some point the phonological restrictions typical

    of core lexemes begin to operate on lexical roots derived from onomatopoeia. In fact, speakers

    of another dialect represent the sound of knocking as [tuN tuN].

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    10/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 9

    /dyO?/ [ndy?] hot pepper

    /nyoo/ [] cheek (buttock)

    /g?/ [Ng?] to grind again

    /No?/ [N?] to make

    In spite of these contrasts, free variation occurs in some words between

    voiced stops and nasals at the same point of articulation in both word-initial

    and word-medial positions (11). For each word I have listed the preferred or

    more frequent variant first.

    (11a) /b/ ~ /m/

    [mbee ~ mee] where?

    [masu?~m

    basu?] nearby[mbenta tu?~ menta tu?] to reject

    [me ?a\i?~ mbe?a\i?] to give birth

    [munti?~ mbunti?] sp. rodent

    [miniki ~ mbiniki] how?

    [\aamb\a?~ \aam\a?] mountain lion (archaic)

    (11b) /d/ ~ /n/

    [ndah~ nah] nose

    [ndap\i ~ nap\i] coffee (archaic)

    [nek\u?~ ndek\u?] dog (archaic)[tulunda?~ tuluna?] cut off (adj)

    (11c) /g/ ~ / N/

    [Naa\i ~ Ngaa\i] long

    [Nata?~ Ngata?] pulp

    [Ngatika?~ Natika?] to desert

    [Ngakwta ~ Nakwta] answer!

    [No?~ Ngo?] to make

    The alternation occurs between the consonant cluster /dy/ and the alveopal-

    atal nasal //, which in some word-initial occurrences represents the under-

    lying cluster /ny/; the semiconsonant /w/ and the word-medial cluster /Nw/

    also vary freely in some words:

    (12) [ndyaNNg\a?~ aNNg\a?] join of torso with thighs (in front)

    [ooni?~ ndyooni?] twisted

    [tii\igwa?~ tii\iNwa?] fruit of plane tree

    Speakers who admit this free variation are rarely able to distinguish betweenthe oral segment and its nasal counterpart; they are usually not even con-

    scious that this variation exists in their speech. At the same time many words

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    11/42

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    12/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 11

    carry), are suffixed by a limited set of suffixes to form lexical stems. Most

    of these suffixes indicate direction of movement or location. Their function

    in stem formation is illustrated in (14) for the following: /-Na/ stem, /-ka/

    away, out, /-k\i/ elevated, /-mta/ inside, /-ta/ up, toward, and /-t?/down, away:

    (14a) [sa ?ndi?] /sa ?-t?/ get down (off donkey)

    [sa?Nati?] /sa ?-Na-t?/ took (child) down (off donkey)

    [sa?Ng\i] /sa ?-k\i/ impale (with something long)

    [sa?ndka] /sa ?-ta-ka/ slip out (something long)

    (14b) [uuk \i] /u-k \i/ sit on chair

    [uuti?] /u-t ?/ sit down on ground[uumata] /u-mta/ sit inside something

    (14c) [moota] /mo-ta/ carry upslope

    [moomata] /mo-mta/ carry below

    [mootak\i] /mo-ta-k \i/ carry from below to an elevated

    position

    Examples of an alternation which must be triggered by lexical nasality

    appear in (15) (intransitive verb) and (16) (transitive verb), where [-ta] up,toward occurs in alternation with [-na] up, toward. These allomorphs

    occur when the suffix [-ta] is affixed to a verbal root in order to form a verb

    stem. The lexical form for [-na] appears when the stem is inflected to form

    the imperative in (15c), because the imperative morpheme /-ya/ (here with

    the allomorph [ca]; see 4.3) interposes itself between the verb root [hi] and

    the suffix [-ta]. It also appears in (16b), where the stem-forming morpheme

    [-ko] interposes itself between the verb root and the suffix [-ta]:

    (15a) [h] /hY/ to creep along

    (15b) [hnak\]

    /hY-ta-k\i/

    creep-up-elevated

    to go up a slope

    (15c) [htyatk\]

    /hY-tya-ta-k\i/creep-impsg-up-elevated

    go up!

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    13/42

    international journal of american linguistics12

    (16a) [tn] /t-ta/ shuck corn

    (16b) [tk ot]

    /t-ko-ta/shuck-stem-toward

    shuck corn!

    This alternation cannot be attributed to lexical tone, because the syllable

    preceding [-ta] in (16a) does not bear lexical tone (see also 20 and 21 be-

    low). In the case of a handful of morphemes, tone does condition alterna-

    tions between voiceless stops and nasal consonants. For example, [-nta]

    negative (conditional) occurs immediately following high tone (17a) and

    [-na] occurs elsewhere (17band 17c). Note that the consonant variant imme-diately following high tone is always a cluster composed of a nasal conso-

    nant and a voiceless stop, in obedience to the requirement that CV syllables

    bearing lexical tone must be heavy (i.e., bimoraic):

    (17a) [mk a\t hti\ htaNnt]

    /ma-uka-\a-ti hti\a hta-Ne-t/

    if.fut-2-pl-cont this.way do-cond-neg

    If you (pl) dont do it this way . . .

    (17b) [mk at be nyan kumya\ yNen]

    /maka-ti be neya-ni kumiya-\i

    since.3fut-cont where still-3sg.gen food-top

    yA-Ne-t/

    be-cond-neg

    Since there wont be food anywhere . . .

    (17c) [s-t NapNa\ kken?win ki\?]

    /isa-ti NaapNa-\i k-ke-t-?wi-na ki\o?/if.3-cont wildcat-top eat-cond-neg-inv-1sggen chicken(s)

    If the wildcat hadnt eaten my chickens . . .

    Examples (18)(21) illustrate the variation between [-na] and [-ta] up,

    toward for verb roots beginning with voiceless stops (18), voiced stops (19),

    vowel-initial roots (20), and semiconsonants (21).

    (18) [c?-mi-k \esak u-n]

    /tya?-mi-ka \eesa k VU

    -ta/why-not-2int cow milk-toward

    why dont you (sg) do the milking?

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    14/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 13

    [c?-mi-k \esak u-kwi-t]

    /tya?-mi-ka \esa k VU-wi-ta/

    why-not-2int cow milk-inv-toward

    why dont you (sg) milk the cow?

    (19) [ndn]

    /dO-ta/

    enter-toward

    to enter

    [ndcat]

    /dO-ya-ta/enter-imp-toward

    enter!

    (20) [n]

    /U-ta/

    drink-toward

    to drink (through a straw)

    [kat]/U-ka-ta/

    drink-stem-toward

    sip (through a straw)!

    (21) [wn] /wI-ta/ pour out liquid

    [wkothnin]

    /wI-ko-ta-ni-nu/

    pour-stem-out-ben-1sgpour it out for me!

    This class of verb roots contrasts with other roots for which the nasal variant

    [-na] never occurs and which have identical phonetic tone patterns, as illus-

    trated in (22a) and (22b):

    (22a) [ku-ta-k-nt]

    /k-ta-ka-de/

    go.up-up-stem-1ev

    Im getting up

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    15/42

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    16/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 15

    (23b) [u-t] to sit up /uta/

    [u-ke-w\a?y-t]

    /u-ke-wa\a?ya-ta/

    sit-pl-imppl-up

    sit up! (pl)

    (24a) [uku] drink! / U-ko/

    [una]

    /U-ta/

    drink-toward

    to drink with a straw

    [u-ku-ta]/U-ko-ta/

    drink-stem-up

    drink with (the) straw!

    [kape uNwa-?a]

    /kpe U-wa-a/

    coffee drinking-is

    its time to drink coffee

    (24b) [ndina] remove hair from goat carcass /dY-ta/

    [ndi-ko-ta]

    /dY-ko-ta/

    cut-stem-up

    take the hair off!

    [ni ndi-Nwa-?a-k\i]

    /ni dY-wa-a-k\i/

    gencutting-nom-is-elevated

    what was trimmed off

    Compare the nominalized forms in (24a) and (24b) with the corresponding

    forms of (25a) and (25b).

    (25a) [kii?\o] to work /ki?\/

    [kii?\-wa] work (noun) /ki?\wa/

    (25b) [kake?] to be named /kke?/

    [kake-wa?] name11 /kke?wa/

    11 Word-final glottal is always present for some words. For other words there is considerable

    variation between speakers as to whether the word-final glottal is present or not.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    17/42

    international journal of american linguistics16

    Further, verbs requiring the [-na] allomorph of the directional morpheme

    up, toward also require the [-Na] allomorph of the directional morpheme

    [-ka]:

    (26a) [aNa] go away / A-ka/

    [acaka]

    /A-ya-ka/

    go-2impsg-away

    go away!

    (26b) [ndiNa] cut off a piece /dY-ka/

    [ndi-ko-ka]

    /dY-ko-ka/cut-stem-away

    cut off a piece!

    Other verbs show analogous variants for the following stem-forming suffixes:

    /-ta/ ([-na]) up, toward; /-t?/ ([-ne?]) distributed; /-ka/ ([-Na]) away, out;

    /-\e/ ([-ne]) pl (subject); /-ke/ ([-Ne]) pl subject); /-wa/ ([-Nwa]) mid/nomz;

    and possibly /-\i/ ([-ni]) adjective. Verbs with roots that trigger nasal variants

    of one or more of these suffixes are listed in Appendix A; out of approximately590 verbs appearing so far in the data, 77 (13%) have occurred in morpho-

    phonemic contexts indicating that the lexical root must be nasal.

    4.2. The glottal off-glide.

    4.2.1. The intransitive causative. The same roots that require [NV] al-

    lomorphs of certain suffixes also require the insertion of a glottal off-glide

    ([h]) immediately preceding the transitivizing or transitive causative suffixes

    /Na/ and /No?/. When transitivized by the suffixes /-No?/ or /-Na/, verb roots

    requiring the [-na] form of the directional morpheme change their phonetic

    form from [(C)V(V)] to [(C)V(V)h]in other words, the root is immediatelyfollowed by a glottal off-glide. This alternation is illustrated below for the

    verb stems /A-ta/ to return (27a), /dO-ta/ to enter (27b), and /mA-ta/ to

    go by (27c).

    (27a) [

    go-stem-toward

    bring (it) back!

    (27b) enter-stem-toward

    put in, cause to enter

    ah-Na-ta]

    [n

    doh

    -No?-ta]

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    18/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 17

    (27c) ni yu?u]

    pass-stem-toward ser go.2impsg

    keep passing it on!The behavior of these verbs contrasts with the behavior of verbs for which

    the morpheme /-ta/ appears suffixed to the root, such as /kta/ to go out:

    when these latter verbs occur with /-No?/ or /-Na/ transitive stem the glottal

    off-glide does not appear. In (28) the morpheme /-ta/ occurs following the

    verb root /k/, the same allomorph of the verb root occurs preceding the

    stem-forming suffix /-Na/, and the same allomorph of the directional mor-

    pheme /-ta/ follows:

    (28) /k-ta/open-toward

    go out (through a door)

    [ka-Na-ta]

    open-stem-toward

    open the gate!

    Thus the glottal off-glide alternation before verbs analogous to /A-ta/ re-

    turn appears to be triggered by the same factor triggering the variation be-tween [-ta] and [-na]the verb root is marked for nasality in the lexicon.

    Tone cannot be triggering the glottal off-glide: note that the phonetic and

    lexical tone patterns of the lexical root and the whole word cross a street

    (29a) and pass on (29b) are the same:

    (29a) [manak]

    /mA-ta-ka/

    pass-toward-out

    cross a street(29b) [mahNat]

    /mA-Na-ta/

    pass-stem-toward

    pass (something) on

    Equally, forms such as (29b) preclude one from arguing that the off-glide is

    inserted to provide a mora in obedience to the requirement that a syllable

    containing a vowel with lexical tone be heavy. In fact, the interaction of lexi-

    cal tone with the metrical system for CV syllables, in which the onset of thefollowing syllable lengthens in order to provide an extra mora, is blocked

    precisely in this causative (see 27aand 27b). This indicates that the insertion

    [maah-Na-ta

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    19/42

    international journal of american linguistics18

    of the off-glide is triggered by the morphology; otherwise this blocking

    should not occur.12Contrasts such as (30a) and (30b) confirm the morpho-

    logical function of the off-glide: in these examples it refers to an unspecified

    causative agent.

    (30a) [n Ag\i]

    /nA-k\i/

    be vertical-elevated

    come from (higher elevation)

    [nAhg\i]

    /nA-h-k\i/

    be vertical-caus-elevatedbe set upright, set upright

    (30b) [kug\i]

    /kVV-k\i/

    be.burned-elevated

    be burned and stuck on

    [kuhg\i]

    /kVV

    -h-k\i/be.burned-caus-elevated

    be deliberately burned, and stuck on, deliberately burn and stick on

    The only viable alternative way to account for the insertion of the glottal off-

    glide, other than specifying nasality in the lexicon, is to establish verb

    classesand exactly the same arguments apply as in the case of the allo-

    morph [-na] of /-ta/.

    4.2.2. Isolated verb roots and compound words. The connection of the

    off-glide with nasality is not as exotic as first appears, when acoustic factsare taken into consideration: Blevins and Garrett (1992:14) note work by

    previous investigators indicating that phonation effects associated with [h]

    or breathy voice and vowel nasalization affect vowel spectra identically.

    This indicates that a natural acoustic and articulatory relationship exists be-

    tween [h] or breathy voice and vowel nasalization. Beddor (1993:184) fur-

    ther notes that breathy voicing mimics certain of the effects of nasalization

    on the low-frequency spectrum. The connection can be seen in the Chimila

    data, not only in the case of the intransitive causative but also when nasal

    verb roots of the form CV occur alone (31a) or in aspectual or serial verbconstructions (31b), in that the isolated verb root always has the form

    12 In CV roots the morphologically triggered glottal off-glide does provide the second mora,

    fulfilling the requirement that syllables bearing lexical tone must be heavy (27aand 27b).

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    20/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 19

    [(C)Vh]. In these cases the stricture of the off-glide is heavy. (31c) shows that

    the off-glide is not inserted due to the serial verb linker /ni/:

    (31a) [isa sah]/isa s/

    3.conj grind

    its ground/it was ground

    (31b) [isa bihni tekwa]

    /isa bY ni

    3.conj roll.up.in.ball ser

    te-kwa/position (round object)-extended surface

    it rolls up in a ball (centipede)

    (31c) [isa ana ni yu?u]

    /isa A-ta ni yu?-a/

    conj go-toward ser go-cont

    its returning (toward speaker)

    The connection between the off-glide and lexical nasality can also accountfor the puzzling but consistent difference in stricture noted in 2above. Upon

    suffixation isolated roots with heavier stricture turn out to be marked for

    nasality, and those with little or no stricture are oral. The stricture functions

    as a mora inserted to form a bimoraic foot, as stipulated by the minimal word

    constraint; light stricture disappears when the root is incorporated into a

    compound word ([witu?] be acquainted), whereas heavier stricture does

    not ([sihtu?] be black). In be acquainted the first syllable forms a defec-

    tive foot, but the bimoraic foot of the second member fulfills the minimal

    word constraint, so that one must conclude that the off-glide of be blackhas not been inserted in order to fulfill the minimal word constraint.

    The distribution of the glottal off-glide in nouns and adjectives also re-

    veals a close association with nasality: in the vast majority of cases it occurs

    in stem-internal position preceding nasal consonants, the lateral sonorant /l/,

    or voiced stops (always prenasalized). In syllable-final position its stricture

    does not vary according to the preceding vowel, in contrast to word-initial

    occurrences. In syllable-final position it tends to blend with immediately

    following nasals, so that the nasal devoices to varying degrees;13following

    13 For some speakers the syllable /NV/, when immediately preceded by the glottal off-glide,

    takes the form [Hv$]; in other words a nasalized glottal fricative replaces the nasalized consonant

    and the following vowel is heavily nasalized. See (5) in 2above.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    21/42

    international journal of american linguistics20

    bimoraic vowels, it tends to blend with immediately following nasal conso-

    nants. In words of tone pattern HML it occurs exclusively preceding nasal

    consonants or the lateral /l/:

    (32) [khmet] thin, delicate [\hNak?] brush

    [thnidy?] skunk [mbhlady?] bow (weapon)

    [shnak] night [lhlond?] hollow (adj)

    In words of tone pattern HL the glottal off-glide occurs preceding nasal con-

    sonants or voiced stops:

    (33) [khm?] a little bit [ndhN?] dough

    [mbhn?] two [ndhb\] lazy

    In words of phonetic tone pattern MHL the glottal off-glide occurs in the

    coda of the leftmost syllable preceding stops, nasals, or the liquid /l/:

    (34) [ehmt] go on an outing [k ahns] root

    [hohllond?] hollow (adj.) [gwahNt] clear! (a field)

    [kahmb\nt] piece of wood [ndihtnt?] shine

    Words in which the off-glide immediately precedes oral stops are always

    nouns and adjectives formed by compounding a nominal root with an adjec-

    tive (piece of wood), or they are compound verbs (lazy), or reduplicatedforms (shine). Only in words of three or more syllables with initial tone

    pattern MH do voiceless stops appear following the glottal off-glide, and all

    the examples in the data involve reduplication.14

    In contrast to verbs, there is less morphological evidence associating the

    glottal off-glide and nasality in nouns and adjectives. First, only seven

    monosyllabic noun roots of the form [(C)Vh] occur in the data:

    (35) [a h] hair [ga h] wing

    [boh

    ] manure [ka h

    ] tree[dah] nose [ma h] sweet potato

    [dih] tooth

    Of these, three occur with classifier suffixes:

    (36) [ NNgu\]

    /A-gw\a/

    hair-branch

    body hair

    14 See Malone (2004) for details on noun morphology, where I argue that all multisyllabic

    nouns are the result of word compounding or the addition of classifier suffixes.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    22/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 21

    (37a) [NgNNgu\]

    /gA-gw\a/

    feather-branch

    arm

    (37b) [NgaNNk\?]

    /gA-kik\a?/

    feather-bone

    wing

    (38) [k NNg\?]

    /kA-g\a?/

    wood-border.of

    arrow

    Nasal prosody has occurred in (37b), where the first syllable of the classifier

    suffix /-kik\a?/ bone has nasalized.15According to the analysis of Malone

    (2006) one must further conclude that feather bears lexical tone, because

    the onset of the following syllable lengthens, in response to the requirement

    that CV syllables in which the vowel bears tone must be heavy. In addition,

    the leftmost syllables of (36), (37a), and (38) all sport nasal consonant codas;

    according to the analysis of Malone (2006) the vowels of these syllables bearlexical tone, and the consonant has been inserted in order to provide the

    required second mora. Thus the nasal consonant represents the inserted mora

    in syllables when the lexical root containing the tone-bearing vowel is

    lexically nasal.

    Note that the behaviors described here exclude an analysis of the off-glide

    as a breathy or voiceless vowel. This would result in an unnecessary prolif-

    eration of phonemic vowel contrasts,16when an entity already demonstrated

    to exist in the lexicon (nasality) can account for the same data. Also, it ignores

    the limited distribution of the glottal off-glide, its close relationship withnasal consonants and roots independently demonstrated to be nasal, and the

    morphophonemic data of (36)(38).

    4.2.3. Other occurrences of the glottal off-glide. The following verbal

    suffixes can trigger a glottal off-glide when occurring on the right of the

    word: [-No?] transitive stem; [-Noye?] transitive causative; [-mata] inside

    15 The suffix originates from the lexical root /kik\a?/ bone. This suggests that some lexical

    roots might be unmarked for nasality. At present this is the only potentially unmarked root that

    has appeared in the data.16 Cho (1993:64) argues that the so-called voiceless vowels found in some languages have

    to be represented phonologically as aspirates; the Chimila data would appear to lend support

    to her proposal.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    23/42

    international journal of american linguistics22

    container; [-me] want; be able; [-mesa] purpose; [-na] 1.sg.poss; [-na\a]

    1.pl.poss; [-ne] pl; [-ne?] pas; [-ni] 3.sg.poss; [-ni] ben; [-nine]

    3.pl.poss; [-nu?] 1sg.obj; [-nu\a?] 1.pl.obj. The glottal off-glide is in-

    serted immediately preceding the initial consonant of the suffix. All of thesesuffixes begin with a nasal consonant, and the vowel following the nasal con-

    sonant is heavily nasalized, whether or not the off-glide precedes itin con-

    trast to the lighter nasalization on the vowels of syllables headed by nasal

    consonants in words such as [kene?] eat or [ndanu] be afraid. At first

    glance one might suppose that the off-glide is inserted in order to prevent the

    occurrence of unfooted syllables (39), and this is often the case. However, it

    occurs in environments where if it were not inserted, no unfooted syllables

    would occur (40). Its insertion can result in unfooted syllables not only in the

    stem (always footed separately before suffixing) but also when it immediate-ly precedes a disyllabic suffix (41).

    (39) [i sa ta a \ah ne]

    | | | | | | |

    (mm) (mm) (mm)

    /isa taa\a-ne$/

    conj talk-pl

    they are talking(40) [i h ne anawah ne]

    | | | | | | | | |

    (mm)(mm)m(mm)

    /inE? A-ta-wa-ne$/

    conj.pas go-toward-pat-pl

    they returned it

    (41) [maka wesa ooniwah

    nine og

    we?] | | | | | | | | | | | | | | |

    (m m) (mm)m(mm)m(mm)m(mm)

    /maka wesa oni-wa-nIne$ owe/

    conj.fut really cry-pat-their child

    they will really mourn for their son/daughter

    In 4.1we saw that suffixes exist which must be marked [-nasal] in the

    lexicon, and others which must be unmarked for nasality; one is tempted to

    conclude that the set of suffixes behaving analogous to [-ne] in (39)(41) is[+nasal]. This occurrence of the off-glide also raises the question of whether

    nasal prosody could be operating from right to left. Some evidence exists,

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    24/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 23

    for example (42a), where the voiced stop of [mbhn?] becomes a nasal con-

    sonant at the same point of articulation when prefixed; (42b), with the same

    classifier prefixed to three, is included for comparison.

    (42a) two (people) [ti-mhn?] two (birds)

    (42b) [m ahn?] three (people) [ti-mahn?] three (birds)

    Another possible example is (43), where the dental stop of the suffix /-ta/

    becomes a nasal consonant at the same point of articulation when suffixed by

    /-mta /:

    (43) [ihne ha\u p\antamata haatakanamata]

    /inE? ha\u p\-ta-mta haataka-ta-mta/ conj.pas all fall (drops)-toward-inside house-toward-inside

    The (rain) got into everything in the house.

    The evidence is, however, ambiguous. Regarding (42), numerical classifiers

    are the only prefixes occurring in the data, and whether or not nasal prosody

    has occurred, voiced stops do not occur stem-internally in core lexemes in

    intervocalic position. In (43), the suffix /-namta/ could also originate from

    /nA/ be vertical and /-mta / inside. If right-to-left nasal prosody can be

    unambiguously shown to occur, there are restrictions, depending on whetherit originates with a lexically nasal root (42a) or a suffix (39, 40, 41, and 43):

    nasal prosody originating from suffixes cannot nasalize consonants, whereas

    nasal prosody originating from roots can.

    4.3. Allomorphs of the intransitive imperative. If roots analogous to

    /A/ to go are lexically nasal, the insertion of the voiceless stops [t] and [k]

    in the imperative forms for roots containing bimoraic vowels such as /A-ta/

    to return (44), /dO-ta/ to enter (45), and /mA-ta/ to go by (46) must be

    explained: one does not expect voiceless stops to be inserted after nasal mor-phemes in a language allowing nasal prosody. These stops occur in the coda

    of the syllable containing CV verb roots (44 and 45), whereas they occur in

    the onset of the imperative morpheme for CV and CVV verb roots (46):

    (44) [a t-ya-ta]

    go-2impsg-toward

    return! (sg)

    [ak-wa?\e-ta]go-2imppl-toward

    return! (pl)

    [mbhn?]

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    25/42

    international journal of american linguistics24

    (45) [ndot-ya-ta]

    enter-2impsg-toward

    enter! (sg)[ndok-wa?\e-ta]

    enter-2imppl-toward

    enter! (pl)

    (46) [ma-tya-t]

    pass-2impsg-toward

    pass by, go on! (sg)

    [maa-kwa?\e-ta]pass-2imppl-toward

    pass by, go on! (pl)

    The root /mA-/ already constitutes a minimal foot (thus fulfilling the mini-

    mal word requirement), so that inserted consonants (especially in the following

    syllable onset) would seem superfluous.17Nevertheless, all intransitive verbs

    which can independently be shown to have nasal roots insert [t] in the

    singular and [k] in the plural imperative, no matter what their syllable struc-

    ture is.18

    In contrast, most intransitive imperatives inflect as in (47) below. Conso-

    nants other than the glottal stop do not appear between the verb root and the

    imperative morphemes in the singular and plural imperatives; note that the

    occurrence of the glottal stop is not predictable for disyllabic verbs. Further-

    17 Only four CVV verb roots with /aa/ occur so far in my data: /gw/ be positioned (some-

    thing flat, wide and floppy), /mA-/ pass by, /nA-/ be vertical, and /NA/ lie in hammock.

    /mA-/, /gw/, and /NA-/ condition nasal allophones. /nA-/ requires the plural morpheme [-\e]

    (instead of [-ne]) but otherwise behaves identically to /mA/ and /NA/. The occurrence of[na\] instead of *[nn] is the only exception I have been able to find with respect to the con-

    ditioning of nasal allomorphs reported in this paper.18 Consonants are also inserted immediately following nasal roots for the following suffixes:

    /-wi/ [-wi ~ -kwi] inv(compare 17 to 48c); /ya/ [-ya ~ -tca] cond; /-wa/ [-wa ~ -kwa] pat; and

    the transitive plural imperative /-waro/ [-waro ~ -kwaro]. Malone (2006) represents the under-

    lying form for the allomorph [-kwi] as /-kawi/. This representation fails to account for the fact

    that /-kawi/ would only be suffixed to verbs with nasal roots (i ), whereas when [-wi] does not

    appear [-ka] is suffixed to both nasal and nonnasal roots (ii ).

    (i ) [owi] s/he did it /o-wi/ [uk wi] s/he drank it / U-wi/

    (ii ) [oka] someone did it /o-ka/ [uka] someone drank it / U-ka/

    The representation /-kawi/ also obscures the analogy of the [k] insertion for /-wi/ with other

    morphemes where the presence of /-ka/ cannot be justified, as well as the analogy with [t]

    insertion: a generalization is lost.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    26/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 25

    more, in the phonetic forms for the singular and plural imperatives, the light

    stops preceding the glides /y/ and /w/ are voiced instead of voiceless. No

    verbs independently shown to have nasal roots inflect the imperative in this

    manner.

    (47a) /kta/ [kata] to go out

    /k-ya-ta / [kadyata] get out! (sg)

    /k-wa?\e-ta/ [kagwa?\eta] get out! (pl)

    (47b) /h-ta/ [hata] to come

    /h-ta-ya/ [hatadya] come! (sg)

    /hta-?wa\a-ya/ [hata ?gwa\adya] come! (pl)

    (47c) /k-ta ?/ [kita?] to sing/k-ta-?ya/ [kita ?dya] sing! (sg)

    /kta-wa\a-?ya/ [kita gwa\a?dya] sing! (pl)

    At the same time, roots showing no evidence elsewhere in the language for

    lexical nasality require the insertion of the voiceless stops in the imperative,

    as in (48) with a disyllabic root ending in a syllable bearing lexical high tone.

    In all these cases a CV syllable with lexical tone immediately precedes the

    imperative morpheme, and the CV syllable is always the rightmost syllable

    in a multisyllabic stem:

    (48) /ki ?\/ to work

    [kii?\tya] work! (sg) /ki?\-ya/

    [kii?\kwa\a?ya] work! (pl) /ki?\-wa\a-?ya/

    [kii?\gwa] work (noun) /ki?\-wa/

    CV syllables bearing high tone must form a bimoraic foot when suffixed; the

    resulting metrical structure is illustrated in (49) for the singular imperative

    of to work.

    (49) [kii?\ t.y a]| | | | |

    (mm)(mm)

    work! (sg)

    One cannot attribute the inserted consonants to tone in lexically nasal roots,

    because not all lexically nasal roots bear lexical tone (/gw-ya/ [gwaaca]

    lay an egg!). One is tempted to assume that syllables with lexically nasal

    vowels must also be heavy. Although there is evidence to support this as-

    sumption,19and it accounts for the behavior of monosyllabic nasal roots, it

    19 Stems such as /t-ta / shuck corn, /u$-ka/ drink, and /w$-ta/ pour out water vary with respect

    to the length of the suffix-initial consonant: sometimes it is single and sometimes it is bimoraic.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    27/42

    international journal of american linguistics26

    does not account for bimoraic roots such as /jY/ creep, /gw-/ be posi-

    tioned (flat, floppy object), and /mA-/ pass by. Instead, the inserted voice-

    less stop for the imperatives of roots analogous to /A/ and /mA-/ appears to

    block nasal prosody (15c, 19, 26a, 44, 45, and 46); the voiced transitionstops do not, as is clear from (24a) and (24b) above.

    The constraints for apparent blocking appear to be related to the boundary

    between derivation and inflection: nominalization and suffixation of direc-

    tional/location morphemes create stems which in turn are a modification of

    inherent meanings of roots, whereas imperatives do not affect the inherent

    meaning of the root (or, for that matter, the stem). When imperatives are

    formed from stems, the stem-forming suffixes must separate themselves

    from the root, the inflectional morpheme is suffixed to the root, and the stem-

    forming morpheme is suffixed to the inflectional morpheme. When bareroots function as stems (/gw-ya/ [gwaaca] lay an egg!), the relationship

    between inflection and derivation is clear, whereas it is not as clear for de-

    rived stems. Thus nasal prosody operates within a stem, but not outside the

    stem, and the voiceless stop occurs at the root boundary; in the case of non-

    nasal verb stems analogous to /ki?\/ the voiceless stop occurs at the stem

    boundary. Transitive imperatives such as [kwakata] pour! and [ukuta]

    drink with (the) straw! demonstrate what is to be expected if nasal prosody

    is indeed in operation: the voiceless stop in the onset of the transitive stem-

    forming morphemes /-ka/ and /-ko/ blocks nasal prosody, as is the case forother languages in which nasal prosody is known to operate (see Barnes

    1996 for an example). Here one can assume that the transitive stem-forming

    morphemes /-ko/ and /-ka/ and the singular imperative /-ya/ and the plural

    /-wa\a/ are specified as [-nasal], whereas the directional morpheme /-ka/ is

    unmarked for nasality. Thus the inserted stops signal the root boundary; they

    also indicate that the preceding root is nasal andthe immediately following

    suffix is oral, i.e., they signal a transition from nasality to orality.

    One might ask if the transition from orality to nasality is also signaled.

    Under certain conditions this seems to be the case, for morphemes of theform /-NV(CV)(?)/ at the right edge of the phonological word, as described

    in 4.2.3. However, a comparison of (50a)(50c) indicates that this is not the

    primary function of the inserted off-glide: in (50b) an alveopalatal affricate

    [c] (/ty/) appears instead of the expected glide [dy] (compare to 50c).

    Speakers tend to lengthen stem-internal nasals; they also tend to lengthen the suffix-initial

    consonant following CV rootsas if the minimal word constraint were in the process of be-

    ing reinterpreted as a minimal root constraint. Comparison with suffix-initial consonants in

    words such as /-kwa/ (cut-extended surface), whose roots are independently known to bearlexical tone, suggests that stems whose suffix-initial consonants vary in length are unmarked

    for lexical tone. Nasal vowels are not mentioned as instantiating syllable weight in the com-

    prehensive survey of Gordon (2002).

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    28/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 27

    (50a) [maka ooniwa hnu]

    /maka oni-wa-nU/

    conj.fut cry-pat-1objsg

    s/he will mourn for me

    (50b) [maka ooniwa tcu]

    /maka oni-wa-yu/

    conj.fut cry-pat-2objsg

    s/he will mourn for you

    (50c) [maka wiwidyu]

    /maka wi-wi-yu/conj.fut send-inv-2objsg

    s/he will send you

    /-wa/ pat is lexically oral, so the inserted stop in (50b) must instead signal

    the stem boundary when the stem is suffixed by an oral morpheme, much as

    the inserted stops of the intransitive imperatives indicate the root boundary

    of nasal roots followed by oral inflectional morphemes.

    5. Analysis. In this paper I have described phonetic and morphological

    behaviors pointing to the existence of lexical nasality or associated with

    nasality in Chimila. In this section I tie together loose ends of the analysis

    and demonstrate that a relationship exists between these seemingly disparate

    phenomena, using the proposal of Zoll (1996), who argues that subsegments

    such as nasality can express themselves as latent segments (inserted seg-

    ments), attach to an already present segment as a feature ([+nasal]), or float

    (remain unexpressed phonetically).

    5.1. Lexical nasality in core lexemes. The nasal allomorphs docu-mented in 4.1leave little doubt that nasality is operating in the Chimila lexi-

    con. There we saw that there must be verb roots marked [+nasal] in the

    lexicon; there must also be suffixes marked [-nasal] and [0nasal]. A curious

    gap in the inventory of suffixes (none were marked [+nasal]) was resolved,

    once it was demonstrated in 4.2that the glottal off-glide is intimately related

    to nasality. If, as proposed in 4.2.3and 4.3, its insertion and the insertion of

    the stops [t] and [k] in the intransitive imperative signal a transition between

    roots and suffixes differing with respect to their lexical specifications for

    nasality ([h] signals a transition to a [+nasal] suffix and the stops signal atransition to an oral suffix), then [+nasal] suffixes must also exist. Thus the

    insertion of these transitional segments is controlled by lexical specifications

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    29/42

    international journal of american linguistics28

    for nasality, in interaction with morpheme class (root or suffix), position in

    the phonological word, and lexical tone.

    The assumption that morphemes in the core lexicon are marked for nasal-

    ity avoids the theoretically awkward, ad hoc specification of some suffix-initial stops (for instance, the velar stops of /-ka/ stem or /-ke?/ aside) as

    blockers of nasal prosody, and others as unmarked for nasality (for in-

    stance, the velar stops of /-ka/ out, away or /-ke/ pl). Under this analysis

    the feature [+/-nasal] spreads onto root nodes dominated by the morpheme

    in question, in interaction with language-specific constraints on feature spec-

    ification. This analysis is different from that of Walker (1998) (inter alia),

    who attributes the phenomena of blocking and transparent consonants to

    opacity effects. Her analysis calls on a phonological representation that

    may never surface because it cannot be physically implemented (Walker1998:100), i.e., nasalized voiceless stops cannot be phonetically imple-

    mented. Nevertheless, in Chimila voiceless nasalized stops are phonetically

    implementedas nasal consonants at the same point of articulation.

    This (along with other data presented in this paper) indicates that lexical

    nasality in Chimila is not expressed phonetically on vowels in core lexemes

    unless they are contiguous to a nasal consonanta unique trait, judging

    by the comprehensive surveys of languages with nasal prosody in Piggott

    (1992) and Walker (1998). Lexical nasality is also unique in that it expresses

    itself as syllable-final nasal consonants in non-word-final syllables bearinghigh tone, as the glottal off-glide in roots functioning as isolated words, or

    word-finally in isolated verb and noun roots or in the first member of com-

    pound words, and it indirectly manifests its presence through consonantal

    transitions between roots and suffixes ([h], [t], and [k]). The data force the

    conclusion that lexical nasality must be expressed as a nonvocalic subseg-

    mental feature: it can only dock on consonants or, alternatively, it can only

    dock on syllable margins. It either expresses itself as a latent segment (in-

    serted nasal consonants or the glottal off-glide, depending on its position in

    the word), attaches to an already present segment as the feature [+nasal], orfloats (remains unexpressed phonetically), as is the case with /U-ka/ [uka]

    drink (compare /o-ka/ [oka] do).20

    Regarding the representation of lexical nasality, mora insertion has been

    independently documented for Chimila in Malone (2006), in response to the

    requirement that syllables bearing lexical tone be heavy; this involves the

    insertion of a timing unit in CV syllables bearing lexical tone, whether or

    not a vowel is lexically nasal. In these cases, place features are copied

    from the following syllable onset; place features are also copied when nasal

    20 Terminology is from Zoll (1996). I am grateful to an anonymous referee for referring me

    to Zolls work.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    30/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 29

    consonants are inserted. This is suggested by a variation which occurs be-

    tween nasal consonants with place features and a nasal consonant [N] in syl-

    lable-final position, as in [k\iNk\initi?~ k\iNk\initi?] stay (pl), [kantawa?

    ~ kaNtawa?] jungle, or [p\omp\oni?~ p\oNp\oni?] be face down (pl),

    where [N] is articulated in the boundary area at the rear of the alveopal-

    atal region and the front edge of the velar region; it is further back than

    the alveolaralveopalatal glide [dy] but not as far back as the velar nasal

    [N].21If a CV verb root is marked [+nasal], nasality attaches to the inserted

    (righthand) node, as in figure 1, for the leftmost syllable of /p\Op\OnI?/

    [p\omp\oni?] (only mora-bearing root nodes are shown in figure 1).

    In fact, the requirement that syllables bearing lexical tone be heavy may

    have motivated the requirement that lexical nasality can only be expressed

    as a consonantal feature. One sees this in the case of morphemes which block

    the insertion of the glottal off-glide when they immediately precede suffixes

    which otherwise would trigger it (51a, where the suffix /-wi/ immediately

    21 By implication, [N] cannot have place features, whereas other nasals (including the coro-

    nal nasal) in Chimila must have place features. This nasal, preceded by a phonetically nasal

    vowel, also appears as a cross-generational equivalent for nasal vowels in lexemes originating

    from onomatopoeia: compare [s?sa s?sa s?] stinging sensation, as pronounced by an olderspeaker, and the pronunciation of the same word by her daughter (also a fluent, competent

    speaker): [sNksa sNksa sNk]. (The velar stop is fronted and unreleased; it appears to be tran-

    sitional between the nasal consonant and the voiceless fricative.)

    Morpheme tier M

    | \

    Moraic tier m | \ m

    | \

    Root nodes \

    / | [+nasal] / |

    [-cons] / | / |

    Laryngeal node | [+cons] |

    / | |

    [tone] | |

    | |

    [pr o m pr o n i ?]

    Fig. 1

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    31/42

    international journal of american linguistics30

    precedes the suffix /-ne$/), and in the case of verbs which bear lexical tone on

    the second syllable of the stem (51b, where the stem /wiwakwa/ immedi-

    ately precedes the suffix /-ne$/). In (51b) the deletion of a morpheme triggers

    a tone shift (Malone 2006).22This in turn triggers lengthening of the follow-ing consonant, in contrast to (51c), where the glottal off-glide occurs:

    (51a) [gwah Nowin e]

    | | | | | |

    (mm)m(m m)

    /gw-NO?-w-ne$/

    kill-stem-inv-pl

    they killed it

    (51b) [wik w an e]

    | | | | |

    (m m)(mm)

    /wi-wa-kwa-ne$/

    command-mid-extended-pl

    they sold (him/her/it)

    (51c) [wik w ahne]

    | | | | |(m m)(mm)

    /wi-kwa-ne$/

    command-extended-pl

    they sent (him/her/it)

    The following morphemes block the insertion of the glottal off-glide in this

    environment: [-No?~ -ko] transitive stem; [-ne?] pas; [-ti] cont; [-ti?]

    down, away; [-wi] inv; [-ya?] fut; [-e?~ -ye?] affected pat; [-yo?]transitive stem. All these suffixes bear lexical tone and therefore trigger

    doubling of the immediately following syllable onset (including nasal con-

    sonants), in obedience to the requirement that syllables bearing lexical tone

    must be heavy.

    The proposed representation suggests that the mysterious glottal off-glide

    [h] in root-final position is best considered to be an allophone of the glottal

    fricative /h/, in spite of its phonetic differences and restricted distribution; in

    word-final position (in isolated and in compound words) it functions as do

    nasal consonants inserted in word-medial position. The inventory of seg-ments in word-final position is more severely restricted than that of stem-

    22/-wa/ mid is included in the lexical form of (51b), on the basis of the meaning. The exact

    identity of the omitted syllable is uncertain: /-ya/ dative obj is another possibility.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    32/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 31

    medial, syllable-final segments: it is limited to [?], [h], [m], and [N]. The

    word-final nasal consonants occur only in lexemes originating from loans or

    onomatopoeia that are in the process of migrating into the core lexicon, so

    that one can safely conclude that segments with place features are generallynot allowed in word-final position in core lexemes, i.e., only laryngeal con-

    sonants can occur in syllable codas in word-final position.

    The observed relationship between the glottal off-glide and nasality suggests

    that the off-glide must be a sonorant; Hall (2000) proposes this represen-

    tation for /h/ and /?/ in general, and Um (1996) argues that the representation

    of laryngeals with respect to [sonorant] depends on their behavior in the lan-

    guage in question. Obviously, the off-glide must bear the feature [+nasal],

    even though one hears no phonetic nasalization when a nasal consonant is

    not present.The representation in figure 1 accounts for the inserted glottal off-glide in

    words such as /kA/ [kah] tree, where the insertion of a nasal consonant is

    blocked in word-final position because there is no way for it to acquire place

    features. Figure 1 also accounts for the insertion of [t] and [k] in the codas

    of CV verb roots for the intransitive imperative and the glottal off-glide; the

    constraint [-nasal] [-sonorant] accounts for the voicing specification of

    the inserted stops. In the case of CV and CVV verb roots, a representation

    such as that proposed in Steriade (1994) for Mazatec consonant clusters ac-

    counts for the phonetic association with the onset of the immediately fol-lowing syllable. (52a) below gives representations for the stop /t/ and the

    glide /y/ [dy], where A0represents the closure phase of the segment and Amaxrepresents the release phase (Steriade 1994); the two phases are dominated

    by a single node.23 (52b) represents the cluster /ty/ [c]: The A0 and Amaxphases of the onset stop have merged with the respective A0 and Amaxphases of the glide, and the result is a single segment (dominated by one

    node). Place assimilation (both segments are coronal) triggers the merger.

    (52a) td

    y| | |

    A0Amax A0 Amax

    (52b) t y

    | |

    A0Amax

    The inserted stop of the singular imperative /mA-ya-ta/ [maacata] pass

    by! can be represented as in figure 2; the representation for the inserted stop

    in the plural imperative /mA-wa?\e-ta/ [ma-kwa?\-t] is analogous (see

    23 Steriade (1994:207) represents glides as only having Amax. The light stops preceding the

    Chimila labial and coronal glides suggest that they also have an A0phase. For /y/, [+coronal],

    and for /w/, [+velar] associate to the A0phase.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    33/42

    international journal of american linguistics32

    figure 3). Again, the respective A0 and Amaxphases of the stop and glidemerge. The merger occurs in the onset of an oral morpheme, so the associ-

    ation with the nasal morpheme is deleted (M represents the morpheme

    tier). The representation in figure 4 accounts for cases such as /Uka/ [uka] to

    drink; note that [+nasal] floats, because there is nowhere it can dock within

    the verb stem (root nodes not bearing moras are omitted).

    5.2. Lexical nasality in interjections and onomatopoeia. A referee has

    proposed that there could be a contrast between oral and nasal /h/ in inter-

    jections. However, in core lexemes and onomatopoeia, nasal morphemes can

    be shown independently to exist; thus there is no justification for proposingan additional nasal segmental phoneme which would only be found in nasal

    morphemes in interjections. An alternate analysis is available: one only has

    to specify that lexical nasality associates to vowels, instead of consonants,

    in interjections. There is little or no evidence for nasal prosody in interjec-

    tions and onomatopoeia (once they are suffixed they enter into the realm of

    core lexemes), so that one must also specify that vowels are marked nasal or

    unmarked for nasality. In the case of onomatopoeia one can either specify

    vowels as nasal, or whole morphemes; the latter is preferable, given the

    close relationship of onomatopoeia to the core lexicon. Thus the differencebetween noncore lexemes and core lexemes with respect to lexical nasality

    is manifested as a difference in its segmental manifestation; no other expla-

    nation is necessary.

    M M

    / | | \

    [+nasal] | | [-nasal]

    | |t y

    | |

    A Amax

    Fig. 2

    M M

    / | | \[+nasal] | | [-nasal]

    | |

    k w

    | |

    A0 Amax

    Fig. 3

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    34/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 33

    5.3. Variation in realization between voiced stops and nasals. Where

    does this variation fit in the general picture of lexical nasality in Chimila?

    Some roots which undergo this alternation, such as /d/ pull toward

    (something long) and /gA/ of /gA-t?-ka/ abandon can be independently

    shown to be nasal. Walker (1998) states that languages in which both /t/ and/d/ [are] realized as [n] under nasalization are unattested and suggests that

    this can be understood as a consequence of the highly neutralizing effect of

    such an outcome (1998:116, n. 15). The alternation may indeed be a syn-

    chronic remnant from a time when voiced stops in roots were realized as

    nasal consonants at the same point of articulation when undergoing nasal

    prosody. The synchronic occurrence of the alternation shows that the pros-

    ody is now optional morpheme-internally and occurs only in contexts where

    the voicing (i.e., phonemic contrast) of the underlying stop is recoverable.

    This is certainly the case for suffix allomorphs where nasals alternate withvoiced stops within stems. In fact, the ternary specification of nasalization at

    the morpheme level accounts for patterns of suffix variation within stems

    and allows one to determine underlying lexical forms. In addition to phono-

    logical factors (lexical tone and nasality), the specification of the root with

    regard to transitivity controls allomorphs. When suffixes such as /-ta/ [-ta ~

    -nta ~ -nda ~ -na] up, toward or /-t?/ [-te?~ -nte?~ -ne?] distributed (both

    [0nasal]) form stems with roots unspecified for transitivity, allomorphs anal-

    ogous to [-nta] occur following vowels bearing high tone (53a), and those

    analogous to [-nda] occur following oral syllables bearing nonhigh tone(53b). Those analogous to [-na] occur following nasal roots specified for

    transitivity, either transitive (53c) or intransitive (53d), and alternatives anal-

    ogous to [-ta] occur elsewhere for all roots (53a53d):

    Morpheme tier M M

    | \ |

    Moraic tier m | \ m |

    | \ | [-nasal]

    Root nodes [+nasal]

    / | / |

    [-cons] | [-cons] |

    | |

    [u -k a]

    Fig. 4

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    35/42

    international journal of american linguistics34

    (53a) /pta/ [pnt] to shed skin (snake)

    /pNata/ [pNat] pull (it) out! (blade of grass from

    sheath)

    (53b) /tyi?da/ [c? be gutted

    /tyi?Nata/ [c?Nat] to gut

    (53c) /wIta/ [wn] to pour out

    /wIta ditake/ [wk otnin] pour out (water) for me!

    (53d) /wY-ta/ [wn] be wild, untamed

    /wY-Na-ta/ [w hNat] scare, make wild

    Some nasal suffixes have allomorphs analogous to (53a) if lexical tone im-

    mediately precedes them, such as /-t/ [-nta~ -na] neg (see 17) or /-d?/[-nta?~ -nda?~ -na?] adj (see 11b; note also /-b\d?/ [-mb\inta?] piece vs.

    /-b\b\id?/ [-mb\imb\ida?] pieces). Some do not, such as /-nE?/ [-ne?] re-

    mote past. Oral suffixes, such as /-t?/ [-nti?~ -ti?~ -ndi?] down, away, up,

    toward, demonstrate variants typical of unmarked suffixes, except that they

    block nasal prosody: [-nti?] occurs following vowels bearing high tone,

    [-ndi?] within stems following a root-final glottal stop, and [-ti?] elsewhere.

    Allomorphs such as [-nta], [-nta?], [-nte?], and [-nti?] are ambiguous with

    respect to indicating nasality of the lexical root to which they are affixed.

    Verb roots occur with these allomorphs where one cannot be sure regardingthe specification of the root for nasality (54a); others occur where one can

    be reasonably sure that the root is nasal (see 54b):

    (54a) [ponte?]

    /p-t?/

    /be.lit-dispersed/

    be lit

    [poNote?]

    /p-No-t?/

    /light-stem-dispersed/

    to light

    (54b) [ginti?]

    /gY-ti?/

    /extinguished-away/

    be extinguished

    [gih

    Na?a]/gY-Na-a/

    /extinguish-stem-cont/

    calm down a crying child

    nd]

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    36/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 35

    An alternative occurs for (54b) be extinguished, in which the root is

    specified for transitivity (in this case, [-transitive]): [gti?] (/gY-t?/) be

    extinguished. In this root lexical nasality is unexpressed phonetically, be-

    cause of the interaction of the specification [-transitive] and the requirementthat CV syllables bearing lexical tone must be heavy. One cannot escape the

    conclusion that the suffix allomorphs not only signal phonological informa-

    tion concerning the root and root boundaries but also lexico-morphological

    information concerning the root itself.

    6. Conclusions. The existence of nasality in the Chimila lexicon has

    implications with respect to morphological analysis. First, it casts consider-

    able light on the verb morphology; in fact, it has led to a breakthrough in

    determining the inventory of verb suffixes and their phonetic forms, also indistinguishing between similar morphemes, for instance, /-Na/ [-Na] stem

    (root unmarked for transitivity), /-ka/ stem (inherently transitive root) and

    /-ka/ [-ka ~ -Na] out, away, or /-wa/ [-wa ~ -Nwa] mid/nomz ([0nasal]) and

    /-wa/ [-wa ~ -kwa] pat ([-nasal]). Second, the phenomena reported to be

    associated with nasality (variation in realization between voiced stops and

    nasals, morphophonemic alternations, the glottal off-glide, and insertion of

    [t] or [k]) provide dependable diagnostics for recognizing nasal roots: in all

    cases where one of these occurs with respect to a given verb root, one or

    more of the other phenomena have eventually appeared in the data.24

    The proposed analysis also has theoretical implications. In order to pro-

    vide a unified account of the behavior of nasality and nasal prosody, I have

    assumed a ternary distinction for marking lexical nasality ([+nasal], [-na-

    sal], [0nasal]); a ternary distinction is also useful in explaining some occur-

    rences of the glottal off-glide, as well as the insertion of [t] and [k] in verb

    root margins, and morphological variants of verb suffixes. The issue of ternary

    features is under debate and bears on current debates concerning opacity, lo-

    cality, feature specification, the nature of feature geometry, the structure of the

    lexicon, and interactions between phonology and morphologyall crucialin dealing with harmony systems and none of which I can explore in detail

    here.

    When one looks at other languages within the Chibchan language fam-

    ily, the presence of nasality in the Chimila lexicon comes as no surprise.

    Even though no contrast between nasalized and nonnasalized vowels ap-

    pears in two related languages spoken in northern Colombia, Ika (Landaburu

    2000, Tracy and Tracy 1973; 1976, and Frank 1990) and Tunebo (Headland

    24 At this stage of the analysis, using one or more of the other criteria for identifying nasal

    roots and stems would add at least 20 verb roots to the list in Appendix A; see (30b) and (54b)

    for examples.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    37/42

    international journal of american linguistics36

    and Headland 1976 and Headland 1997),25 in three other northern Colom-

    bian Chibchan languages nasalized and nonnasalized vowels contrast: Bar

    (Mogolln Prez 2000), Damana (Williams 1997), and Kogi (Gawthorne

    and Hensarling 1984).26Constenla (1981) reports nasal vowels in contrast tooral vowels for the following Central American Chibchan languages: Cab-

    car (also Margary Pea 1982 and Jones 1983:154), Bribri (also Schlabach

    1974), and Terraba. A contrast between oral and nasal vowels is reported by

    other researchers for the additional languages Bocot de Chiriqu (Gunn and

    Gunn 1974 and Margary Pea 1988), Guaym Movere (Kopesec and Kope-

    sec 1974 and Abarca Gonzlez 1985), Teribe (according to Quesada 2000

    and others, best considered to be the same language as Terraba) (Koontz and

    Anderson 1974 and Oakes 2001), and Rama (Holt 1986).27The analysis pro-

    posed here locates Chimila with other Chibchan languages in which nasalvowels contrast with oral vowels. At the same time the behavior of nasality

    and nasal prosody in Chimila is unarguably unique, even though it exhibits

    characteristics typical of well-known phonological systems which include

    nasality and nasal prosody. Its uniqueness is found in the constraints on the

    segmental manifestation of nasality in core lexemes, and the interaction of

    these constraints with the prosodic system, lexical tone, syllable structure,

    and the morphology.

    APPENDIX ALexically Nasal Verb Roots and Stems

    The phonetic form is as pronounced phrase-internally. The following lexical stem-

    forming morphemes occur with the verbs listed below:

    /-Na/ alongside, parallel to /-kwa/ extended area

    /-No?/ transitive stem /-o/ transitivizer

    /-ka/ away, out /-ta/ up, toward

    25 Tunebo contrasts an oral bilabial glide and a nasalized bilabial glide (Headland and Head-

    land 1976).26 For Damana, Trillos (2000) reports /u/ in contrast with /u$/ but no contrasts for other vow-

    els; she observes that /i,a,o,u/ nasalize contiguous to nasals and when they bear stress

    (2000:752). For Kogi, Ortiz Ricaurte (2000) reports no vowel contrasts but hypothesizes that

    nasality functions as a suprasegmental.27 Wheeler (1972), Levinsohn (1975), and Frank (1988) do not reconstruct this contrast for

    the proto-language giving rise to northern Colombian Chibchan languages, although Frank ob-

    serves that the proto-language most likely had nasalization as a suprasegmental feature oper-

    ating on the word level (1988:18). Another investigator suggests proto-nasalized vowels as a

    possible motivation for some of the reflexes of *min Kogi (Jackson 1990). In his reconstruction

    of Proto-Chibchan, Constenla (1981) includes nasality as a suprasegmental phoneme. Althoughthe data available to him were not nearly as reliable as that available to more recent researchers,

    he includes all the northern Colombian Chibchan languages mentioned above in his reconstruc-

    tion and proposes possible reflexes for nasalization in these languages, including Chimila.

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    38/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 37

    /-ka/ transitive stem /-t?/ distributed

    /-ke?/ aside /-t?/ down, away from

    /-kri/ elevated /-wa/ middle voice (mid)

    Lexical Root or Stem Phonetic Realization Gloss

    a ?ah go

    a-ta ?ana return

    a-ka ?aNa leave, go away

    by mbih roll into a ball

    by-ta mbina cut head of grain off stem

    by-t?-k\i mbitik\i wind string in ball

    by-t?-kwa mbitikwa throw/shoot into the

    ground

    b\a

    m

    b\ah

    beginb\a-ta mb\ana wake up

    de mdeh say, mention

    de-ta ndena get out of harms way

    d$-ta ndina to skin

    d$-ka ndiNa cut off a piece from

    do-ta ndona enter

    do ndoh swallow

    ekuna d ?Ekuna ndo stretch ones self

    d-ta ndona pull toward (something

    long)ka ga-t?-No?-ka aka ganeNoka get divorced (two people)

    ga-t?-ka Ngatika abandon

    ga-t?-o Ngato abandon

    g()-wa Nga()Nwa be lying down; be

    (something big)

    gv-ka gguNa fetch water

    gv-ta Nguna take off clothes

    g\v-ka Ng\uNa step aside

    gw Ngwaa be brooding (hen)

    Na Naa be hanging (somethinglong)

    y-t? ?ine? ascend vertically

    ha-ta-ka hanaka leak, escape (liquid)

    ha-ka haNa get away, escape (animate)

    jy hi creep

    jy-ta hina creep along

    j hoh smoke (cigarette)

    j-ta hona eat with a stick (thick

    liquid)

    ka-ka-t? kakati? grab, catch

    ka-ta kana grasp with hands

    ka-t? kane? be bright, lit up, blond

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    39/42

    international journal of american linguistics38

    kNo ha-wa koNo haNwa fetch water, arrive at

    arroyo

    kvu$-ta kuuna to milk

    kw-ta k w

    ana approachkw k wah be on, at (mass or group)

    kw k wah to pour

    kw-ta k wana pour into; to bud out

    (tree)

    kw-ta-ka k wanaka pour onto ground

    kwa-ka k waNa throw into stack

    (wNa) ly-ta (waNa) lina wash clothes

    ma-ta maana pass by

    mo moh carry hanging

    mo-ta-k\i monak\i carry crossways onna-k\i naaNg\i originate from; be vertical

    na-ta-k\i naanak\i originate from (higher

    elevation)

    n no hear

    o ?oh roast

    o-ta ?ona fry

    \-ta-ka \anaka slide toward

    \u$ \uh be unkempt, ruffled

    s sah grind

    sa- sa- pound earth firmsa-ta sana pound on

    sy-ke? sike? tie

    sy-ta-kwa sinak wa be blindfolded

    sv-Na suNa cook, boil

    t-ta tona shuck corn

    tv-wa tuNwa be alive

    tv-ta tuna sprout

    tu$-wa tuNwa see

    u$-ka(root-stem) uka drink

    u$-ta una drink through straw

    wa wah be there

    wa-ta wana be abundant

    w-ka waNa throw out

    w$-ta wina pour out water

    w$-ta-ka winaka pull out of

    wy-ta wina be wild, untamed

    ya yah exist; live

    ya-ta yana be born

    yv

    yu

    h

    look foryv-ta yuna insert horizontally

  • 8/13/2019 Nasality Malone 652753

    40/42

    nasal prosody in chimila 39

    REFERENCES

    Abarca Gonzlez, Roco. 1985. Anlisis fonolgico del guaym movere. Estudios de

    Lingstica Chibcha, Serie Anual, 4:746. San Jos: Universidad de Costa Rica.

    Barnes, Janet. 1996. Autosegments with three-way lexical contrasts in Tuyuca. IJAL 62:3158.Beddor, Patrice Speeter. 1993. The perception of nasal vowels. Phonetics and Phonology 5:

    Nasals, Nasalization, and the Velum, ed. Marie K. Huffmann and Rena A. Krakow, pp. 171

    96. New York: Academic Press.

    Blevins, Juliette, and Andrew Garrett. 1992. Ponapean nasal substitution: New evidence

    for rhinoglottophilia. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 18:221.

    Cho Yu, Young-mee. 1993. The phonology and phonetics of voiceless vowels. Proceedings

    of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 19:6475.

    Constenla, Adolfo. 1981. Comparative Chibchan phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, University

    of Pennsylvania.

    Frank, Paul. 1988. Proto-Arhuacan phonology. Ms.

    . 1990. Ika Syntax: Studies in the Languages of Colombia I. Summer Institute of Lin-

    guistics and the University of Texas at Arlington Publications in Linguistics 93. Dallas:

    Summer Institute of Linguistics and University of Texas at Arlington.

    Gawthorne, Linda, and Grace Hensarling. 1984. Fonologa del cogui. Sistemas Fonolgi-

    cos de Idiomas Colombianos 5:356. Bogot: Ministerio de Gobierno and Instituto Lings-

    tico de Verano.

    Gordon, Matthew. 2002. A phonetically driven account of syllable weight. Language 78:5180.

    Gunn, Robert D., and Mary R. Gunn. 1974. Fonologa bocot. Lenguas de Panam, vol. 1,

    Sistemas Fonolgicos, ed. Patricia Baptista, pp. 3148. Panam: Instituto Lingstico de

    Verano and Instituto Nacional de Cultura-Repblica de Panam.

    Hall, T. Alan. 2000. Phonologie: Eine Einfhrung. New York: Walter de Gruyter.

    Hayes, Bruce. 1995. A Metrical Theory of Stress. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Headland, Edna. 1997. Diccionario Bilinge Uw Cuwa (Tu