Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain [email protected]

30
Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain [email protected] Phonetics and Phonology in Iberia 2005 The stability of phonological features within and across segments

description

The stability of phonological features within and across segments. Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain [email protected] Phonetics and Phonology in Iberia 2005. INTRODUCTION. 1. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain [email protected]

Page 1: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Maria-Josep Solé

Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain

[email protected]

Phonetics and Phonology in Iberia 2005

The stability of phonological features within and across segments

Page 2: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

1

how the articulatory-acoustic stability of features may be endangered by their combination with other features within and across segments.

INTRODUCTION

WITHIN

feature co-occurrence restrictions (Ohala, 1983)

phonological universals (preferred combinations)

system gaps (disfavoured combinations)

1

Page 3: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

ACROSS SEGMENTS

sequential constraints (Kawasaki, 1982)

assimilation

sound change

2

Page 4: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

HYPOTHESIS: the stability of features may not only be affected by

concurrent features, but also by features in adjacent segments

which may coincide in time due to coarticulatory overlap.

features that do not combine well within a segment,

are not likely to combine in adjacent segments.

3

Page 5: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

• co-occurring & coarticulatory velopharyngeal opening

for nasality on the stability of segments requiring a high

pressure build-up in the oral cavity, such as fricatives.

4

Page 6: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Background: Stevens’ (1972, 1989) Quantal Theory

• gradual variation, may have a categorical acoustic-auditory result

• the range of allowable variation will define the stability of the correlation

• however, stable range will vary with co-occurring features in same segment and, we suggest, in adjacent segments.

5

Page 7: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Background: Production constraints of fricatives

• tight aerodynamic and articulatory constraints (DAC:4)(Recasens et al. 1997, Solé 2002)

Critical cross-sectional area of constriction

• Turbulence for fricatives

Sufficient pressure drop (P) across the lingual constriction

– sufficient rate of flow

– sufficient time

6

Page 8: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

• H: aerodynamic conditions for frication/trilling impaired by concurrent (whithin same segment)

or coarticulatory (across segments) nasalization.

an open velopharyngeal port for nasality would allow the air to escape through the nose

venting the Po, and reducing or eliminating the required P across the oral constriction for frication or trilling.

7

Page 9: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

1. Results on co-occurring nasality

2. Experiment on coarticulatory nasality

8

Page 10: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

CO-OCCURRING FEATURES

Frication and trilling cannot combine with [+nasal]

- no nasal fricatives- no nasal trills

‘The rarity of such segments [nasalized liquids, glides and fricatives] can be attributed to an antagonistic constraint NAS/CONT: A nasal must not be continuant' (Pulleyblank 1997: 76)

9

Page 11: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Ohala, Solé & Ying 1998

• Varied oropharyngeal pressure with catheters of varying cross-sectional areas, simulating the effects of varying degrees of velopharyngeal opening, i.e., nasalization.

• Amount of nasal leakage is determined by the impedance or resistance to exiting air.

10

Page 12: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Ohala, Solé & Ying 1998

• Venting fricatives with catheters

7-10mm2 (a higher impedance than that at VT) frication unaffected

18mm2 (similar impedance) fricatives adversely affected

> 31,7mm2 (lower impedance) extinguished frication

 

• Q: Would fricatives (or adjacent Vs) sound nasalized with the small VP openings (<18mm2) which do not impair frication?

• A: At least 36-40mm2 of velopharyngeal opening needed to obtain a stable percept of nasalization in Vs (Whalen & Beddor, 1989; Maeda, 1993)

11

Page 13: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Ohala, Solé & Ying 1998• Frication & nasalization as physical phenomena are not binary

• But there is a range within this continuum where a reliable id. of frication (or nasalization) can be obtained

 

• The two ranges do not overlap. Hence, lack of nasal fricatives.

0

20

40

60

80

100

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70

velo-pharyngeal opening in mm2

% id

fricative nasalized

12

Page 14: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

FEATURES IN CONTIGUOUS SEGMENTS

Question: Can the weakening/loss of fricatives before

nasals be explained by the same principles?

 

13

Page 15: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

DATA(1) Historical change & dialectal variation

Lat. mesnata ‘kid’ > Catalan mainadaLat. rosmarinus ‘rosemary’ > Spanish romero, Cat.romaníO.Fr. ae(s)mer, Stand.Cat. esma > Bal. Cat. ejma, English aimStand.Cat. quaresma > Bal. Cat. [ko’rm]Burmese *sna > [na]

 

(2) Phonological alternations

Lat. dec(i)mare, dec(i)mu > Cat.. deumar,'reduce',deume 'tribute'

BUT desèé 'tenth‘

IE *gras-men > Latin gramen 'fodder', English gramineous

BUT IE *gras-ter>Greek gaster,English gastric14

Page 16: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

(3) Stylistic variation

isn't [et], [t]; doesn’t [d (Gimson, 1962)

somethinglike thembusiness [bb

Spanish ovni ‘UFO’ [oni] ~ [omni]

(4) Acquisition data

snake [nnek], snow [nno] (Smith 1979)

 

 

Weakening: - independent articulators, e.g./zm/

& same articulator, e.g./zn/

- mostly voiced fricatives (less airflow due to glottal impedance).

15

Page 17: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

• Conflicting requirements of contiguous fricatives and nasals

Transitional frequency: fricative + N < fricative + C

CELEX Lemma database for English, Dutch and German.

 

 

16

Page 18: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

C1 C2

Table I. Number of combinations of sounds in the rows (C1) followed by sounds in the columns (C2) in word-medial position calculated in the CELEX ‘Lemma database’ for English, German and Dutch.

Table II. No. of combinations of sounds in the rows followed by sounds in the columns in word medial position calculated in the CELEX ‘Lemma database’ for English, German and Dutch. In parenthesis rare sequences showing no voicing agreement. Shadowed cells indicate the cases where fricative +N sequences have a significantly lower frequency of occurrence than fricative +oral sequences, as expected.

 ENGLISH

  n t d l r

s 330 2653 34 617 47

z 15 17 38 46 14

88 8 9 78 12

0 0 0 0 0

19 6 3 42 113

1 1 4 4 1

f 16 209 14 247 193

v 110* 14 13 222 28

  m b p

s 146 72 679

z 122 40 18

32 10 13

0 0 0

9 10 7

9 0 2

f 9 17 18

v 24 19 17

fricative + C sequences > fricative + N sequences

17

Page 19: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

 

DUTCH

C1 C2 C1 C2

C1 C2

  m b p

s 1521 3360 10

z 0 0 1537

11 4 9

0 0 0

x 16 107 1

191 0 259

f 123 162 0

v 0 0 322

  n t d l r

s 568 3400 0 1770 570

z 0 0 976 1 0

1 0 4 3 6

0 0 0 0 0

x 181 2799 2 131 1232

185 0 529 388 1474

f 43 797 1 898 698

v 1 0 507 459 401

fricative + C sequences > fricative + N sequences

18

Page 20: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

 

GERMAN

  n t d l r

s 55 2635 80 379 175

z 0 0 0 2 4

134 1826 3 445 235

0 0 0 0 0

x 169 1193 41 139 50

f 78 478 24 489 515

v 0 0 0 0 8

C1 C2

  m b p

s 495 370 395

z 0 0 0

114 621 30

0 0 0

x 64 20 118

f 49 47 113

v 0 0 0

fricative + C sequences > fricative + N sequences

19

Page 21: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

EXPERIMENT:CONTIGUOUS FEATURES

HYPOTHESIS : anticipatory velopharyngeal opening for the N diminishes the oropharyngeal pressure required to achieve the P for frication.

20

Method

• Oropharyngeal pressure, oral flow & nasal airflow. PCquirer.

• fricative+nasal (e.g., Fresno, Mesmer; Dessna, Missmer).

• 3 speakers x 4 tokens x 5 repetitions x 2 speaking rates.

• slow and fast speaking rates:

increased articulatory overlap and changes in the timing of oral and velic Gs on the pressure build-up for fricatives.

Page 22: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Results: anticipatory velum lowering

Fig.3. (1) Audio signal, (2) filtered oropharyngeal pressure (Po), (3) unfiltered Po, (4) oral airflow, (5) nasal airflow and 0-5KHz spectrogram of ‘Say Mesmer again’.

[ s e m e z m r g e n]

onset oral closure

onset velum lowering

Drop in A0 of friction

1

2

3

4

5

21

Page 23: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

C1

velum

C2

C2

synchronous,nasal flow at 0

bleeding Po, oral & nasal flow before 0

epenthetic stop, no nasal & no oral flow +15 ms

epenthetic V, oral & nasalflow after 0

s n

0

patterns of velic coordination

22

Page 24: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

patterns of velic coordination

= onset oral closure for N - = velum opening lead + = velum lowering lag

23

Speaker DM

-60 -45 -30 -15 0 15 30 45 60

SL

OW

FA

ST

time

Page 25: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Velum may lower during the acoustic duration of the fricative, bleeding required P across the fricative constriction.

Drop in A0 of high frequency noise when velum starts to lower.

perceptual loss of the fricative

Comparable degree of anticipatory velum lowering in fast speech

a larger percentage change due shorter duration of segments at fast rates

24

Page 26: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Prediction:

• If nasal leakage due to anticipatory velopharyngeal opening extinguishes/attenuates frication

for a few ms

fricatives preceding Ns should be phonetically shorter than preceding non-nasals.

• Fricative duration measured on spectrogram for test & control tokens.

25

Page 27: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Results: fricative duration

0

20

40

60

80

100

120

140

__n __m __D __l __d ___r

voiced

voiceless

0

20

40

6080

100

120

140

__n __m __D __l __d __r

time

in m

s.

speaker DM speaker JO

Fig.4. Duration of voiced and voiceless fricatives preceding nasal [n, m], and non-nasal [, l, d, r] consonants.

• fricatives are shorter preceding nasal vs non-nasal Cs (p<0.0001) for the 2 speakers.

• voiced fricatives are shorter than voiceless fricatives

26

Page 28: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

CONCLUSIONS

• [+nasal] does not combine with frication, due to VPO venting the oropharyngeal pressure necessary for the generation of turbulence for fricatives

within segments

across segments

• Relating constraints on the combination of features within and across segments illustrates the generality that can be achieved by a physically based explanation.

28

Page 29: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

CONCLUSIONS (…)

The unstability of frication when combined with nasalization:

feature co-occurrence restrictions (e.g., lack of nasal trills and nasal fricatives)

phonological change

morphological alternations

transitional probabilities (lower lexical frequency of fricatives followed by nasals) and constraints on the sequencing of segments.

29

Page 30: Maria-Josep Solé Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain mariajosep.sole@uab.es

Gimson, A.C. 1962. An intro. to the pronunciation of English. London: Arnold.Maeda, S. 1993. Acoustics of vowel nasalization and articulatory shifts in

French nasal vowels. In M.K. Huffman & R.A. Krakow (eds), Nasals, nasalization and the velum.San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 147-167).

Ohala, J.J.& Ohala, M. 1993. The phonetics of nasal phonology: theorems and data. In M.K. Huffman & R.A. Krakow (eds.), Nasals, nasalization and the velum. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 225-249.

Ohala, J.J. & Kawasaki, H. (1984) Phonetics and prosodic phonology, Phonology Yearbook,1, 113- 127.

Ohala, J.J., Solé, M.J. & Ying, G. (1998) The controversy of nasalized fricatives, Proceedings of the 135th Meeting of the ICA/ASA. Seattle, Washington, 2921-2922.

Solé, M.J. , Ohala, J.J. & Ying, G. (1998) Aerodynamic characteristics of trills, Proceedings of the 135th Meeting of the ICA/ASA, Seattle, Washington, 2923-2924.

Stevens, K.N.1972. The quantal nature of speech: Evidence from articuatory-acoustic data. In P.B. Denes and E.E. David Jr. (eds), Human Communication, A Unified View (51-66). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Stevens, K.N. 1989. On the quantal nature of speech. Journal of Phonetics 17. 3-46.

Recasens, D. 2002. Weakening and strengthening in Romance revisited. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 14(2), 327-373.

Smith,N.V. 1973. The acquisition of phonology: A case study.Cambridge:CUPStraka, G. 1964. Remarques sur la ‘desarticulation’ et l’amuïssement de l’s

implosive. Mélanges de lingüistique Romane et de philologie médiévale offerts à Maurice Delbouille 1: 607-628, Glemboux.