María J. Arche , Laura Domínguez & Florence Myles

28
The L2 Acquisition of the semantics and morphology of Aspect: a study of the acquisition of the Spanish imperfect-perfective contrast by native speakers of English María J. Arche, Laura Domínguez & Florence Myles

description

The L2 Acquisition of the semantics and morphology of Aspect: a study of the acquisition of the Spanish imperfect-perfective contrast by native speakers of English. María J. Arche , Laura Domínguez & Florence Myles. This talk. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of María J. Arche , Laura Domínguez & Florence Myles

Page 1: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

The L2 Acquisition of the semantics and morphology of Aspect: a study of the acquisition of the Spanish imperfect-perfective contrast by native speakers

of English

María J. Arche, Laura Domínguez & Florence Myles

Page 2: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

This talk• To investigate the L2 acquisition of Grammatical

Aspect by looking into the acquisition of the semantics of the Spanish imperfect by native speakers of English.

• To evaluate whether the predictions of the Aspect Hypothesis (Andersen 1991, Andersen & Shirai 1996) can be extended to the acquisition of meaning.

• To examine the path of emergence of the semantics of the imperfect.

• To present new Spanish L2 cross-sectional comprehension data.

Page 3: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Grammatical Aspect

• Semantic category represented in the Syntax.

• Conveys information about –whether eventualities are in progress,

finished or about to start.– the number of occasions an

eventuality takes place.

Page 4: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Grammatical Aspect

• Comrie 1976, Smith 1991, Verkuyl 1993, Demirdache & Uribe-Etxebarria 2000, Arche 2006.

Meaning Number of occasions

Status Examples

Perfective 1 Finished John walked in the park; John was sick the whole

2002.Progressive 1 Unfinished John was walking

in the park

Habitual>1 Period unfinished;

Each instance, finished

John used to walk in the park

Continuous Unfinished John was sick when I visited him

Page 5: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Syntax of Aspect (Arche 2006)

Ordering Predicate

QuantifierOccasions

AspP AspP

AspP AspP

Asp’ Asp’

Asp’ Asp’

Asp

Asp Asp

Asp

TT TT

TT TT

within within

within after

Q<occ>

Q<occ>

|1| >1

|1| ∃

VP

e

Progressive Habitual

PerfectiveContinuous

Q<occ>

Q<occ>

Page 6: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Grammatical Aspect and Inner Aspect• Inner Aspect/Situation Aspect: internal temporal

structure of eventualities (e.g. duration, culmination, or delimitation).– Vendler 1967, Verkuyl 1993, Smith 1991, a.o.

Grammatical Aspect

States Activities Accomplishments Achievements

Non dynamic DurativeNo endpoint

Dynamic DurativeNo endpoint

Dynamic DurativeEndpoint

Non-dynamic Non-durativeEndpoint

Perfective ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓Progressive ✗ ✓ ✓

Habitual ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓Continuous ✓ ✗ ✗ ✗

Page 7: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Aspect Hypothesis(Andersen 1991; Andersen &Shirai, 1996; Bardovi-Harlig, 2002; Sugaya&Shirai, 2007):

1. Learners first use (perfective) past marking with achievement and accomplishment verbs, eventually extending use to activity and state verbs.

2. In languages that encode the perfective-imperfective distinction morphologically, imperfective past appears later than perfective past, and imperfective past marking begins with states and activities (i.e., atelic verbs), then extends to accomplishments and achievements (i.e., telic verbs.

3. In languages that have progressive aspect, use of progressive marking begins with activity verbs and then extends to accomplishment and achievement verbs.

4. Learners do not incorrectly attach progressive marking to stative verbs.

Page 8: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Research Questions

1. Which of the three meanings associated with the Spanish imperfect is acquired first?

2. Can the acquisition of the meanings of the Spanish imperfect be explained by the LAH?

3. What is the acquisition task regarding Spanish aspect for native speakers of English?

Page 9: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Grammatical Aspect

• Understood as an ordering predicate (Demirdache & Uribe-Etxebarría 1999 - 2007).

• Tense orders the Topic Time with respect to the Speech Time (before, after, within).

• Aspect orders Topic Time with respect to the Event time.

• Aspect is syntactically located at a lower position than Tense.

Page 10: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Aspect in English and Spanish (Summary)

Meaning English morphology Spanish morphology

Perfective (events & states) Past preterit

Continuous (states) Past Imperfect

Habitual (events & states) Periphrasis(used to/would)

Imperfect or periphrasis(soler + INF)

Progressive (events) Periphrasis(copula + V-ing)

Imperfect or periphrasis(copula + V-ndo)

• Both languages have the same syntax and semantics for Aspect.

• Spanish & English temporal inflection includes tense and aspect information.

Page 11: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

The learning task• We assume all the relevant semantic features

can be transferred from L1.• The learning task is defined as the mapping of

the semantic features onto L2 morphology.– In learners’ L1 there is a one to one form to

meaning correspondence for imperfect meanings, while in the L2 one single form can correspond to three different meanings.

– L1 has ambiguous forms (past forms with states).• John was ill when I visited him (imperfect)• John was ill the whole winter of 2001 (perfective)

Page 12: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Predictions

1. If semantics to morphology mapping is the issue, states will be the most difficult case where the correct meaning to form mapping can be established.

2. If emergence and development of imperfect is guided by lexical aspect properties of the verb (at least in the path suggested by AH), imperfect should be acquired with states early on.

Page 13: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

The experiment

Page 14: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Participants

Page 15: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Comprehension task

CONTEXT TYPE OF PREDICATE TARGET FORM

Habitual Eventive Imperfect

Habitual Stative Imperfect

One-off event Eventive Preterit

One-off event Stative Preterit

Continuous Stative Imperfect

Progressive Eventive (non-achievements) Imperfect

Progressive Eventive (achievements)coercion Imperfect

• Sentence-context matching task• 32 sentences

Page 16: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Comprehension Task

Learners were given the prompt

in English

Test measures both acceptance of the correct form and rejection of the

incorrect one

Five-point Likert scale

Page 17: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Overall Results

Y10 Y13 UG NS

-0.20

-

0.20

0.40

0.60

0.80

1.00

1.20

1.40

1.60

-0.03

1.06

1.27 1.28

-0.16

0.42

0.73

1.58

-0.11

0.62

1.03

1.55

Mean Averages of Correct Responses(Acceptance of imperfect and Rejection of Preterit)

Habitual Continuous Progressive

Page 18: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Results

1. Beginners do not show acquisition of imperfect.2. Rates of correct acceptance of the imperfect

and correct rejection of the preterit were significantly lower in continuous contexts for the intermediate and advanced groups.

2. No statistical differences in acceptance of the imperfect and rejection of the preterit were found according to type of predicates (stative or eventive).

Page 19: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Continuous

• Intermediate learners had lower scores than advanced learners but not significantly lower.

• There is no significant difference between the mean in the continuous and the mean in the progressive (eventive) for any of the learner groups.

Page 20: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Habitual• According to the

paired t-test, there is no significant difference between the mean in the habitual eventive and the mean in the habitual stative tasks for any of the learner groups.

• This means that none of the groups’ results were influenced by the aspectual properties of the verbs. (Being eventive or stative did not affect the participants’ choices)

Page 21: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Discussion and conclusions

1. Acquisition looks gradual and attainable (advanced group behaved mostly native-like in some scenarios).

2. Beginners don't seem to distinguish between the meanings.

3. Intermediate and advanced learners do distinguish between the meanings of the imperfect and are better with some of them than with others. Not all of the meanings of the imperfect are equally problematic.

– The habitual meaning seems to be the earliest and best acquired.

Page 22: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

4. Continuous meaning (available only with states) is the one where learners perform the worst.

5. Difficulty cannot be explained by lexical aspect properties of the predicate, but by the need of establishing a new semantic-morphology mapping with no morphological equivalent in L1.

6. Event type does not have an impact on the correct acceptance of imperfect and rejection of the preterit.

• The semantics of the imperfect is not first acquired with states. Lexical properties do not seem to be at the root of learner’s choices.

Page 23: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

The SPLLOC 2 project is supported by ESRC research grant (RES-062-23-1075)

We would like to thank all the participants in the project, including subjects, transcribers and fieldworkers

www.splloc.soton.ac.uk

Acknowledgments

Page 24: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Spare slides

Page 25: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Semantics of Tense & Aspect• Interval-based semantics

– Temporal categories ORDER one interval with respect to another (Reichenbach 1947, Lemmon 1967, Zagona 1990, Stowell 1993).

• Relevant intervals:– Speech time: the reference time with respect to which

every event is ultimately understood. – Event time: the interval in which the event takes place – Topic Time: the interval that is referred to in the sentence

(Partee 1973, Stowell 1993, Klein 1994).

• Maria was cuddling the baby {when John entered the room} TT.

Page 26: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Syntax of Tense & Aspect(1) TP

RT T′ T AspP TT Asp′ Asp VP ET VP

e VP

Before/after/within

Before/after/within

• Before: future• After: past• Within: present

• Before: prospective• After: perfective• Within: imperfect

(progressive/habitual/continuous)

Stowell 1993 - 2007; Demirdache&Uribe-Etxebarría 1998 - 2007.

Page 27: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

• The quantifier over occasions (Verkuyl 1993) enables us to account for the differences found within the imperfect. Progressive, habitual and continuous share the ordering property (within) but differ in the amount of instances of the event they refer to (one, more than one or, possibly none, ).

Page 28: María  J.  Arche , Laura  Domínguez  & Florence Myles

Illustrative examples (2) In his twenties, John used to go by bus to uni.------[x------x--------x-------x---------x------UTT(3) When I called him, John was going to uni---------[-----x------------------UTT(6) In his twenties, John was grey-haired.

------------[---------------------UTT(7) John was sick the whole of 2002------------[--------]---------UTT

Red font text: Topic Time