Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

22
This article was downloaded by: [Virginia Commonwealth University Libraries] On: 03 March 2014, At: 12:38 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The Journal of Social Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vsoc20 The Power of Change: Interpersonal Attraction as a Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude Alignment Chelsea A. Reid a , Jody L. Davis a & Jeffrey D. Green a a Virginia Commonwealth University Accepted author version posted online: 26 Jul 2013.Published online: 26 Sep 2013. To cite this article: Chelsea A. Reid , Jody L. Davis & Jeffrey D. Green (2013) The Power of Change: Interpersonal Attraction as a Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude Alignment, The Journal of Social Psychology, 153:6, 700-719, DOI: 10.1080/00224545.2013.824404 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2013.824404 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,

Transcript of Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Page 1: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

This article was downloaded by: [Virginia Commonwealth UniversityLibraries]On: 03 March 2014, At: 12:38Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

The Journal of SocialPsychologyPublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vsoc20

The Power of Change:Interpersonal Attraction as aFunction of Attitude Similarityand Attitude AlignmentChelsea A. Reid a , Jody L. Davis a & Jeffrey D.Green aa Virginia Commonwealth UniversityAccepted author version posted online: 26 Jul2013.Published online: 26 Sep 2013.

To cite this article: Chelsea A. Reid , Jody L. Davis & Jeffrey D. Green (2013) ThePower of Change: Interpersonal Attraction as a Function of Attitude Similarityand Attitude Alignment, The Journal of Social Psychology, 153:6, 700-719, DOI:10.1080/00224545.2013.824404

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2013.824404

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all theinformation (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform.However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness,or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and viewsexpressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of theContent should not be relied upon and should be independently verified withprimary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for anylosses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,

Page 2: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of theContent.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 3: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

The Journal of Social Psychology, 2013, 153(6), 700–719

Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

The Power of Change: InterpersonalAttraction as a Function of AttitudeSimilarity and Attitude Alignment

CHELSEA A. REIDJODY L. DAVIS

JEFFREY D. GREENVirginia Commonwealth University

ABSTRACT. Does attitude alignment predict attraction? Would you like a stranger morewho shifts her/his attitudes to more closely align with yours? In pairs, participants(N = 77) discussed social issues about which they disagreed and received false feedbackon whether the partner engaged in attitude alignment (shifted her/his attitudes toward theparticipant’s attitude) following discussion. Participants also received false feedback aboutthe proportion of similarity to the partner on a set of issues (i.e., 25%, 50%, or 75%).Participants reported greater attraction to partners who engaged in attitude alignment andwho were more similar. Moreover, similarity and attitude alignment interacted. Similaritypredicted attraction when attitude alignment did not occur, but did not predict attractionwhen attitude alignment did occur. Finally, partner attitude alignment led to participant atti-tude alignment, and perceived reasoning ability mediated the attitude alignment-attractionrelationship.

Keywords: attitude alignment, attitude similarity, attraction

IN THE HEAT OF AN ELECTION SEASON, new neighbors Riley Republicanand Dana Democrat (not their real names) have a discussion about topics such aslegalizing gay marriage. Though they held the expected opposing positions priorto their discussion, Dana was successful in persuading Riley to move closer toher own position in favor of gay marriage. However, the two still held divergentpositions on a host of other issues including energy policy, affirmative action, edu-cation, budget policy, and more. Dana had a similar discussion with Lee Liberal,

The authors thank Rebecca Lester, Richard Xiao, Farren Larson, Charlie Duda, JessicaFriesen, and Jesse Parsons for their assistance in data collection.

Address correspondence to Chelsea A. Reid, Virginia Commonwealth University,Department of Psychology, 806 West Franklin St., Richmond, VA 23284-4000, USA;[email protected] (e-mail).

700

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 4: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 701

another new acquaintance. They held similar positions on most issues, but noton gay marriage. In contrast to the discussion with Riley, Dana was unable topersuade Lee to modify her position. According to the venerable similarity andattraction literature, Dana would probably like Lee, with whom she shares ahigh proportion of similar attitudes, though Lee did not move closer to Dana’sposition on gay marriage. Similarly, according to this literature, Dana would prob-ably not like Riley, with whom she shares a low proportion of similar attitudes,though Riley moved closer to Dana’s position on gay marriage. But might attitudealignment also exert a significant influence on attraction? Put another way, mightattitude alignment on one issue influence attraction as much as the total proportionof similar attitudes across a host of issues? If so, Dana might like Riley as muchas, or even more, than Lee.

A robust phenomenon in the psychological literature is the link between atti-tude similarity and attraction (see Byrne, 1997), but what role does attitude changeplay in attraction? When confronted by disagreements, individuals often engagein attitude alignment, shifting their attitudes to more closely match those of thepartner (Davis & Rusbult, 2001). This process has been characterized as a pro-relationship response to disagreement-induced relational tension and is associatedwith healthy couple functioning. Though previous research has investigated therole of attitude alignment in ongoing close relationships, this research is the firstto examine the influence of attitude alignment on initial attraction. The impor-tance of attitude alignment in predicting attraction may even rival that of attitudesimilarity, so we examine their joint influence on attraction.

Similarity and Attraction

A well-established link exists between similarity and attraction, with similar-ity predicting greater attraction whether or not the target individual has desirablepersonality characteristics (Tenney, Turkheimer, & Oltmanns, 2009). Both friendsand romantic partners tend to be similar on a variety of characteristics includ-ing demographics, level of physical attractiveness, and personality (Etcheverry& Agnew, 2009; Kandel, 1978; McCrae et al., 2008; Till & Freedman, 1978).Attraction also is linked to similarity in values, personal interests, and politicalorientations (Luo, 2009). Both actual and perceived similarity influence attrac-tion, though perceived similarity exerts a stronger effect than actual similaritywhen individuals actually interact (Montoya, Horton, & Kirchner, 2008).

Work in this area has focused particularly on the influence of attitude similar-ity and attraction (Byrne, 1961a, 1961b; Byrne & Blaylock, 1963; Byrne, Bond,& Diamond, 1969; Byrne & Nelson, 1965; Kandel, 1978; McGarva & Warner,2003; McGinley, Nicholas, & McGinley, 1978). The relationship between attitudesimilarity and attraction is linear; a greater proportion of similar attitudes betweentwo strangers results in a greater degree of attraction between them (Byrne &Nelson, 1965), due in part to gains in consensual validation (Byrne & Blaylock,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 5: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

702 The Journal of Social Psychology

1963). Individuals tend to rate strangers with similar attitudes as more attractive,intelligent, and moral than those holding dissimilar attitudes. In addition, they arelikely to believe that such strangers have greater knowledge of current events andare better adjusted (Byrne, 1961a, 1961b).

The similarity-attraction link is mediated by several factors, includinginferred attraction and cognitive evaluation (Singh, Ho, Tan, & Bell, 2007; Singh,Ng, Ong, & Lin, 2008; Singh, Yeo, Lin, & Tan, 2007). Individuals are attractedto others who they expect will agree with them and in turn be attracted to them(i.e., inferred attraction; Condo & Crano, 1988), and they also are attracted tothose whom they view as having higher worth as a person (i.e., cognitive evalua-tion; Montoya & Horton, 2004). A recent meta-analysis of 240 laboratory studies(Montoya & Horton, 2012) investigated additional mediators. The meta-analysisfound some support for Byrne’s reinforcement approach: Individuals holding sim-ilar attitudes validate our views and make us feel good, and these positive feelingsbecome associated with the individuals. Greater support was found for an informa-tion processing approach: Individuals make inferences about the positive qualitiesof others based on similarity. Overall, the similarity-attraction link is robust and isexplained by both cognitive and affective factors.

Attitude Alignment and Attraction

Individuals select close others based on pre-existing similarity, particularlysimilarity in attitudes. We hypothesize that attitude alignment also will increaseinitial attraction, as individuals seek to establish balance during relationship for-mation. Balance theory presents a useful framework for understanding the roleof attitude change in attraction by examining the relations between an individual,another individual, and an object or issue. A balanced state occurs when triadicrelations among an individual (p), another person (o), and an attitude object (x)are harmonious. Balance occurs when all three relations among p, o, and x arepositive, or when one relation is positive and two are negative. When the triad isimbalanced, tension exists (Heider, 1958); tension has been hypothesized to begreater when the attitude object x is considered important and when x is jointlyrelevant to p and o (Newcomb, 1953, 1959). Shifts toward balance will then likelyoccur (Heider, 1958). Specifically, individuals’ attitude about the issue (x), theirperception of the partner’s attitude, or their attraction toward the partner couldchange to achieve balance (Taylor, 1967).

Attitude alignment is the tendency of individuals to shift their attitudes tomore closely match the attitudes of another. Individuals engage in the great-est amount of attitude alignment when topics of disagreement are salient andare peripheral (i.e., not important) to individuals’ self-concept, but central (i.e.,important) to the other’s self-concept. Individuals are less likely to engage inattitude alignment when issues are central (vs. peripheral) to their self-concept.Though attitude alignment typically is greater for couples, attitude alignment also

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 6: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 703

has occurred between strangers (Davis & Rusbult, 2001). Why do individualsstrive toward similarity? Consistent with balance theory, past research proposesthat knowledge of attitude dissimilarity may cause discomfort (Davis & Rusbult,2001), conflict (Kalmijn, 2005), and negative emotions (Orive, 1988). Shiftingattitudes to more closely match the attitudes of a partner may relieve some of thediscomfort caused by a disagreement.

Attitude alignment may provide individuals with an opportunity to increasean interaction partner’s liking and acceptance of them. Individuals have a strongdesire for social attachments; they readily seek out relationships and fear rejectionand exclusion (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). From an evolutionary perspective,group inclusion was beneficial whereas exclusion was costly. Engagement inattitude alignment with another individual may serve an adaptive function thatpromotes social cohesion and acceptance. Thus, when an individual is alerted thatan interaction partner shifted her or his attitudes to align with the individual’sattitudes, it also may serve an affiliative cue and produce attraction for the part-ner. In support of this notion, individuals expect agreement to produce liking anddisagreement to produce disliking (Insko, Sedlak, & Lipsitz, 1982).

Why might a shift toward agreement serve an afffiliative cue and produceattraction? Individuals like others who like them (Backman & Secord, 1959).When individuals received information about the proportion of a stranger’s sim-ilarity to them as well as the stranger’s judgment of the individual, positiveevaluations of the individual had greater impact on attraction than similarity(Byrne & Rhamey, 1965). The impact of positive evaluations in comparisonto attitude similarity for predicting attraction has been bolstered by additionalresearch (Bell & Baron, 1974; Byrne & Ervin, 1969; Bryne & Griffitt, 1966; Inskoet al., 1973; Singh, 1975; Singh et al., 2007). For instance, in a recent examina-tion of the relationship between attitude similarity and attraction, the previouslyestablished positive relationship between attitude similarity and attraction wasreplicated, but larger effects occurred for reciprocity of liking (Lehr & Geher,2006). Though receiving information that a stranger has engaged in attitude align-ment is not a direct judgment of individuals, attitude alignment likely still containsa more evaluative component than general attitude similarity. When a strangerwho was originally in disagreement shifts toward agreement, the shift likelyimplies a positive reaction to the individual and/or the arguments the individualput forth.

Overview and Hypotheses

The role of attitude similarity in predicting attitude change is well docu-mented in the literature. However, relatively little is known regarding the role ofattitude change. Thus, we examined the independent and interactive effects of bothattitude similarity and attitude alignment on attraction to an interaction partner.We predicted that participants would be more attracted to partners who were more

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 7: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

704 The Journal of Social Psychology

similar (i.e., had a higher proportion of similar attitudes; Hypothesis 1 [H1]) andwho engaged in attitude alignment (vs. did not engage in attitude alignment; H2).We also expected an interaction between attitude similarity and attitude alignment(H3). Specifically, we expected the simple effect for similarity to be significant inthe no attitude alignment condition, but not in the attitude alignment condition.However, we expected the simple effect for attitude alignment to be significantacross all similarity conditions. In other words, we expected attitude similarityonly to affect attraction when partners had not engaged in attitude alignment, butwe expected attitude alignment to affect attraction across all levels of similarity.In a more exploratory vein, we also examined whether perceptions of the align-ing partner as having greater reasoning ability, a form of cognitive evaluation,mediates the relationship between attitude alignment and attraction. In addition toassessing participants’ degree of attraction to the partner, we also measured indi-viduals’ amount of attitude alignment toward the partner’s attitude and advanceda parallel set of hypotheses. We expected participants to engage in greater attitudealignment with partners who were more similar (i.e., had a higher proportion ofsimilar attitudes; H4) and who engaged in attitude alignment (vs. did not engagein attitude alignment; H5).

Method

Participants

Undergraduate students (N = 92; 60 women, 31 men, 1 did not report)from introductory psychology courses at a large, public, mid-Atlantic universityparticipated for partial course credit. Data from fifteen students that did not fitnecessary criteria were removed (see Data Analysis), leaving a final sample of77 participants (52 women, 25 men) with a mean age of 20.12 years old (SD =3.54). Participants’ ethnicity varied (White/Caucasian = 38.96%, Black/African-American = 24.68%, Asian = 27.27%, Hispanic/Latino = 1.30%, and Other =7.79%).

Procedure

Participants arrived for a study described as examining personality and inter-personal processes. They participated in randomly assigned pairs with a partnerwho had been a stranger to them prior to the study. They first completed an atti-tudes questionnaire in which they reported their position on a variety of issues andthe centrality of each issue to their self-concept (Davis & Rusbult, 2001). Thenthey completed individual difference measures and demographic information.

While participants completed the latter questionnaires, the experimenterselected for discussion two issues about which partners disagreed. Partners wereconsidered to be in disagreement on an issue if their scores differed by six or more

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 8: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 705

dashes along a 20-point scale, consistent with past research (Davis & Rusbult,2001). Participants discussed two target issues with the partner—one issue thatwas central to the self but peripheral to the partner, and one issue that wasperipheral to the self but central to the partner. An issue was considered to becentral to participants’ self-concepts for ratings of 5 and higher, and an issuewas considered peripheral to participants’ self-concepts for ratings of 4 and loweron a 0–8 scale (Davis & Rusbult, 2001). Only issues that were central to self -peripheral to partner and peripheral to self - central to partner were selected.1 Theexperimenter instructed participants to refrain from including in discussion anyinformation regarding their responses on other items or questionnaires. Beforeexiting the room, the experimenter instructed participants to “tell each other howyou feel about each issue and why you feel the way that you do.” Participants thenspent two minutes discussing each of the two selected issues.

Following the discussion, participants moved to separate rooms and com-pleted the attitudes questionnaire again. The experimenter then delivered falsefeedback individually to participants regarding the partner’s overall similarity onthe attitudes questionnaire and whether or not the partner engaged in attitudealignment following the discussion. Order of similarity and alignment feed-back delivery was counterbalanced across participants. Consistent with previousresearch on similarity and attraction (e.g., Clore & Baldridge, 1968), participantswere randomly assigned to be told that the partner agreed with them on 25%, 50%,or 75% of the attitudes expressed via the questionnaire. Participants were told:

I have some information about how similar your partner’s attitudes were to your atti-tudes overall. You may have noticed that you answered many items about social issues,but only discussed two issues. I have determined how many issues you agreed on anddisagreed on overall on the second attitudes questionnaire that you completed. In otherwords, I’ve determined the percent of the total questions that you and your partnergenerally agreed upon. It looks like you and your interaction partner agreed on about25% [50%, 75%] of the issues.

Participants also received feedback regarding the partner’s attitude shift onthe issue that was central to them but peripheral to the partner.2 Participants heardthat the partner either did or did not engage in attitude alignment (i.e., shifted theirattitude to more closely match the attitude of the participant). Participants in theattitude alignment condition were told:

First, I calculated shift in attitude following your discussion and found that your part-ner shifted their post-discussion attitudes from their pre-discussion questionnaire inthe direction of your attitude. This means that they changed their attitude to moreclosely match yours after discussing the issue with you. Your partner shifted theirattitudes toward yours by more than 5 dashes on the 20-dashed scale. This is a verysignificant shift toward your position.

Participants in the no attitude alignment condition were told:

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 9: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

706 The Journal of Social Psychology

First, I calculated the shift in attitude following your discussion and found that yourpartner did not shift their post-discussion attitude from their pre-discussion question-naire. This means that their attitude did not change to more closely match yours afterdiscussing the issue with you, but they did not shift to disagree with you more either.

Participants next responded to a questionnaire on their current state (i.e., stateself-esteem and affect), their perception of the partner’s reasoning ability, andtheir attraction to the partner. They also completed several manipulation checkquestions (e.g., “For the issue on which you received feedback, how much didyour interaction partner shift her or his attitude?,” on a scale of 1 [shift away frommy attitude] to 7 [shift closer to my attitude]; “During the experiment, we told youabout the percentage of similar attitudes you shared with your interaction partner.Approximately what percentage [from 0% to 100%] of your interaction partner’sattitudes was similar to your attitudes?”). Finally, participants were fully debriefedabout the study’s purpose and the false feedback.

Measures

Attitudes questionnaire. Based on previous attitude alignment research (Davis& Rusbult, 2001), participants completed an attitudes questionnaire containing51 morality-relevant issues (e.g., “The restaurant which serves my favorite ethnicdishes is fined for exploiting immigrant labor. I would continue to eat there”; “Inorder to marry someone I love, I must change my religion. I would change my reli-gion”). Participants indicated their attitudes about each issue by placing a checkmark along 20 dashed lines ranging from “disapprove” to “approve.” In addition,participants provided information regarding the centrality of each issue to theirself-concept (i.e., “Ask yourself how important the issue is, indicating the degreeto which the issue is central to who you are and how you think about yourself,” ona scale from 0 [very unimportant] to 8 [very important]). An issue was consideredcentral to a participant’s self-concept for ratings of 5 and higher, and an issue wasconsidered peripheral to a participant’s self-concept for ratings of 4 and lower.

Attraction. Attraction was determined using the Affective Attraction Assessment(Montoya & Insko, 2008). The Affecting Attraction Assessment is a 5-item mea-sure of affective attraction (e.g., “How unpleasant/pleasant do you feel about yourpartner?”; “How distant/close do you feel to your partner?”; Cronbach’s α = .90).Participants provided their responses on a 9-point scale, ranging from 1 (extremelyunpleasant) to 9 (extremely pleasant) and 1 (extremely distant) to 9 (extremelyclose).

Participant attitude alignment. Participant attitude alignment was assessed witha single created items. The item measured of the extent to which participantsthought that they had engaged in attitude alignment with their partners

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 10: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 707

post-discussion (i.e., “After discussion of the issue with your partner, how muchdid you shift your attitude?”) Participants provided their responses on a 7-pointscale, ranging from 1 (shift away from partner’s attitude) to 7 (shift closer topartner’s attitude).

Perceived reasoning ability. Perceived reasoning ability was assessing using twocreated items. These items measured the extent which participants thought theirpartners had the ability to use reason in decision making (i.e., “My partner isintelligent,” “My interaction partner has the ability to clearly reason when formingjudgments”; α = .87). Participants provided their responses on a 7-point scale,ranging from 1 (disagree) to 7 (agree).

Self-esteem and affect. State self-esteem was assessed using the state self-esteemscale (Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995). The state self-esteem scaleincludes 15 adjectives (e.g., good, adequate, attractive) rated according to howparticipants felt at that moment (α = .86). Participants provided their responseson an 11-point scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 11 (extremely). Affect wasassessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark,& Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS is a 20-item measure of positive (α = .89) andnegative affect (α = .79). Participants provided their responses on a 5-point scale,ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). These measures wereincluded to be ruled out as additional possible mediators for the effect of attitudealignment on attraction.

Data Analysis

SAS proc mixed was used to examine the effects of attitude similarity (i.e.,25%, 50%, or 75% similar), attitude alignment (i.e., did or did not engage in atti-tude alignment), and the interaction between similarity and attitude alignment onattraction while accounting for the nonindependence of the dyadic data due tointeraction pairing (Campbell & Kashy, 2002). Fifteen participants were removedfrom analyses because they either claimed to know their interaction partner priorto the experimental session or because the experimenter was unable to identifyissues for the pair that met the disagreement and centrality selection criteria. Datafrom the remaining 77 participants were included in all analyses.

Results

Initial Analyses

We first examined the accuracy with which participants recalled the part-ners’ overall attitude similarity. Of the 77 participants, 91% accurately recalled

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 11: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

708 The Journal of Social Psychology

their similarity condition when asked at the end of the study.3 Next, we examinedparticipants’ perceptions of the partners’ degree of attitude alignment based upontheir attitude alignment condition. As expected, being told that the partner engagedin attitude alignment significantly affected participants’ perceptions of the part-ners’ attitude shift, F(1, 22.2) = 66.15, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 2.05. Participantswho were told that the partner engaged in attitude alignment (M = 5.88, SD =1.03) reported greater perceptions that the partner shifted attitudes to more closelymatch their own than participants who were told that the partner did not engage inattitude alignment (M = 3.91, SD = 0.89; 4.00 is the scale mid-point indicatingperception of no shift in either away or toward own attitudes).

Using the aforementioned data analysis plan, we conducted analyses to testwhether the similarity and attitude alignment manipulations, and their interaction,significantly affected self-esteem and affect, and we examined the effect of sexon attraction. There were no significant main effects or interactions of similar-ity or attitude alignment on positive affect, negative affect, or self-esteem, withone exception (attitude alignment and similarity interacted to affect self-esteem,but pairwise comparisons revealed no significant differences). Additionally, therewere no significant effects involving feedback order or sex (though there was aneffect of sex-pairing on attraction).4

Impact of Attitude Similarity and Partner Attitude Alignment on Attraction

We next examined the effects of attitude similarity, partner attitude alignment,and the interaction between attitude similarity and partner attitude alignment onattraction.5 Consistent with H1, attitude similarity significantly affected attraction,F(2, 75) = 3.17, p = .048. Participants reported greater attraction to partners with75% similar attitudes (M = 6.51, SD = 0.96) than 25% similar attitudes (M =5.97, SD = 1.70), t(75) = –2.44, p = .04, d = 0.39. Participants did not report sig-nificantly different levels of attraction to partners with 50% similar attitudes (M =6.48, SD = 0.85) than 25% or 75% similar attitudes. Most importantly, and con-sistent with H2, participants reported being attracted to partners who engaged inattitude alignment (M = 6.55, SD = 0.86) more than partners who did not engagein attitude alignment (M = 5.94, SD = 1.65), F(1, 75) = 8.38, p < .005, d = 0.46.

Consistent with H3, partner similarity and attitude alignment interacted toinfluence attraction (see Figure 1), F(2, 75) = 4.59, p = .01. The effect of attitudesimilarity on attraction depended on the level of partner attitude alignment; specif-ically, attitude similarity predicted attraction when partner attitude alignment didnot occur, but did not predict attraction when partner attitude alignment did occur.We performed a linear contrast of the similarity conditions for participants whowere told that the partner did not engage in attitude alignment and found thatthe contrast was significant, F(1, 11.8) = 7.43, p = .02, with increasing levels ofattraction at increasing levels of similarity. The analogous contrast for participantsthat were told the partner engaged in attitude alignment was not significant, F(1,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 12: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 709

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

No Attitude Alignment Attitude Alignment

Partner Attitude Alignment

Att

ract

ion

25%

50%

75%

FIGURE 1. Interaction of attitude similarity and attitude alignment onattraction.

43) = 0.23, p = .63. Consistent with a great deal of past research, individualswere more attracted to more similar others. However, and consistent with ourprediction, this relation only held in the absence of partner attitude alignment.

The effect of partner attitude alignment on attraction similarly depended onthe level of attitude similarity. Specifically, when partners were only 25% sim-ilar, participants reported greater attraction to partners who engaged in attitudealignment (M = 6.47, SD = 0.93) than partners who did not engage in attitudealignment (M = 4.87, SD = 2.47), t(75) = −3.54, p = .009, d = 0.86. However,when partners were 50% or 75% similar, participants reported similar levels ofattraction to partners whether or not they engaged in attitude alignment, p = .29;p = .99, respectively. In other words, participants perceived partners with a lowlevel of similarity as attractive as long as the partner engaged in attitude align-ment. Table 1 includes pairwise comparison values. Partner attitude alignment ona single issue appears to have been enough to fully counteract or overwhelm theeffect of more general attitude dissimilarity.

Impact of Attitude Alignment on Perceived Reasoning Ability

We next examined whether attitude similarity with partner and partner atti-tude alignment affected participants’ perceptions of the partner’s reasoning ability.Participants perceived partners who engaged in attitude alignment (M = 7.03,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 13: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

710 The Journal of Social Psychology

TABLE 1. Interaction of Attitude Similarity and Attitude Alignment onAttraction Pairwise Comparison Values

Condition Condition

Similarity-attitudealignment

Similarity-attitudealignment t-value Cohen’s d

25%: No alignment 25%: Alignment −3.54∗ 0.8625%: No alignment 50%: No alignment −1.34 0.4425%: No alignment 75%: No alignment −3.75∗ 0.9325%: Alignment 50%: Alignment −0.91 0.4525%: Alignment 75%: Alignment 0.35 0.1750%: No alignment 50%: Alignment −2.11 1.7250%: No alignment 75%: No alignment −1.76 1.1150%: Alignment 75%: Alignment 1.07 0.6475%: No alignment 75%: Alignment 0.65 0.31

Note. No alignment = participant was told attitude alignment did not occur; Alignment =participant was told attitude alignment did occur; p values are presented with Tukey-Krameradjustment.∗p < .01.

SD = 1.34) to have greater reasoning ability than partners who did not engagein attitude alignment (M = 6.30, SD = 1.82), F(1, 30.4) = 5.86, p = .02, d =0.46. Neither similarity nor the interaction of similarity and attitude alignmentsignificantly affected perceived reasoning ability. Because attitude alignmentsignificantly affected perceived reasoning ability, we examined whether reason-ing ability mediated the relationship between attitude alignment and attraction.We conducted bootstrapping analysis to examine the indirect effect of attitudealignment on attraction via perceived reasoning ability using PROCESS (Hayes,2012), which uses a regression-based path-analytical approach to testing medi-ation. The model, conducted with 5,000 bootstraps yielded a bootstrap meanestimate of the indirect effect of .38. The 95% confidence interval did not includezero (.04–1.04); thus, we conclude that perceived reasoning ability mediated therelationship between attitude alignment and attraction. Participants who weretold their partner engaged in attitude alignment perceived their partner as hav-ing greater reasoning ability and, in turn, were more attracted to the interactionpartner (see Figure 2).

Impact of Similarity and Partner Attitude Alignment on Participant AttitudeAlignment

Do individuals engage in greater attitude alignment if their interaction part-ner is similar to them, or if their interaction partner has engaged in attitude

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 14: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 711

Attitude Alignment

Perceived Reasoning Ability

Attraction

.74*

.23

.52**

FIGURE 2. Effect of attitude alignment on attraction mediated by perceivedreasoning ability. Perceived reasoning ability significantly mediated the effectof attitude alignment on attraction (mean estimate of indirect effect = .38;95% confidence interval = .04–1.04). ∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .001.

alignment? We next examined whether attitude similarity and partner attitudealignment affected individuals’ own likelihood of engaging in attitude alignmenton a separate topic. Attitude similarity significantly affected individuals’ attitudealignment, F(2, 75) = 4.06, p = .02. Participants engaged in greater attitude align-ment with partners who were 50% similar (M = 5.05, SD = 1.10) than partnerswho were 25% similar (M = 4.28, SD = 1.19), t(75) = −1.62, p = .02, d = 0.67(the other two pairwise comparisons were not significant). In addition, partici-pants engaged in greater attitude alignment with partners who engaged in attitudealignment (M = 4.81, SD = 1.01) than partners who did not engage in attitudealignment (M = 4.22, SD = 1.10), F(1, 75) = 7.09, p = .01, d = 0.56.

The interaction between attitude similarity and attitude alignment wasmarginally significant, F(2, 75) = 2.87, p = .06 (see Figure 3), and the patternlargely paralleled the results for attraction. The effect of partner attitude align-ment on participants’ attitude alignment (marginally) depended on the level ofattitude similarity. At low levels of similarity (25% similar), participants engagedin greater attitude alignment with partners who engaged in attitude alignment(M = 4.65, SD = 0.93) than partners who did not engage in attitude alignment(M = 3.44, SD = 1.33), t(75) = −3.16, p = .03, d = 1.05. When partners hada greater degree of similarity, participants engaged in similar levels of attitudealignment whether or not the partners engaged in attitude alignment [50% sim-ilar, p = .54; 75% similar, p = .99]. In other words, participants engaged inattitude alignment toward partners with a low level of similarity as long as thepartner engaged in attitude alignment. But at high levels of similarity, participants

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 15: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

712 The Journal of Social Psychology

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

No Attitude Alignment Attitude Alignment

Partner Attitude Alignment

Par

tici

pant

Att

itud

e A

lignm

ent

25%50%75%

FIGURE 3. Interaction of interaction partner attitude similarity and attitudealignment on participant attitude alignment.

engaged in attitude alignment regardless of partner attitude alignment. See Table 2for pairwise comparison values.

Discussion

Most animals are perceptually wired to notice changes in stimulus intensity.We do not feel our clothes constantly touching our bodies, but notice a mosquitolanding on our arm. Changes in brightness or loudness are readily perceived,and moving objects capture attention more than stationary ones. Analogously, themovement of someone’s attitude toward greater similarity on one issue may be asinfluential on attraction as knowing about static degree of similarity across dozensof issues. Though attitude changes are more abstract than perceptual changes,we nevertheless may be similarly prepared to notice even small changes, per-haps for similar reasons. As it is adaptive to preferentially attend to changes inone’s physical environment, it is adaptive to pay particular attention to changesin one’s social environment. A person who engages in attitude alignment withyou sends an important social signal that may help develop or enhance socialcohesion.

Attitude alignment is an important predictor of interpersonal attraction:Individuals were more attracted to those who aligned with their own attitude onan issue than those who did not. In addition, attitude similarity may not predict

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 16: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 713

TABLE 2. Interaction of Attitude Similarity and Participant AttitudeAlignment on Attitude Alignment Response Pairwise Comparisons Values

Condition Condition

Similarity-attitudealignment

Similarity-attitudealignment t-value Cohen’s d

25%: No alignment 25%: Alignment −3.16∗ 1.0525%: No alignment 50%: No alignment −2.11 0.9525%: No alignment 75%: No alignment −2.77† 0.9725%: Alignment 50%: Alignment −1.92 0.5525%: Alignment 75%: Alignment 0.54 0.2550%: No alignment 50%: Alignment −1.70 0.7950%: No alignment 75%: No alignment −0.07 0.0450%: Alignment 75%: Alignment 2.07 0.8975%: No alignment 75%: Alignment 0.22 0.11

Note. No alignment = participant was told attitude alignment did not occur; Alignment =participant was told attitude alignment did occur; p-values are presented with Tukey-Krameradjustment. ∗p < .05. †p = .07.

attraction to the same extent when attitude alignment has taken place. In otherwords, when individuals receive feedback that a partner with whom they previ-ously disagreed has shifted toward agreement, individuals may rate partners asattractive regardless of their level of overall attitude similarity. This finding is par-ticularly encouraging considering that attitude alignment feedback was based on asingle item, whereas overall attitude similarity feedback was based on a proportionof 49 items.

Alignment also can beget alignment. Interestingly, a similar (but onlymarginally significant) pattern emerged when examining individuals’ own atti-tude alignment toward the partner. Participants shifted their own attitudes towardthe partner’s attitudes when the partner was more similar and with partners whoengaged in attitude alignment. If participants received information that the part-ner was highly similar, participants then engaged in attitude alignment with thepartner regardless of the partner’s attitude alignment. However, at low levels ofsimilarity, participants only engaged in attitude alignment when the partners hadengaged in attitude alignment, suggesting that partner attitude alignment on a sin-gle issue (i.e., 2% of the total number of issues) appears to have been enough tofully overwhelm the effect of low general attitude similarity. Thus, it is possiblethat alignment on an important issue could contribute to an ever-upward spiral ofreciprocal alignment and increased liking by both partners.

These findings hint at the possible greater ecological validity of attitudealignment versus attitude similarity in shaping attraction. When forming new

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 17: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

714 The Journal of Social Psychology

relationships, it is unlikely for individuals to learn about another’s attitudes, espe-cially a large number of them, in a static way, whereas individuals are likelyto engage in dynamic conversations as they meet new people and form newrelationships. For instance, Dana Democrat may see a campaign sign in RileyRepublican’s yard during election season, but she is unlikely to receive a greatdeal of information about her new neighbor’s other opinions in a similar man-ner. Dana is likely to learn much more about Riley’s opinions on a varietyof topics during conversations at neighborhood barbeques or school functions.Previous research (Byrne, 1961a; 1961b; Montoya & Horton, 2004; Singh et al.,2007) examining the relationship between attitude similarity and attraction fre-quently used a procedure in which participants received written information abouta partner’s attitudes (i.e., phantom-other technique). In our experiment, partic-ipants received feedback regarding the partner’s attitudes, but also engaged inconversations with the partner, which arguably more closely matches real-worldinteractions.

Attitude alignment is framed as a pro-relationship behavior which may pro-vide partners with a means of promoting and sustaining healthy functioning inrelationships. Attitude alignment was found to be stronger among individualsin ongoing romantic relationships, but attitude alignment also occurred amongstrangers, suggesting that attitude alignment also may play a significant role ininitiating close relationships (Davis & Rusbult, 2001). Though past research iden-tified the circumstances under which attitude alignment is likely to occur (Davis &Rusbult, 2001), it did not assess relationship outcomes. Our study, which involvedreal and vivid, though brief, conversations, sheds light on this process. From abalance theory perspective, disagreements should produce tension that can causeshifts in the form of attitude change in order to relieve tension and allow therelationship to exist in harmony. Knowing that an interaction partner has shiftedtoward agreement signals that balance has been obtained. Attitude alignment alsomay signal that the other person is more agreeable or is more likely to accom-modate when there are non-correspondent outcomes (i.e., when individuals havedifferent preferences regarding behavioral options such as which movie to attend).Reduced tension and a perception of harmony in the relationship may allow indi-viduals to react more positively to the partners. They not only feel more attractedto the partners, but they are also likely to engage in their own shifts toward balanceas well (i.e., attitude alignment toward the partners).

We theorized that attitude alignment would be more influential in predictingattraction than attitude similarity as a result of the evaluative component of atti-tude alignment. In the case of attitude similarity, individuals generally like otherswho are similar to themselves (Byrne 1961a, 1961b), but they did not have anyinfluence on the existence of such similarity—the similarity was pre-existing.Receiving feedback that someone who disagreed now agrees with us is a moreactive form of feedback and serves as an implicit evaluation of us or our argu-ment. Gain-loss theory may shed some additional light on this effect. Aronson

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 18: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 715

and Linder (1965) demonstrated that gains in esteem (i.e., receiving few nega-tive evaluations followed by many positive evaluations) produced higher levels ofattraction than invariant high esteem (i.e., receiving all positive and no negativeevaluations), and suggested that the previous “negative drive state will increasethe attractiveness of an individual who has both created and reduced this negativedrive state” (p. 157). From the gain-loss perspective, attitude alignment may actas a gain in esteem from others.

However, the gain-loss perspective does not explain why attitude alignmentis a strong enough gain to produce high levels of attraction under the condi-tion of low similarity, which could be considered a consistently low negativedrive state. Thus, it appears that previous relations found between positive eval-uations in comparison to attitude similarity in producing attraction (Byrne &Rhamey, 1965) is a better-suited theoretical framework from which to operatefor the current effect. We found that perceived reasoning ability, which can beconsidered a measure of cognitive evaluation, mediated the attitude alignment-attraction relationship. More specifically, participants who were told the partnerengaged in attitude alignment perceived the partner as having higher levels ofreasoning ability and, in turn, were more attracted to the partner, which fitsthe Byrne and Rhamey (1965) framework. It may be useful in future studiesto further examine inferred attraction and cognitive evaluation and determineif these variables partially mediate the relationship between attitude alignmentand attraction in ways that are similar to their roles in the relationship betweenattitude similarity and attraction (Condo & Crano, 1988, Singh et al., 2007,2008). For instance, whether the attitude alignment-attraction relationship canbe accounted for by cognitive evaluation or inferred attraction may be moder-ated by opportunities for future interaction. Attraction may be driven more byinferred attraction if participants expect to interact with the partner in the future,(i.e., partners are perceived as attempting to ingratiate themselves for a smootherrelationship), whereas attraction may be driven more by cognitive evaluation ifparticipants do not expect to interact with the partner in the future (i.e., part-ners are perceived as seeing the error in their argument and making a personaladjustment).

Some limitations should be noted. Because participants met the partner (whowas also a participant) face-to-face and had a real conversation about two attitudeissues, we were unable to completely control the interaction. It is likely that somepairings and conversations ran more smoothly than others and that other types ofsimilarity (e.g., demographics) may have been apparent in some pairings but not inothers. The nature of this research design may also account for the smaller effectof similarity than in previous similarity-attraction research using the phantom-other technique, because other processes (see Byrne, 1992 for a review) may comeinto play during an actual interaction (that more closely matches the real world)than during the phantom-other technique. However, the pairings were random andthus we expect that these influences would be minimal, and cannot account for

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 19: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

716 The Journal of Social Psychology

the observed differences in attraction based on attitude alignment and similarity.We also believe that the realistic nature of the interaction provides a strong testof the effect and goes beyond merely providing participants with experimentally-controlled written information about the partner.

Future research also could examine attraction longitudinally to provide amore complete understanding of how attraction unfolds over time based on atti-tude alignment information (i.e., by measuring attitudes both before and afterdiscussing issues). Future research also may benefit from examining additionalpotential moderators of the attitude alignment-attraction relation. Our researchdid not find any differences based on self-esteem or affect, but other variables(e.g., narcissism, power motivation, agreeableness) may play a role and could beexamined further.

Furthermore, our research examined interpersonal attraction in a generalsense rather than specifically within the context of romantic or sexual relation-ships. It is possible that the attitude alignment-attraction relation may operatedifferently (e.g., different variables may moderate or mediate the relationship,effect sizes may vary by relationship type) within romantic and sexual contextsand could be examined by further research in online or speed dating studies.Finally, future research should further examine participants’ own attitude align-ment toward their partners (i.e., attitude alignment response) and develop bettermeasures. Our measure for this area of reciprocated attitude alignment consistedof a single item, which can carry greater risk of being psychometrically problem-atic than measures consisting of multiple items. Future research may benefit fromdeveloping additional items to create a more powerful measure of participants’own attitude alignment.

This research contributes to the literature examining attitudes and attractionby extending the research of Davis and Rusbult (2001) and identifying two out-comes of partner attitude alignment (i.e., attraction and attitude alignment towardthe partner). It is particularly intriguing that a shift on a single item has the poten-tial to be more influential than overall attitude similarity on 49 items. This researchalso provides evidence of a pro-relationship function of attitude alignment, whichwe believe has great potential as a developing research area. We hope that futureresearch will continue to examine the mechanisms behind this relation.

NOTES

1. Davis and Rusbult found that peripheral to self-central to partner issues producedrelatively greater attitude alignment, whereas central to self-peripheral to partner issuesyielded an interesting comparison group of lesser attitude alignment. (Issues that werecentral to both partners or peripheral to both partners were not selected.)

2. It is unlikely that participants would believe feedback that the partners had shiftedtheir attitudes if they learned during discussion that the issue was highly important to thepartner (previous attitude alignment research established that individuals are unlikely toshift their attitudes on issues which they consider central to their self).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 20: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 717

3. We ran analyses with the seven participants who inaccurately recalled their attitudesimilarity condition removed, but the pattern of results remained the same. Thus, we choseto keep the participants in the dataset for all analyses.

4. Similarity did not significantly affect self-esteem, positive affect, or negative affect(all Fs < .32, ps > .73). Attitude alignment did not significantly affect self-esteem, positiveaffect, or negative affect (all Fs < 1.54, ps > .20). Finally, the interaction between similarityand attitude alignment did not significantly affect positive affect or negative affect (all Fs< 2.30, ps > .11), but did affect self-esteem, F(2, 75) = 3.37, p = .04. However, fur-ther examination of pairwise comparisons revealed no significant effects. We examined theeffect of feedback order and sex on attraction in order to rule out any feedback order andsex differences, and found no significant differences in attraction based on feedback orderor sex (all Fs < 2.06, ps > .15). We did, however, find a significant effect of sex-pairing(i.e., whether pair of participants were of the same [55.84%] or opposite [44.16%] sex) onattraction. Participants rated the partner as more attractive if the partner was of the samesex (M = 6.60, SD = 1.03; N = 43) than if the partner was of the opposite sex (M = 5.93,SD = 1.48; N = 34), F(1, 22.5) = 5.28, p = .03, d = .53.

5. Because initial analyses showed a significant effect of sex pairing, we also ran themain analysis including attitude similarity, attitude alignment, sex pairing, and all possibleinteractions in the model. Sex pairing did not significantly affect attraction, and it did notsignificantly interact with either attitude similarity or attitude alignment. The three-wayinteraction was also not significant (all Fs < 3.42, ps > .05).

AUTHOR NOTES

Chelsea A. Reid is affiliated with the Department of Psychology, VirginiaCommonwealth University. Jody L. Davis is affiliated with the Department of Psychology,Virginia Commonwealth University. Jeffrey D. Green is affiliated with the Department ofPsychology, Virginia Commonwealth University.

REFERENCES

Aronson, E., & Linder, D. (1965). Gain and loss of esteem as determinants of interpersonalattractiveness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 156–171.

Backman, C. W., & Secord, P. F. (1959). The effects of perceived liking on interpersonalattraction. Human Relations, 12, 713–715.

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M., R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonalattachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.

Bell, P. A., & Baron, R. A. (1974). Environmental influences on attraction: Effects ofhea, attitude similarity, and personal evaluations. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,4, 479–481.

Byrne, D. (1961a). Interpersonal attraction and attitude similarity. The Journal of Abnormaland Social Psychology, 62, 713–715.

Byrne, D. (1961b). Interpersonal attraction as a function of affiliation motivation andattitude similarity. Human Relations, 14, 283–289.

Byrne, D. (1992). The transition from controlled laboratory experimentation to lesscontrolled settings: Surprise! Additional variables are operative. CommunicationMonographs, 59, 190–198.

Byrne, D. (1997). An overview (and underview) of research and theory within the attractionparadigm. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 417–431.

Byrne, D., & Blaylock, B. (1963). Similarity and assumed similarity of attitudes betweenhusbands and wives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 636–640.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 21: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

718 The Journal of Social Psychology

Byrne, D., Bond, M. H., & Diamond, M. J. (1969). Response to political candidates as afunction of attitude similarity-dissimilarity. Human Relations, 22, 251–262.

Byrne, D., & Ervin, C. R. (1969). Attraction toward a Negro stranger as a function of prej-udice, attitude similarity, and the stranger’s evaluation of the subject. Human Relations,22, 397–404.

Byrne, D., & Griffit, W. (1966). Similarity versus liking: A clarification. PsychonomicScience, 6, 295–296.

Byrne, D., & Nelson, D. (1965). Attraction as a linear function of proportion of positivereinforcement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 659–663.

Byrne, D., & Rhamey, R. (1965). Magnitude of positive and negative reinforcements as adeterminant of attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 884–889.

Campbell, L., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Estimating actor, partner, and interaction effects fordyadic data using PROC MIXED and HLM: A user-friendly guide. Personal Relations,9, 327–342.

Clore, G., I., & Baldridge, B. (1968). Interpersonal attraction: The role of agreement andtopic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 340–346.

Condo, J. W., & Crano, W. D. (1988). Inferred evaluation and the relation between attitudesimilarity and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,789–797.

Davis, J. L., & Rusbult, C. E. (2001). Attitude alignment in close relationships. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 81, 65–84.

Etcheverry, P. E., & Agnew, C. R. (2009). Similarity in cigarette smoking attracts:A prospective study of partner selection by own smoking and smoker prototypes.Psychology of Additive Behaviors, 23, 632–643.

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variablemediation, moderation and conditional process modeling. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf.

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley.Insko, C. A., Sedlak, A. J., & Lipsitz, A. (1982). A two-valued logic or two-valued balance

resolution of the challenge of agreement and attraction effects in p-o-x triads, and a theo-retical perspective on conformity and hedonism. European Journal of Social Psychology,12, 143–167.

Insko, C. A., Thompson, V. D., Stroeb, W., Shaud, K. F., Pinner, B. E., & Layton, B. D.(1973). Implied evaluation and the similarity-attraction effect. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 25, 297–308.

Kalmijn, M. (2005). Attitude alignment in marriage and cohabitation: The case of sex-roleattitudes. Personal Relationships, 12, 521–535.

Kandel, D. B. (1978). Similarity in real-life adolescent friendship pairs. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 36, 306–312.

Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as aninterpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 68, 518–530.

Lehr, A. T., & Geher, G. (2006). Differential effects of reciprocity and attitude similairtyacross long- versus short-term making contexts. The Journal of Social Psychology, 146,423– 439.

Luo, S. (2009). Partner selection and relationships satisfaction in early dating couples: Therole of couple similarity. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 133–138.

McCrae, R. R., Martin, T. A., Hrebickova, M., Urbanek, T., Boomsma, D. I., Willemsen,G., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2008). Personality trait similarity between spouses in four cultures.Journal of Personality, 76, 1137–1164.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4

Page 22: Libraries] Function of Attitude Similarity and Attitude ...

Reid, Davis, & Green 719

McGarva, A. R., & Warner, R. M. (2003). Attraction and social coordination: Mutualentertainment of vocal activity rhythms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32,335– 354.

McGinley, H., Nicholas, K., & McGinley, P. (1978). Effects of body position and attitudesimilarity on interpersonal attraction and opinion change. Psychological Reports, 42,127–138.

Montoya, R. M., & Horton, R. S. (2004). On the importance of cognitive evaluation as adeterminany of interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,86, 696–712.

Montoya, R. M., & Horton, R. S. (2012). A meta-analytic investigation of the processesunderlying the similarity-attraction effect. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,30, 64–94.

Montoya, R. M. & Horton, R. S., & Kirchner, J. (2008). Is actual similarity necessary forattraction? A meta-analysis of actual and perceived similarity. Journal of Social andPersonal Relationships, 25, 889–922.

Montoya, R. M., & Insko, C. (2008). Toward a more complete understanding of thereciprocity of liking effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 477–498.

Newcomb, T. M. (1953). An approach to the study of communicative acts. PsychologicalReview, 60, 393–404.

Newcomb, T. M. (1959). Individual systems of orientation. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology:A study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 384–422). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Orive, R. (1988). Social projection and social comparison of opinions. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 54, 953–964.

Singh, R. (1975). Reinforcement, affect, and interpersonal attraction. Psychologia, 18,142–148.

Singh, R., Ho, L. J., Tan, H. L., & Bell, P. A. (2007). Attitudes, personal evaluations, cog-nitive evaluation and interpersonal attraction: On the direct, indirect and reverse-causaleffects. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 19–42.

Singh, R., Ng, R., Ong, E. L., & Lin, P. K. F. (2008). Different mediators for the age,sex, and attitude similarity effects in interpersonal attraction. Basic and Applied SocialPsychology, 30, 1–17.

Singh, R., Yeo, S. E., Lin, P. K. F., & Tan, L. (2007). Multiple mediators of the atti-tude similarity-attraction relationship: Dominance of inferred evaluation and subtletyof affect. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29, 61–74.

Taylor, H. F. (1967). Balance and change in the two-person group. Sociometry, 30,262–279.

Tenney, E. R., Turkheimer, E., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2009). Being liked is more than hav-ing a good personality: The role of matching. Journal of Research in Personality, 43,579–585.

Till, A., & Freedman, E. M. (1978). Complementarity versus similarity of traits operatingin the choice of marriage and dating partners. The Journal of Social Psychology, 105,147–148.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief mea-sures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

Received March 11, 2013Accepted July 8, 2013

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Vir

gini

a C

omm

onw

ealth

Uni

vers

ity L

ibra

ries

] at

12:

38 0

3 M

arch

201

4