journla of marketing research

98
Editorial Staff Editor P. RAJAN VARADARAJAN JAMS Department of Marketing Texas A&M University 4112 TAMU College Station, TX 77843-4112 Phone: (979) 862-1019 Fax: (979) 862-1020 E-mail: [email protected] Book Reviews PEGGY H. CUNNINGHAM Queen’s University School of Business 229 Dunning Hall 99 University Avenue Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6 Canada Phone: (613) 533-2327 Fax: (613) 583-2321 E-mail: pcunningham@ business.queensu.ca Marketing and the Law ANITA CAVA ANN MORALES OLAZÁBAL RENÉ SACASAS Business Law Department School of Business Administration University of Miami P.O. Box 248022 Coral Gables, FL 33124 Phone: (305) 284-4633 Fax: (305) 284-3762 The Academy of Marketing Science is a member of the International Association for Management Education (AACSB). It gratefully acknowledges the financial support of the Mary Kay Cosmetics Excellence in Marketing Fund. The JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE is the official journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. It is an international, refereed journal intended to further the science of marketing throughout the world by promoting the conduct of research and the dissemination of research results through the study and improvement of marketing as an economic, ethical, and social force. For manuscript submission information, refer to the inside back cover. *Founding Fellow MANOJ K. AGARWAL Binghamton University CHRISTIE H. AMATO University of North Carolina–Charlotte JONLEE ANDREWS Indiana University KWAKU ATUAHENE-GIMA City University of Hong Kong RICK BAGOZZI Rice University SHARON BEATTY University of Alabama DAN BELLO Georgia State University *HAROLD W. BERKMAN University of Miami SUNDAR BHARADWAJ Emory University DAVID M. BOUSH University of Oregon JAMES R. BROWN Virginia Tech STEVEN P. BROWN Southern Methodist University TOM J. BROWN Oklahoma State University MICHELE BUNN University of Alabama ALAN BUSH University of Memphis ROGER CALANTONE Michigan State University JOSEPH P. CANNON Colorado State University GOUTAM CHAKRABORTY Oklahoma State University GOUTAM CHALLAGALLA Georgia Institute of Technology RAJESH CHANDY University of Minnesota BRUCE CLARK Northeastern University JOSEPH COTE Washington State University DAVID W. CRAVENS Texas Christian University PEGGY H. CUNNINGHAM Queen’s University MICHAEL R. CZINKOTA Georgetown University PRATIBHA DABHOLKAR University of Tennessee PETER A. DACIN Queen’s University PETER DICKSON Florida International University PAM SCHOLDER ELLEN Georgia State University ELLEN GARBARINO Case Western Reserve University JIM GENTRY University of Nebraska RONALD C. GOODSTEIN Georgetown University EVERT GUMMESSON Stockholm University GREG GUNDLACH University of Notre Dame CHRISTIAN HOMBURG University of Mannheim G. TOMAS M. HULT Michigan State University MICHAEL HYMAN New Mexico State University CHARLES A. INGENE University of Mississippi JEAN L. JOHNSON Washington State University SUSAN KEAVENEY University of Colorado at Denver AMNA KIRMANI Southern Methodist University MASAAKI KOTABE Temple University VICKI LANE University of Colorado at Denver MICHAEL R. LEVY Babson College DEBBIE MACINNIS University of Southern California SCOTT B. MACKENZIE Indiana University GREG MARSHALL Oklahoma State University CHARLOTTE MASON University of North Carolina AJAY MENON Colorado State University ANIL MENON IBM Corporation BANWARI MITTAL Northern Kentucky University DAVID B. MONTGOMERY Stanford University MITZI MONTOYA-WEISS North Carolina State University ROBERT M. MORGAN University of Alabama KENT NAKAMOTO Virginia Tech CHERYL NAKATA University of Illinois at Chicago DAS NARAYANDAS Harvard Business School RICHARD C. NETEMEYER University of Virginia DAVID J. ORTINAU University of South Florida AMY L. OSTROM Arizona State University THOMAS J. PAGE Michigan State University A. PARASURAMAN University of Miami ROBERT A. PETERSON University of Texas at Austin S. RATNESHWAR University of Connecticut WILLIAM T. ROBINSON Purdue University SAEED SAMIEE University of Tulsa SANJIT SENGUPTA San Francisco State University VENKATESH SHANKAR University of Maryland JAGDIP SINGH Case Western Reserve University JAMES M. SINKULA University of Vermont K. SIVAKUMAR Lehigh University AMY K. SMITH George Washington University DANIEL C. SMITH Indiana University N. CRAIG SMITH London Business School RICHARD SPRENG Michigan State University DEVANATHAN SUDHARSHAN University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign DAVID M. SZYMANSKI Texas A&M University STEPHEN S. TAX University of Victoria SHIRLEY TAYLOR Queen’s University GLENN VOSS North Carolina State University BRIAN WANSINK University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign ROBERT B. WOODRUFF University of Tennessee JOHN WORKMAN Creighton University MANJIT S. YADAV Texas A&M University GEORGE M. ZINKHAN University of Georgia SHAOMING ZOU University of Missouri at Columbia Editorial Review Board For Sage Publications: David Neyhart, Gillian Dickens, Ken Berthel, and Kelli Palma

description

January edition of indian journal of marketing

Transcript of journla of marketing research

  • Editorial StaffEditorP. RAJAN VARADARAJANJAMSDepartment of MarketingTexas A&M University4112 TAMUCollege Station, TX 77843-4112Phone: (979) 862-1019Fax: (979) 862-1020E-mail: [email protected]

    Book ReviewsPEGGY H. CUNNINGHAMQueens UniversitySchool of Business229 Dunning Hall99 University AvenueKingston, Ontario K7L 3N6

    CanadaPhone: (613) 533-2327Fax: (613) 583-2321E-mail: pcunningham@

    business.queensu.ca

    Marketing and the LawANITA CAVAANN MORALES OLAZBALREN SACASASBusiness Law DepartmentSchool of Business

    AdministrationUniversity of MiamiP.O. Box 248022Coral Gables, FL 33124Phone: (305) 284-4633Fax: (305) 284-3762

    The Academy of MarketingScience is a member of theInternational Association forManagement Education(AACSB). It gratefullyacknowledges the financialsupport of the Mary KayCosmetics Excellence inMarketing Fund.

    The JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OFMARKETING SCIENCE is the official journalof the Academy of Marketing Science. It is aninternational, refereed journal intended tofurther the science of marketing throughout theworld by promoting the conduct ofresearch and the dissemination of researchresults through the study and improvement ofmarketing as an economic, ethical, and socialforce. For manuscript submission information,refer to the inside back cover.

    *Founding Fellow

    MANOJ K. AGARWALBinghamton UniversityCHRISTIE H. AMATOUniversity of North CarolinaCharlotteJONLEE ANDREWSIndiana UniversityKWAKU ATUAHENE-GIMACity University of Hong KongRICK BAGOZZIRice UniversitySHARON BEATTYUniversity of AlabamaDAN BELLOGeorgia State University*HAROLD W. BERKMANUniversity of MiamiSUNDAR BHARADWAJEmory UniversityDAVID M. BOUSHUniversity of OregonJAMES R. BROWNVirginia TechSTEVEN P. BROWNSouthern Methodist UniversityTOM J. BROWNOklahoma State UniversityMICHELE BUNNUniversity of AlabamaALAN BUSHUniversity of MemphisROGER CALANTONEMichigan State UniversityJOSEPH P. CANNONColorado State UniversityGOUTAM CHAKRABORTYOklahoma State UniversityGOUTAM CHALLAGALLAGeorgia Institute of TechnologyRAJESH CHANDYUniversity of MinnesotaBRUCE CLARKNortheastern UniversityJOSEPH COTEWashington State University

    DAVID W. CRAVENSTexas Christian UniversityPEGGY H. CUNNINGHAMQueens UniversityMICHAEL R. CZINKOTAGeorgetown UniversityPRATIBHA DABHOLKARUniversity of TennesseePETER A. DACINQueens UniversityPETER DICKSONFlorida International UniversityPAM SCHOLDER ELLENGeorgia State UniversityELLEN GARBARINOCase Western Reserve UniversityJIM GENTRYUniversity of NebraskaRONALD C. GOODSTEINGeorgetown UniversityEVERT GUMMESSONStockholm UniversityGREG GUNDLACHUniversity of Notre DameCHRISTIAN HOMBURGUniversity of MannheimG. TOMAS M. HULTMichigan State UniversityMICHAEL HYMANNew Mexico State UniversityCHARLES A. INGENEUniversity of MississippiJEAN L. JOHNSONWashington State UniversitySUSAN KEAVENEYUniversity of Colorado at DenverAMNA KIRMANISouthern Methodist UniversityMASAAKI KOTABETemple UniversityVICKI LANEUniversity of Colorado at DenverMICHAEL R. LEVYBabson College

    DEBBIE MACINNISUniversity of Southern CaliforniaSCOTT B. MACKENZIEIndiana UniversityGREG MARSHALLOklahoma State UniversityCHARLOTTE MASONUniversity of North CarolinaAJAY MENONColorado State UniversityANIL MENONIBM CorporationBANWARI MITTALNorthern Kentucky UniversityDAVID B. MONTGOMERYStanford UniversityMITZI MONTOYA-WEISSNorth Carolina State UniversityROBERT M. MORGANUniversity of AlabamaKENT NAKAMOTOVirginia TechCHERYL NAKATAUniversity of Illinois at ChicagoDAS NARAYANDASHarvard Business SchoolRICHARD C. NETEMEYERUniversity of VirginiaDAVID J. ORTINAUUniversity of South FloridaAMY L. OSTROMArizona State UniversityTHOMAS J. PAGEMichigan State UniversityA. PARASURAMANUniversity of MiamiROBERT A. PETERSONUniversity of Texas at AustinS. RATNESHWARUniversity of ConnecticutWILLIAM T. ROBINSONPurdue UniversitySAEED SAMIEEUniversity of Tulsa

    SANJIT SENGUPTASan Francisco State UniversityVENKATESH SHANKARUniversity of MarylandJAGDIP SINGHCase Western Reserve UniversityJAMES M. SINKULAUniversity of VermontK. SIVAKUMARLehigh UniversityAMY K. SMITHGeorge Washington UniversityDANIEL C. SMITHIndiana UniversityN. CRAIG SMITHLondon Business SchoolRICHARD SPRENGMichigan State UniversityDEVANATHAN SUDHARSHANUniversity of Illinois at Urbana-

    ChampaignDAVID M. SZYMANSKITexas A&M UniversitySTEPHEN S. TAXUniversity of VictoriaSHIRLEY TAYLORQueens UniversityGLENN VOSSNorth Carolina State UniversityBRIAN WANSINKUniversity of Illinois at Urbana-

    ChampaignROBERT B. WOODRUFFUniversity of TennesseeJOHN WORKMANCreighton UniversityMANJIT S. YADAVTexas A&M UniversityGEORGE M. ZINKHANUniversity of GeorgiaSHAOMING ZOUUniversity of Missouri at Columbia

    Editorial Review Board

    For Sage Publications: David Neyhart, Gillian Dickens, Ken Berthel, and Kelli Palma

  • Salesperson Cooperation:The Influence of Relational, Task, Organizational, and Personal FactorsCengiz Yilmaz and Shelby D. Hunt 335

    The Influence of Complementarity, Compatibility,and Relationship Capital on Alliance PerformanceMB Sarkar, Raj Echambadi, S. Tamer Cavusgil, and Preet S. Aulakh 358

    Customer Switching Behavior in Online Services:An Exploratory Study of the Role of SelectedAttitudinal, Behavioral, and Demographic FactorsSusan M. Keaveney and Madhavan Parthasarathy 374

    Managing Culturally Diverse Buyer-Seller Relationships:The Role of Intercultural Disposition and Adaptive Sellingin Developing Intercultural Communication CompetenceVictoria D. Bush, Gregory M. Rose, Faye Gilbert, and Thomas N. Ingram 391

    COMMISSIONED ARTICLEGuidelines for Conducting Research and Publishing in Marketing:From Conceptualization Through the Review ProcessJohn O. Summers 405

    REVIEWS OF BOOKS 416MARKETING AND THE LAW 424INDEX 427

    Journal of theAcademy ofMarketingScienceFall 2001 Volume 29 Number 4

    The ACADEMY of MARKETING SCIENCECentral Office: School of Business AdministrationUniversity of Miami

    Published by Sage PublicationsThousand Oaks London New Delhi

  • JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION

    Salesperson Cooperation:The Influence of Relational, Task,Organizational, and Personal Factors

    Cengiz YilmazGebze Institute of Technology, Turkey

    Shelby D. HuntTexas Tech University

    Salesperson cooperation has become a crucial issue forthe overall performance of most sales organizations. Theauthors examine the antecedents of task-specific, coopera-tive behaviors of salespersons toward other salespeopleworking in the same organization. The main theses of thestudy are that (1) the four major antecedent categories offactorsrelational, task, organizational, and personalconstitute, collectively, the primary determinants of sales-person cooperation and (2) each antecedent category ex-erts, independently, significant influence on the co-operative behaviors of salespersons. The results supportthe main theses and provide useful insights for sales man-agers attempting to foster cooperation among salespeo-ple. The relative impact of each antecedent category, aswell as the effects of specific variables within each, isdiscussed.

    Recent decades have witnessed a dramatic change inthe nature of the selling job for many companies. The tra-ditional view of a salespersona single, individualistic,persistent person who works independently on a commis-sion basis and who competes fiercely against even fellowsalespersonshas given way to a strikingly different con-ceptualization (Cespedes, Doyle, and Freedman 1989;Weitz and Bradford 1999). Selling in many businessestoday has become an integrated process that requires the

    coordinated efforts of salespeople and other participants,both within and across product lines, functional depart-ments, and geographic districts. Cooperation, defined asthe willful contribution of individuals, groups, and so on,to the successful completion of common tasks and/or tothe achievement of mutual objectives (J. Anderson andNarus 1990; Deutsch 1949; Wagner 1995) has become acritical issue in sales management. Many companies seeksales forces composed of cooperative salespersons whocan work effectively in groups. In such sales forces, sales-people share their skills, knowledge, time, and effort withcoworkers to achieve common objectives. This emergingera of the cooperative salesperson is manifested in thegrowing use of team selling (Moon and Armstrong 1994),relationship selling (Weitz and Bradford 1999), sellingcenters (Hutt, Johnston, and Ronchento 1985), and keyaccount programs (Cohen 1996).

    As a result of the growing importance of cooperativeselling, research in sales force management has begun tofocus on understanding the dynamics of a salespersonsinterpersonal relationships with coworkers. Issues investi-gated include feedback provided by coworkers (Kohli andJaworski 1994), sales force socialization (Dubinsky,Howell, Ingram, and Bellenger 1986), peer mentoring(Pullins, Fine, and Warren 1996), and altruistic behaviorstoward coworkers as a form of organizational citizenshipbehaviors (e.g., Netemeyer, Boles, McKee, andMcMurrian 1997). Nonetheless, salesperson cooperation,a critical determinant of the effectiveness of selling effortsfor many businesses, has received little attention.

    Consider the problem faced by a sales manager whobelieves that salesperson cooperation is important for

    Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.Volume 29, No. 4, pages 335-357.Copyright 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science.

  • sales performance and wants to take action or develop pol-icies to increase such cooperation. The literatures of thedifferent research traditions that have examined coopera-tion give different, sometimes conflicting, advice. As sug-gested by the relationship marketing literature (e.g.,Dwyer, Schurr, and Oh 1987; Morgan and Hunt 1994;J. Smith and Barclay 1997), should the sales managerfocus on taking steps to increase the trust and commitmentof salespeople? Or, should the manager focus on increas-ing the task interdependence of the salespeople, as sug-gested by Deutsch (1973); Van De Ven, Delbecq, andKoenig (1976); and Wageman and Baker (1997)? Or,should the manager simply focus on hiring salespeoplewho have a general proclivity toward cooperativeness, assuggested by the works of Argyle (1991) and Chatman andBarsade (1995)? Answering these questions requiresresearch that crosses disciplinary lines.

    Using an interdisciplinary approach, we address thequestion: Why do some salespeople, more than others,cooperate with coworkers? We develop and test a model ofantecedent factors that affect salesperson cooperation,which is viewed as task-specific, cooperative behaviorsamong salespeople. On the basis of a review of themultidisciplinary literature on interpersonal cooperationin organizations and workgroups, we propose that each ofthe antecedent factors suggested by prior research can becategorized into one of four categories: relational, task,organizational, and personal. The main theses of our studyare that (1) the four major antecedent categories constitute,collectively, major determinants of salesperson coopera-tion; (2) each antecedent category exerts, independently,significant influence on cooperative tendencies amongsalespeople; and therefore, (3) sales managers shouldendeavor to address factors in all four categories and notjust focus on one or two. Thus, our study aims to providesales managers with guidance on how to promote coopera-tion among their salespeople.

    The article is organized as follows. First, we brieflyreview the literature on interpersonal cooperation in orga-nizations. Next, we describe the four main antecedent cat-egories and develop a structural model that incorporatespredictor variables from each. Third, we test the proposedmodel using a large sample of salespersons (N = 531) from112 different automobile dealerships. The final sectionsinclude implications and suggestions for future research.

    INTERPERSONAL COOPERATION

    K. Smith, Carroll, and Ashford (1995) suggest thatapproaches to the study of cooperation can be grouped intofive broad traditions. First, an influential research traditionexplains the emergence of cooperation based on thecalculative orientations of individuals (e.g., Williamson1975). In this view, individuals will cooperate if and only if

    cooperation is in their long-term self-interests based ontheir rational calculations. According to K. Smith et al.(1995), most well-known theoretical explanations ofcooperation belong to this first category (e.g., transactioncost theory and game theory). A second research traditionaddresses the noneconomic aspects of cooperative rela-tionships (e.g., Thibaut and Kelley 1959). Rooted in thesocial exchange literature, research in this traditionfocuses on the effects of interpersonal attraction, psycho-logical attachment, and norms of reciprocity.

    A third approach relies heavily on power and conflicttheories (e.g., Emerson 1962). Conflict, the opposite ofcooperation according to some authors and a key conceptin these theories, stems from diversity in individualsresources, perceptions of injustice, values, and goals. Afourth approach relies on social-structure theories andemphasizes dimensions outside the focal relationship toexplain cooperation (e.g., P. Blau 1974). Social, cultural,and structural aspects of the environment in which therelationship occurs are seen as drivers of cooperation.Finally, the fifth approach involves modeling theories andemphasizes the impact of social learning and imitation oncooperative tendencies (e.g., Bandura 1971). Given thediffering underlying assumptions and units of analysisadopted by each research tradition, the current state ofinquiry on cooperation is replete with explanatory vari-ables (K. Smith et al. 1995).

    Differences notwithstanding, at least three similaritiesexist across the research traditions that explore coopera-tion. First, definitions of cooperation in the traditions con-verge on a common conceptual domain, and all include awillful-contribution element and a common task or objec-tive element.1 Second, the resulting outcome for most tasksituations is increased productivity, especially in complextask situations (Tjosvold 1984; Tjosvold and Tsao 1989),because of cooperating individuals tending to (1) provideeach other with necessary information, (2) more willinglyassist and help each other, (3) understand each otherspoints of view, (4) be influenced by each others interestsand ideas, and (5) rely on division of labor (Laughlin1978).2 Third, some conceptual overlap exists among theexplanatory variables suggested by each approach, eventhough research in each traditiontrue to the silo viewof academiaseldom crosses lines (K. Smith et al. 1995).Perhaps this lack of an interdisciplinary approachaccounts for the low variance explained in most studies ofcooperation.

    Indeed, research in each of the traditions has (necessar-ily) been limited in scope (i.e., in terms of including allmajor antecedents of cooperation). For example, studiesusing game theory generally emphasize structural andpsychological determinants such as task characteristicsand personalities of the participants (e.g., Murnighan1994), whereas studies based on social-exchange theoryfocus on the aspects of the relationship between

    336 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

  • cooperating parties. Similarly, while social-structure theo-ries focus solely on the broader context in which a cooper-ative relationship occurs, such as the structural and cul-tural environment, modeling theories highlight theinfluence of third parties outside the focal relationship(e.g., managers). However, as Pinto, Pinto, and Prescott(1993) note, factors that act as facilitators of cooperation inorganizations may belong to a broad set of antecedent cate-gories, ranging from individual factors such as personali-ties of group members, interpersonal relations and trainingand skills . . . to organizational factors such as strategy,structure, reward systems, and cultural norms (p. 1282).Therefore, using inferences from each of the traditions, weargue that the cooperative behaviors of salespeopleemerge from the combined effects of variables in four dis-tinct categories: (1) the quality of interpersonal relation-ships between organizational members, that is, relationalfactors; (2) specific properties and requirements of the taskat hand, that is, task factors; (3) the structural, cultural, pro-cedural, and managerial dimensions of the organization,that is, organizational factors; and (4) individual charac-teristics of organizational members, that is, personal fac-tors. Table 1 provides a review of the explanatory variablesin the cooperation research. Each antecedent variable usedin the various research approaches can be grouped into oneof the four categories.

    A MODEL OF SALESPERSONCOOPERATION

    Our model of salesperson cooperation is shown in Fig-ure 1. Although the model incorporates antecedent factorsfrom each main category, it is obvious that not all potentialfactors can be included. Thus, the factors from each cate-gory included in our model are those we propose are mostrelevant to salesperson cooperation in the context of thepresent study. For example, factors such as organizationalcommitment and job satisfaction are included in the modelbecause these factors are frequently used attitudinal vari-ables in the sales management literature in explainingsalesperson behaviors. Similarly, factors such as trust incoworkers and task interdependence are included sincesuch factors are key explanatory factors suggested in atleast one of the research traditions exploring cooperation.We discuss each variable in the four antecedent categoriesand the theoretical and empirical grounds for 15 specifichypotheses.

    Relational Factors

    Relational factors are those that cause salespersons tovalue their relationships with coworkers and developmutually beneficial, long-term orientations in workingrelationships. The social-exchange literature implies that

    interpersonal attraction, psychological attachment, andnorms of reciprocitystimulated by loyalties, friendship,and faithful expectationsaffect individuals behavioralchoices in relationships. Although such relational vari-ables as communication quality (J. Anderson and Narus1990), shared values (Chatman 1991; Morgan and Hunt1994), cultural differences (McAllister 1995), person-organization fit (Chatman 1991; Netemeyer et al. 1997),and expectations regarding the future behaviors of rolepartners (Wiener and Doescher 1994) have been theorizedto affect cooperative tendencies, the most prominent rela-tional factors are trust and commitment (Achrol 1991;Morgan and Hunt 1994).

    Indeed, commitment and trust are considered key fordistinguishing social from purely economic exchange(K. Cook and Emerson 1978; G. McDonald 1981). Coop-eration entails vulnerability, and both commitment andtrust are considered necessary for individuals to value arelationship and to be willing to be vulnerable (Mayer,Davis, and Schoorman 1995; Weitz and Bradford 1999).Morgan and Hunt (1994) theorize that an individualscommitment to a relationship and trust in the exchangepartner are key determinants of several behavioral tenden-cies in the relationship, including a disposition to cooper-ate. Similarly, we argue that a salespersons trust incoworkers and his or her commitment to the organizationare central to understanding how relational factors facili-tate cooperation. Specifically, with respect to salespersoncooperation, we model (1) organizational commitment asmediating the effects of intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfac-tion, (2) trust in coworkers as mediating the effects of pastopportunistic behaviors of coworkers and communicationquality, and (3) both trust and commitment as mediatingthe effect of shared values.

    Organizational commitment and cooperation. Organi-zational commitment was originally defined as thestrength of an individuals identification with and involve-ment in a particular organization (Porter, Steers,Mowday, and Boulian 1974:604). Stated this way, highlevels of organizational commitment are characterized bypositive affective responses toward various subgroups, in-cluding coworkers, that form the organization (Becker1992). Thus, a salespersons commitment to the organiza-tion should facilitate his or her cooperative tendencies to-ward coworkers. Salespeople who are committed to theorganization should attach more importance to their rela-tionships with coworkers, anticipate future interactionswith coworkers for a longer time horizon, and highly valuetheir associations with coworkers (OReilly and Chatman1986). Each of these variables, in turn, positively affectscooperative tendencies (Axelrod 1984; Heide and Miner1992). Supporting this view, organizational commitmenthas been shown to promote several forms of constructiveorganizational behaviors (OReilly and Chatman 1986),

    Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 337

  • including organizational citizenship (Tompson andWerner 1997) and level of effort exerted for group mainte-nance (G. Blau and Boal 1987). Specifically, Dubinsky,Kotabe, Lim, and Wagner (1997) demonstrate that sales-people who value pro-social behaviors are also more com-mitted to the organization, and MacKenzie, Podsakoff,and Ahearne (1998) show that organizational commitmentis associated strongly in sales force contexts with varioussupportive, extrarole activities, including those directed topeers.

    Hypothesis 1: Organizational commitment and salesper-son cooperation are positively related.

    Trust in coworkers and cooperation. A salespersonstrust in coworkers stems from his or her perceptions ofsuch trust-generating qualities of coworkers as integrity,reliability, and competence (Larzelere and Huston 1980;Morgan and Hunt 1994; J. Smith and Barclay 1997). Trustexists when the salesperson believes that coworkers pos-sess these major qualities of trustworthiness and is confi-

    338 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

    TABLE 1Factors Affecting Cooperative Behaviors in Organizations

    Factor Effect Exemplar StudiesRelational factors

    a. Trust (+) Jones and George (1998); McAllister (1995); Ring and Van De Ven (1994)b. Commitment (+) Dwyer, Schurr, and Oh (1987); Morgan and Hunt (1994)c. Value congruence (+) Chatman (1991); McAllister (1995); Morgan and Hunt (1994)d. Group homogeneity (+) Chatman and Barsade (1995); Kidwell and Bennett (1993)e. Communication quality (+) J. Anderson and Narus (1990); J. Smith and Barclay (1997)f. Communication frequency, modality, direction, and content McAllister (1995); Mohr and Nevin (1990)g. Past opportunistic behaviors of coworkers () McAllister (1995); Morgan and Hunt (1994)h. Anticipated future interactions with coworkers (+) Heide and Miner (1992); Kelley and Thibaut (1978)i. Expectations regarding future behaviors of coworkers Seabright (1993); Wiener and Doescher (1994)

    Task factorsa. Task interdependence (+) Van De Ven, Delbecq, and Koenig (1976); Wageman and Baker (1997)b. Goal interdependence (+) Tjosvold (1984); Tjosvold and Tsao (1989)c. Outcome interdependence (+) Deutsch (1973); Guzzo and Shea (1992)d. Task complexity (+) Van De Ven et al. (1976); Wageman (1995)e. Costs/benefits of cooperation () Deutsch (1973); Wiener and Doescher (1991)f. Task identifiability/visibility (+) George (1992); Wagner (1995)g. Personal accountability (+) Kidwell and Bennett (1993); Wagner (1995)

    Organizational factorsa. Organizational design and structure Chatman and Barsade (1995); Pinto, Pinto, and Prescott (1993)b. Organizational culture Chatman and Barsade (1995); J. Smith and Barclay (1993)c. Reward system Axelrod (1984); Drago and Turnbull (1991); Petersen (1992)d. Sales force control system E. Anderson and Oliver (1987)e. Leadership style Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Bommer (1996)f. Organizational rules and procedures Galbraith and Nathanson (1978); Moenart and Souder (1990)g. Turnover rate () Kidwell and Bennett (1993); Spicer (1985)h. Accessibility of coworkers (+) Keller and Holland (1983); Pinto et al. (1993)i. Number of coworkers () Steiner (1972); Wagner (1995)

    Personal factorsa. Collectivist orientation (+) Jones and George (1998); Wagner (1995)b. Personal cooperativeness (+) Argyle (1991); Chatman and Barsade (1995)c. Agreeableness (+) Chatman and Barsade (1995)d. Extraversion (+) Thorne (1987)e. External locus of control (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Vancouver and Ilgen (1989)f. Need for social approval (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Hui and Villareal (1989)g. Social competence (+) Argyle (1991); Dodge (1985)h. Empathy (+) Eisenberg and Miller (1987)i. Positive past experience in teams (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Loher, Vancouver, and Chajka (1994)j. Self-efficacy for teamwork (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Paulhus (1983)k. Age (+) Argyle (1991)l. Gender Colman (1982)m. Education (+) Burke, McKeen, and McKenna (1990)n. Organizational tenure (+) Pullins, Fine, and Warren (1996)

    NOTE: Those factors for which the direction of effect was not shown in the table are either higher order, general factors that may influence cooperation indifferent ways through their various dimensions (e.g., leadership style) or categorical variables (e.g., gender).

  • dent that they will be reflected in future behaviors ofcoworkers. Confidence is crucial because this is whatcauses the most important outcome of trusting relation-ships: the willingness to rely on the words, actions, anddecisions of the other party (McAllister 1995:25). Trustreduces perceived uncertainty, facilitates risk-taking be-havior, and fosters a cooperative and/or constructive orien-tation (Mayer et al. 1995; Moorman, Deshpande, andZaltman 1993; Morgan and Hunt 1994). Consistent withits properties, several authors have posited trust as an im-mediate antecedent of cooperation (e.g., Jones and George1998; Ring and Van De Ven 1994) and as a key mediatingconstruct between various relational factors and coopera-tion (Morgan and Hunt 1994).

    Hypothesis 2: Trust in coworkers and salesperson coop-eration are positively related.

    Trust facilitates organizational commitment. Relation-ships with peers, especially the degree and quality of so-cialization with coworkers, are among the primary driversof commitment to the organization (Hunt, Chonko, andWood 1985; Mottaz 1988). High levels of interpersonaltrust allow mutual respect to prevail, reduce the complex-ity of organizational life, enable organizational members

    to develop positive affective responses, and therefore fa-cilitate organizational commitment (Nyhan 1999). Thus, apositive relationship between trust in coworkers and orga-nizational commitment is expected. In support of thisview, Hrebiniak and Alutto (1972) find trust among newemployees as positively related to the subsequent develop-ment of organizational commitment, J. Cook and Wall(1980) report strong correlations between various dimen-sions of trust in peers and organizational commitment, andMorgan and Hunt (1994) find trust to influence relation-ship commitment.

    Hypothesis 3: Trust in coworkers and salesperson orga-nizational commitment are positively related.

    Intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfaction. Empirical stud-ies in sales force contexts show that job satisfaction andseveral forms of cooperative and/or constructive behav-iors, such as peer mentoring (Pullins et al. 1996) and orga-nizational citizenship (Netemeyer et al. 1997), arepositively related. Similarly, Argyle (1991) notes that jobsatisfaction is higher in cooperative groups. While expla-nations for the relationship between job satisfaction andvarious forms of cooperative and/or constructive behav-iors are based on the premise that those who are satisfied

    Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 339

    H10(+)Intrinsic

    JobSatisfaction

    ExtrinsicJob

    Satisfaction

    SharedValues

    PastOpportunistic

    Behaviors

    CommunicationQuality

    Trustin

    Co-workers

    OrganizationalCommitment

    PersonalCooperativeness Age Education

    Tenurein

    Organization

    TaskInterdependence

    CollectivistOrganizational

    Norms

    FinancialRewards

    NonfinancialRewards

    SalespersonCooperation

    Numberof

    Co-workers

    H11(+) H12(+)H13(+)

    H14(-)

    H1(+)

    H2(+)

    H15(+)

    H3(+)

    H4(+)

    H5(+)

    H6(+)

    H7(+)

    H8(-)

    H9(+)

    FIGURE 1Structural Model of Salesperson Cooperation

    NOTE: Relational factors: Trust in Coworkers, Organizational Commitment, Communication Quality, Past Opportunistic Behaviors of Coworkers,Shared Values With Coworkers, Intrinsic Job Satisfaction, Extrinsic Job Satisfaction. Task factor: Task Interdependence. Organizational factors: FinancialRewards, Nonfinancial Rewards, Collectivist Organizational Norms, Number of Coworkers. Personal factors: Personal Cooperativeness, Age, Education,Tenure in Organization.

  • with their jobs will respond in reciprocation to those whohave contributed to their positive job experience, whetherthis relationship is direct or mediated by organizationalcommitment, or both, is still an issue that warrants furtherresearch (cf. Tompson and Werner 1997). Much researchhas found a positive and strong relationship between jobsatisfaction and organizational commitment (e.g.,Johnston, Parasuraman, Futrell, and Black 1990). Further-more, the preponderance of empirical and conceptual evi-dence (see Brown and Peterson 1993) suggests thatsatisfaction precedes organizational commitment causallyin sales force settings because it is more specific, less sta-ble, and more rapidly formed (MacKenzie et al.1998:90). Therefore, we suggest that the satisfaction-cooperation relationship is mediated by organizationalcommitment.

    We further distinguish between the intrinsic and extrin-sic aspects of job satisfaction. The former refers to an em-ployees satisfaction with the specific nature of the jobitself, while the latter concerns those aspects of the job thatare outside the specific scope but still within the generalcontext of the job (Lucas, Parasuraman, Davis, and Enis1987). Major components of (1) intrinsic job satisfactioninclude the joy of actually performing the job, feelings ofaccomplishment received from the job, and the degree offreedom in the job and of (2) extrinsic job satisfaction in-clude fair pay, financial earnings, work conditions, andbenefit plans (Lucas et al. 1987).

    Hypothesis 4: Intrinsic job satisfaction and salespersonorganizational commitment are positively related.

    Hypothesis 5: Extrinsic job satisfaction and salespersonorganizational commitment are positively related.

    Shared values with coworkers. Shared values are de-fined as the extent to which [organizational members]have beliefs in common about what behaviors, goals, andpolicies are important or unimportant, appropriate or inap-propriate, and right or wrong (Morgan and Hunt1994:25). The relationship between shared values and de-velopment of commitment and trust is well documented inthe marketing (Dwyer et al. 1987; Morgan and Hunt 1994)and organizational behavior literatures (Chatman 1991).Shared values positively influence organizational commit-ment because salespeople sharing values with coworkerscan be expected to develop stronger affinities with theiroverall organization. Similarly, shared values positivelyinfluence trust in coworkers because, as Brewer (1979) ob-serves, individuals tend to perceive socially dissimilar in-dividuals as dishonest, untrustworthy, and uncooperative.

    Hypothesis 6: Shared values with coworkers and sales-person organizational commitment are positivelyrelated.

    Hypothesis 7: Shared values with coworkers and sales-person trust in coworkers are positively related.

    Past opportunistic behaviors of coworkers. Empiricalevidence on trust in working relationships suggests thatpeople, when assessing competence and trustworthiness,consider whether partners have carried out role-related re-sponsibilities reliably (J. Cook and Wall 1980). Coworkerswho carry out role responsibilities reliably and in a mannerconsistent with norms of fairness and reciprocity will en-hance partners assessments of their trustworthiness(McAllister 1995). In contrast, when coworkers engage inopportunistic behaviors, which Williamson (1975) definesas self interest seeking with guile (p. 6) and which John(1984) characterizes as deceitful violations of appropriaterole behavior, the subsequent level of trust placed in co-workers will decrease.

    Hypothesis 8: Past opportunistic behaviors of coworkersand salesperson trust in coworkers are negatively re-lated.

    Communication quality. Prior research has focused ontwo general aspects of the communication process:(1) mechanistic aspects such as frequency, modality, di-rection, and content (e.g., Churchill, Ford, and Walker1976; Mohr and Nevin 1990) and (2) qualitative aspects(e.g., E. Anderson and Weitz 1989; J. Anderson and Narus1990). Consistent with much research on trusting relation-ships (e.g., Morgan and Hunt 1994; J. Smith and Barclay1997), we limit our discussion to the qualitative aspects ofthe communication process among salespeople.

    Communication quality is defined as timely and accu-rate sharing of information through both formal and infor-mal means (E. Anderson and Weitz 1989; J. Anderson andNarus 1990; Morgan and Hunt 1994; J. Smith and Barclay1997). The timely and accurate sharing of information al-lows salespeople to be more confident in their attributionsregarding the trustworthiness of coworkers and enablesthem to better assess the motives and intentions behind theactions of coworkers (Boorom, Goolsby, and Ramsey1998). Thus, communication quality results in increasedtrust (Mayer et al. 1995).

    Hypothesis 9: Communication quality with coworkersand salesperson trust in coworkers are positivelyrelated.

    Task Factors

    Ever since Morton Deutsch published his theory ofcooperation in 1949, task factors have been the most com-monly used explanatory variables in cooperation research.Deutschs theory viewed cooperation as a form of socialinteraction that can be characterized by perceptions ofpositive interdependence. That is, Deutsch (1949, 1973,1980) argued that individuals will be more likely to coop-erate if they view (1) one anothers goals as (positively)related and (2) task characteristics as requiring coop-

    340 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

  • eration to achieve those goals (Tjosvold 1984, 1986). Thisnotion of interdependence, further developed by Deutschand Krauss (1960) and Thompson (1967), has resulted inthe extensive interest in structural factors, especially intask factors, among researchers investigating cooperativerelationships. Variables such as task complexity, taskinterdependence, and outcome and goal interdependencehave been posited as key explanatory factors in studies ofcooperation (Kumar, Scheer, and Steenkamp 1995a,1995b; Tjosvold 1984, 1986; Wageman 1995; Wagemanand Baker 1997). Another research stream has investi-gated task characteristics in the context of free riding andsocial loafing. Findings reveal that identifiability of indi-vidual contributions to the task at hand and personalaccountability influence the degree of within-group coop-eration (Kidwell and Bennett 1993; Wagner 1995), espe-cially in reciprocal task-flow situations (i.e., when eachperson acts on the output of the other).

    Consistent with Deutschs theory, we posit that task in-terdependence, defined as the extent to which salespersonsdepend on one another for information and aid to ac-complish their tasks and improve their performance(Thompson 1967), will have a direct and positive effect onsalesperson cooperation. However, Deutsch viewed inter-dependence as central, or even equivalent, to cooperationother factors affecting cooperation can do so only indirectlythrough their impact on perceptions of interdependence(Tjosvold 1986). Hence, for example, trust and commit-ment can have no direct effect on cooperation in Deutschstheory but can only exert indirect influence by magnifyingperceived interdependence. In contrast, the perspectivetaken in the present study is that variables from each of themajor antecedent categories exert direct influence oncooperation.

    Hypothesis 10: Task interdependence and salespersoncooperation are positively related.

    Organizational Factors

    The structural, cultural, managerial, and proceduraldimensions of the organization have long been thought toaffect cooperative tendencies among organizational mem-bers (Mintzberg 1979; Shapiro 1977). Within this context,variables such as physical proximity of participants andtheir opportunity to interact (Wagner 1995), organiza-tional cultural norms (Moch and Seashore 1981), leader-ship style (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Bommer 1996),and the degree to which organizational control systemsreward cooperative efforts versus individual achievement(E. Anderson and Oliver 1987; Petersen 1992) have beenshown to influence cooperative and/or constructive orga-nizational behaviors. Incorporating organizational factorsinto models explaining cooperation is important becausethey provide managers with actionable guidance on how to

    develop and maintain cooperative organizational systems(Pinto et al. 1993).

    Three specific organizational factors are hypothesizedin the present study to influence salesperson cooperation:collectivist organizational norms, reward system, andnumber of coworkers. These three variables are thought torepresent major structural, cultural, and proceduraldimensions of the organization affecting cooperative ten-dencies in our sampling context. Research about pro-social organizational behaviors indicates that several man-agerial variables, particularly leadership style and leaderbehaviors, may also influence cooperative tendencies inorganizations (Podsakoff et al. 1996). The rationale for thepotent effects of leadership variables is based on the mod-eling theories in K. Smith et al.s (1995) review of thecooperation literature. Based on this view, a sales managercan promote cooperation among salespeople by (1) actingas a role model and/or (2) communicating the appropri-ate behavioral patterns in the form of guiding principles(Larson and LaFasto 1989), which further contribute to thedevelopment of organizational norms. The former processinvolves imitation of the leaders behaviors and thereforeis unlikely to bear a substantive effect in our sampling con-text (i.e., a commission-based, retail selling context wheresalespeople work in a relatively independent manner). Thepotential effects of the latter process is captured largely bythe collectivist organizational norms variable that we dis-cuss next.

    Collectivist organizational (cultural) norms. An orga-nizations internal culture is an important determinant ofhow organizational members interact with each other(Deshpande, Farley, and Webster 1993). Socially sharedrules and acceptable forms of behaviors within an organi-zation, commonly labeled as organizational (cultural)norms, tend to limit the variation across behaviors of orga-nizational members by suppressing or supporting certaintypes of behaviors (Moch and Seashore 1981). As such,the norms embedded in the internal culture of an organiza-tion prescribe behavioral patterns (Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn,Snoek, and Rosenthal 1964). One important dimension oforganizational culture closely relevant to cooperativework environments is the extent to which collectivist ver-sus individualistic norms are embedded within the organi-zations culture (Chatman and Barsade 1995).

    Individualism-collectivism, as a determinant of coop-eration, has been studied at societal (e.g., Hofstede 1980),individual (e.g., Eby and Dobbins 1997), and organiza-tional (e.g., Chatman and Barsade 1995; Earley 1993) lev-els. As to organizational cultures, individualism-collectivism captures the relative importance organiza-tional members give to the interests of a larger workgroup(i.e., coworkers) as opposed to personal interests (Wagnerand Moch 1986). Specifically, collectivist organizationalcultures encourage the subordination of personal interests

    Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 341

  • to the goals of a larger work group and, therefore, put moreemphasis on sharing, cooperation, and harmony (Wagner1995).

    Hypothesis 11: Collectivist organizational norms andsalesperson cooperation are positively related.

    Reward system. The motivation literature maintainsthat financial rewards (e.g., compensation plans, bonuses,profit sharing plans) and nonfinancial rewards (e.g., hon-ors, opportunities for personal growth, job security, pro-motion) influence the behaviors of organizationalmembers (Pritchard, Jones, Roth, Stuebing, and Ekeberg1988). We define reward system in this study as the degreeto which rewards in the organization, both financial andnonfinancial, encourage cooperation among salespeople.

    Petersen (1992) notes that managers should carefullydesign reward systems if certain types of behavioral pat-terns, such as cooperation, are to be developed. Axelrod(1984) suggests that cooperation can be reinforced bymaking cooperative behaviors more attractive through theusage of rewards. Research on team effectiveness showsthat when rewards are linked to group performance, a re-ward system that Campion, Medsker, and Higgs (1993) re-fer to as interdependent rewards and Guzzo and Shea(1992) refer to as outcome interdependence, group per-formance is facilitated through increased motivation to-ward group-oriented behaviors. Finally, J. Anderson andNarus (1990) and Wiener and Doescher (1991) note thatindividuals will be more likely to cooperate if they believethat the outcome of cooperation is going to be positive. In-deed, the supposed relationship between financial rewardsand all individual behaviors is so strong in the motivationliterature that including financial rewards as an antecedentto cooperation may be considered a control variable. Thatis, once one controls for financial rewards, do other factorsexplain variance in individual cooperation?

    Hypothesis 12: The degree to which financial rewardsencourage cooperative behaviors is positively re-lated to salesperson cooperation.

    Hypothesis 13: The degree to which nonfinancial re-wards encourage cooperative behaviors is positivelyrelated to salesperson cooperation.

    Number of coworkers. Research on work groups hasposited group size as an important predictor of within-group cooperation (Hechter 1987; Wagner 1995). Becauseindividuals workplace behaviors and incremental taskcontributions are easier to assess, more visible, and/oridentifiable in small groups, people in such groups tendto (1) avoid free riding and social loafing and (2) displaycooperative and/or constructive behaviors (George 1992).Furthermore, Pinto et al. (1993) argue that physical prox-imity and accessibility of organizational members may

    promote cooperative behaviors by making them morefeasible.

    Hypothesis 14: The number of coworkers is negativelyrelated to salesperson cooperation.

    Personal Factors

    Some people are simply more cooperative than others(Argyle 1991). An individuals disposition to behavecooperatively may stem from such personal factors as per-sonality traits (Baron 1983) and demographic characteris-tics (Argyle 1991). For example, Baron (1983) distin-guishes between cooperators, competitors, andindividualists as personality types. Cooperators preferto work in close collaboration with other people and areprimarily interested in the achievement of group objec-tives. Competitors put more emphasis on their personalgoals. Individualists will either cooperate or compete,depending on which best fits their personal needs.

    Researchers have used several personality measures asproxies for personal cooperativeness. Examples includecollectivist orientation (Wagner 1995), agreeableness(Chatman and Barsade 1995), extraversion (Thorne 1987),locus of control and need for social approval (Eby andDobbins 1997), social competence (Dodge 1985), andempathy (Eisenberg and Miller 1987). In addition,although empirical evidence is scant, such demographicvariables as age, gender, education, and tenure in the orga-nization have been proposed as predictors of cooperativedispositions (Argyle 1991; Lu and Argyle 1991; Wagner1995). We focus on personal cooperativeness and severaldemographic variables.

    Personal cooperativeness. Personal cooperativeness,as examined here, is a personality trait that determines thepredisposition of an individual toward working in closecollaboration with others in all life activities. A salesper-son high in this trait

    places priority on associating with others for mutualbenefits, gaining social approval, and working to-gether with others toward a common end or purpose,while a person with low disposition to cooperateplaces priority on maximizing his or her own wel-fare regardless of others welfare. (Chatman andBarsade 1995:424)

    Hypothesis 15: The personality trait of cooperativenessand salespersons cooperative behaviors are posi-tively related.

    Demographic differences. While it has been argued thatdemographic differences are indicators of several driversof cooperative behaviors, such as empathy and perspectivetaking (e.g., Davis 1983), several decades of researchhave, in fact, failed to yield conclusive evidence regarding

    342 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

  • the effects of demographic variables on cooperative and/orconstructive tendencies (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Paine,and Bachrach 2000). Concerning the impact of age, for in-stance, Wagner (1995) reports a positive and significantcorrelation between age and cooperative behaviors, whileLu and Argyle (1991) report a negative correlation. Simi-larly, some studies report significant effects of experience,education, and organizational tenure (e.g., Kidwell andBennett 1993; Pullins et al. 1996; Spicer 1985), andyet others fail to support the view that these variablesare substantively important predictors of cooperationespecially when personality differences are accounted for(Argyle 1991). Given that the literature does not allow usto specify directional hypotheses, we examine the effectsof age, education level, and organizational tenure from anexploratory perspective.

    METHOD

    The research setting involved mail surveys of salespeo-ple and sales managers from new-car automobile dealer-ships. Salespeople from the participating dealerships wereasked to respond to self-administered questionnaires inwhich they were instructed to state their opinions regard-ing their coworkers, defined as other salespersons workingin the same dealership. While several more cooperativeselling contexts (such as those that apply team selling)exist, new-car salespeople represent a pertinent sample forour research for several reasons. First, contrary to the ste-reotype image of the automobile salesperson, cooperativeselling is a rapidly growing practice in this industry. Inresponse to the competition from the Internet and thedemands of the manufacturer firms, many dealershipshave initiated relationship marketing and customer reten-tion programs. Mixed compensation plans (as opposed tofull-commission plans), formal or informal commissionsharing, and year-end bonuses and several forms of manu-facturer incentives based on overall dealership perfor-mance are common practices. Thus, it is not only the casethat some reasonable level of cooperation exists amongnew-car salespeople but also many dealership managersconsider such cooperation desirable for the performanceof the overall firm. Our preliminary interviews with deal-ership managers and salespeople and the data we collectedfor the present research support this view, as we demon-strate in the following sections.

    Second, note that our purpose at this initial stage of the-ory testing is to explain variance and explore relationships.Since sales teams are usually composed of people fromdifferent functional areas and with diverse backgrounds(Weitz and Bradford 1999), using such a diverse samplewould have decreased our ability to explore the true natureof the relationships due to substantial amount of extrane-ous variation that cannot be modeled directly. Third, new-

    car salespeople have relatively similar task requirements,which eliminates such concerns as cooperate in whatmanner? and enables a consistent operational definitionfor the cooperation construct. Fourth, the dealerships inour sample are relatively small organizations (a majorityof them employ less than 10 salespeople), which mini-mizes the possibility of confusion on the part of therespondents as to the question of cooperate with whom?Finally, the fact that our sample is drawn from what is gen-erally considered to be a relatively competitive sellingcontext facilitates a strong test of our thesis that each of thefour main antecedent categories exerts a significant anddistinct influence on salesperson cooperation.

    Data Collection

    Preliminary investigation. The study began with un-structured field interviews with managers and salespeoplefrom four local dealerships. The purpose of the interviewswith managers was to explore whether sales managers inthis sales context regarded salesperson cooperation as im-portant. All four dealership sales managers maintainedthat they wanted their salespeople to cooperate with eachother because they believed such cooperation increasedoverall sales force performance. These interviews alsoprovided useful insights for developing the specific tasksfor measuring the cooperation construct. The interviewswith salespeople provided an on-site pretest of the ques-tionnaire. Ten salespeople from the same four dealershipscommented on items and suggested changes. The finaldraft of the questionnaire was developed after making therequired modifications.

    Sampling procedure. A sample frame of 1,181 new-cardealerships in the state of Texas was developed from amailing list provided by an independent research firm.Dealership sales managers were contacted by mail to so-licit their cooperation in return for the summary of results.One hundred and sixty-five dealerships agreed to partici-pate in the study, providing access to 1,975 salespeople.These dealership managers also responded to a short ques-tionnaire designed to measure several organizational-levelvariables. These variables include number of vehicles soldper year, number of employees, number of salespeople,perceived overall degree of cooperation within the salesforce, and importance of cooperation. Ninety percent ofresponses to the question How important is it for the suc-cess of your dealership that salespersons cooperate witheach other? were above the midpoint of the scale, rangingfrom 1 (very unimportant) to 7 (very important).3

    Four weeks after the initial mailing, the salespersonquestionnaires were mailed to the managers of the 165participating dealerships for distribution to their salespeo-ple. Each questionnaire packet also included a cover letterexplaining the purpose of the study and return envelopes to

    Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 343

  • assure respondent anonymity. Five hundred and eighty-five individual salesperson responses from 112 differentdealerships were received. After the elimination of care-less respondents and a listwise deletion of missing cases,531 questionnaires were retained, resulting in an effectiveresponse rate of 27 percent. The mean within-dealershipresponse rate was 50 percent.

    Nonresponse bias. Tests for nonresponse bias rely onArmstrong and Overtons (1977) argument that late re-spondents are similar to nonrespondents (in comparison toearly respondents). Two different tests were conducted:one for the first sampling stage (dealership managers) andone for the second sampling stage (salespeople). For deal-ership managers, we compared late and early respondentson the means of two critical variables, namely, perceivedoverall degree of cooperation within the salesforce and im-portance of cooperation. For individual salespersons, wecompared the two groups on the covariance matrix of con-struct items (Morrison 1976). No significant differenceswere found in either of the tests, suggesting thatnonresponse bias may not be a problem.

    Sample characteristics. Our sampling process resultedin a sample that varied greatly on both dealership andsalesperson characteristics. The dealerships vary in size asmeasured by number of employees (M 40, SD = 49.16),salespeople (M 12, SD = 9.5), and vehicles sold per year(M 943, SD = 937.5). Individual respondents vary widelyin age (M = 39.26 years, SD = 11.49), sales experience(M = 10.65 years, SD = 9.78), organizational tenure (M =2.57 years, SD = 3.34), and education ( high school di-ploma, 18.15%; some college, 52.45%; college graduate,20.33%; graduate work, 9.07%). Most of the respondentsare male (90.91%) and full-commission salespeople(69.78%).

    Measures

    Constructs are measured using multiple-item mea-sures, whenever applicable. All scales use a 7-point scal-ing format with anchors strongly disagree to stronglyagree, unless otherwise noted. Measurement items areprovided in the appendix. The reliabilities of the multiple-item, reflective measures are presented in Table 2. Thecoefficient alphas, Lisrel-based internal consistency esti-mates (i.e., composite reliability), and the amount of vari-ance captured by each construct in relation to measure-ment error (i.e., average variance extracted) are wellbeyond the acceptable threshold levels suggested byNunnally (1978) and Fornell and Larcker (1981).

    Cooperation. For the sake of operational andnomological clarity, we limit the domain of the coopera-tion construct to cooperative behaviors that represent the

    core task of our respondents, that is, automobile selling.Thus, our conceptualization of salesperson cooperation,based on the work of Laughlin (1978) and Morgan andHunt (1994), requires a measure capturing various formsof task-specific cooperative behaviors that respondents arelikely to display toward their coworkers. Both in-role andextrarole task-specific behaviors (i.e., those that includeand transcend beyond what is formally prescribed by asalespersons organizational role) belong to the domain ofcooperation.

    Measurement items are developed through an interac-tive process with dealership managers and salespeoplewho participated in our preliminary interviews. Theseinformants provided us with valuable insights concerning(1) the nature of cooperation in automobile selling, (2) spe-cific types of cooperative behaviors in various stages of theselling process, and (3) clarity and completeness of theitems in the measure. Relatively higher emphasis is givenin the scale to cooperative behaviors involving relation-ships with customers (e.g., sharing information aboutpotential and current customers, helping one anotherscustomers, etc.), based on the unanimous agreementamong our informants that customer-related cooperationis of critical importance for the success of selling effortsand most representative of a cooperative sales force. Otherfacets of salesperson cooperation frequently mentioned bythe informants include assisting coworkers during salespresentations, sharing information about vehicle specifics,and providing support in terms of activities that facilitatethe selling process (e.g., handling of paperwork). Respon-dents rated the extent to which they engage in each type ofcooperative behavior on a 7-point scoring format, rangingfrom very little to very much.

    Trust in coworkers and organizational commitment.The scale in Morgan and Hunt (1994) is used for measur-ing trust in coworkers. Based on the Dyadic Trust Scale ofLarzelere and Huston (1980), this measure captures re-spondents confidence in the integrity, reliability, compe-tence, and general trustworthiness of relationship partners.An additional item, I consider my coworkers as peoplewhom I would be willing to let make important job-relateddecisions without my involvement, was included to putmore emphasis on the competence dimension. Organiza-tional Commitment is measured using the nine-item ver-sion of Mowday, Steers, and Porters (1979) Organiza-tional Commitment Scale, which has been used exten-sively in prior research (Mathieu and Zajac 1990).

    Measures of exogenous constructs. Shared Values WithCoworkers and Past Opportunistic Behaviors of Cowork-ers use the scales in Morgan and Hunt (1994). The assess-ment of shared values involves a two-stage procedure (cf.Enz 1988): respondents are asked to state the degree towhich (1) they agree and (2) their coworkers would agree

    344 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

  • TABLE 2Descriptive Statistics for the Scales, Reliability Estimates,a and Latent Factor Correlations

    Composite VarianceScale M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Reliability Extracted

    1. Cooperation 5.38 1.18 .87 .87 .632. Organizational Commitment 5.72 1.13 .31 .91 .89 .783. Trust 4.73 1.37 .39 .41 .95 .95 .844. Intrinsic Job Satisfaction 5.70 1.08 .39 .78 .45 .85 .86 .705. Extrinsic Job Satisfaction 5.20 1.35 .23 .61 .36 .61 .89 .89 .846. Shared Values 6.16 1.06 .20 .23 .47 .31 .28 .86 .87 .707. Opportunistic Behaviors 3.61 1.62 .19 .22 .54 .31 .32 .40 .87 .89 .818. Communication Quality 5.10 1.18 .45 .31 .52 .40 .31 .28 .38 .89 .89 .849. Task Interdependence 4.97 1.35 .49 .34 .25 .36 .18 .11 .04 .36 NA NA NA

    10. Collectivist Organizational Norms 5.34 1.15 .41 .44 .44 .42 .30 .27 .20 .34 .49 .84 .85 .6611. Financial Rewards 5.22 1.60 .44 .40 .39 .42 .41 .24 .26 .42 .28 .43 NA NA NA12. Nonfinancial Rewards 4.99 1.62 .38 .37 .37 .33 .30 .16 .21 .42 .22 .39 .61 NA NA NA13. Numbers of Coworkers 16.7 11.4 .08 .05 .05 .09 .03 .01 .05 .03 .08 .02 .02 .08 NA NA NA14. Personal Cooperativeness 5.17 0.96 .52 .25 .28 .30 .13 .01 .11 .31 .35 .37 .37 .33 .00 .74 .77 .6515. Age 39.3 11.5 .04 .03 .06 .17 .16 .15 .17 .04 .04 .03 .05 .04 .03 .07 NA NA NA16. Tenure in Organization 2.57 3.34 .04 .05 .02 .02 .07 .04 .01 .00 .07 .01 .05 .02 .07 .04 .32 NA NA NA17. Education .05 .09 .04 .13 .11 .06 .08 .05 .04 .01 .16 .04 .07 .07 .07 .01 NA NA NA

    NOTE: Discriminant validity is obtained if the variance extracted for a construct is greater than the squared latent factor correlation between a pair of constructs. NA = not applicable because the construct was mea-sured with a formative scale or had fewer than three items.a. Coefficient alphas are reported on the diagonal.

    345

  • with five statements concerning ethical values. The differ-ences between the two responses (subtracted from 7) arethen used to reflect shared values. For opportunistic be-haviors, we added the following item to the original three-item scale: my coworkers avoid fulfilling their responsibil-ities unless they are watched closely.

    Selected items from the marketing practitioners JobSatisfaction Scale of Hunt and Chonko (1984) and thesalesperson Intrinsic Job Satisfaction Scale of Lucas et al.(1987) are used to measure intrinsic aspects of therepondents job satisfaction. Extrinsic Job Satisfactionitems are drawn from Lucas et al.s (1987) study. Items inboth scales come from the Job Dimensions Scale (Groves1981; Schletzer 1965). Similarly, for CommunicationQuality, we use selected items from the CommunicationQuality Scales in Morgan and Hunt (1994) and J. Smithand Barclay (1997). Both scales measure the degree oftimely and accurate sharing of information, and both arebased on the Communication/Participation/FeedbackScale of E. Anderson, Lodish, and Weitz (1987).

    Reward System, the degree to which the rewards in theorganization encourage (discourage) cooperation betweensalespeople, is operationalized for both financial rewardsand nonfinancial rewards. Single items for both dimen-sions are developed to assess the degree to which suchrewards in the dealership favor cooperative behaviors. A7-point scoring format ranging from strongly discouragecooperation to strongly encourage cooperation is used.For Collectivist Norms embedded within the culture of theorganization, we use the Norms subscale of Individualism-Collectivism, developed in Wagner and Moch (1986) andfurther validated in Wagner (1995). Items of the originalscale were modified slightly to assess organizational-levelcultural norms.

    For Task Interdependence, we use the three-item TaskInterdependence Scale in Campion et al. (1993), whichmeasures the degree to which respondents depend on eachother to accomplish their tasks and improve their perfor-mance. While the third item in the scale is a direct measureof interdependence, the first two items tap the degree ofinterdependence from a dyadic perspective in that the firstitem is a measure of the respondents dependence oncoworkers and the second item is a measure of the respon-dents perception of coworkersdependence on him or her.For this reason, responses to the first two items are firstaveraged and then combined with the third item to gener-ate a task interdependence score for each respondent.

    Finally, Personal Cooperativeness is measured usingitems from the Work-Cooperativeness Scale of Lu andArgyle (1991), the School-Cooperativeness Scale of Rob-erts (1991), and the Acceptance of Cooperation/Teamwork Scale of Oliver and Anderson (1994). Thesescales have been used to determine manifest personalitydifferences across individuals in terms of cooperative

    versus competitive behavioral dispositions in specificenvironments. Wordings of the items borrowed from eachscale are altered slightly to develop a measure of GeneralCooperativeness that would apply in all environmentswork, school, family, and so on. Thus, as a significant dif-ference from the Cooperation Scale, which is limited totask-specific cooperative behaviors directed towardcoworkers, items in the Personal Cooperativeness Scalemeasure a salespersons predisposition toward working inclose collaboration with others in general.

    Measure Purification and Validation

    Following the two-step procedure recommended byJ. Anderson and Gerbing (1988), we estimate andrespecify the measurement model prior to incorporatingthe structural restrictions. Maximum-likelihood LISREL 8(Jreskog and Srbom 1993) is used in the analyses, andthe sample covariance matrix is used as input.4 In addition,because some of the scales in this research are either com-pletely new (e.g., Cooperation) or composed of selecteditems from previously used scales (e.g., Intrinsic Job Satis-faction), it is reasonable to anticipate that several itemswill have to be dropped during respecification of the mea-surement model. Cross validation is recommended forsuch measure purification processes to minimize errorprobability and capitalization on chance. Accordingly,responses were randomly split into two halves so as tocross validate the measurement model.

    The initial model, which consisted of all 78 measure-ment items and 17 factors, was estimated using the firstsplit sample. However, several items had high standard-ized residuals and modification indices, making the modelfit not acceptable: 2(2,796) = 5,362, Comparative Fit Index(CFI) = .82, Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI) = .66, root meansquare error of approximation (RMSEA) = .058, standard-ized root mean square residual (SRMR) = .067. Werespecified the model by eliminating three items from theIntrinsic Job Satisfaction Scale, four items from ExtrinsicJob Satisfaction, three from Organizational Commitment,four from Cooperation, two from Trust, three from Com-munication Quality, one from Opportunistic Behaviors,and four from Personal Cooperativeness. Considering thelarge number of constructs and items, the respecifiedmodel fits the data well, 2(1,248) = 2002.7, CFI = .91, GFI =.88, RMSEA = .046, SRMR = .049.5

    Next, we tested the respecified model on the secondsplit sample. The resulting fit indices indicate that themeasurement model has a good fit to the data. While theGFI is an acceptable .88, the RMSEA value of .044 and theSRMR value of .046 indicate a very good model fit. Simi-larly, in terms of incremental fit, the CFI for the model is.93, which exceeds the recommended .90 acceptance crite-rion (R. McDonald and Marsh 1990). The fit of the model

    346 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

  • is even better when it is estimated using the full sample,2(1,248) = 2,420, CFI = .93, GFI = .88, RMSEA = .041,SRMR = .044. In addition, all items load significantly ontheir respective constructs (with the lowest t-value being11.1), providing support for the convergent validity ofmeasurement items.

    Unidimensionality and discriminant validity. Proce-dures for examining the measurement scales forunidimensionality are based on exploratory and confirma-tory factor analyses of scale items, taken one scale at atime, to see if the items in each scale share a single underly-ing factor. Exploratory factor analyses reveal that only onefactor accounts for a major portion of the total variance ineach scale (i.e., only one factor is extracted using aneigenvalue of 1.0 as the cutoff point). Similarly, thegoodness-of-fit indices obtained from one-factor confir-matory factor analyses of the scales are all acceptable (i.e.,GFI > .90, CFI > .90).

    Tests for discriminant validity are based on compari-sons of the chi-square statistics obtained from confirma-tory factor analyses of pairwise combinations of the studyconstructs when the correlation between the constructs are(1) constrained to unity and (2) freed for estimation. A sig-nificantly lower chi-square value for the unconstrainedmodel indicates that the two constructs are distinct.Discriminant validity is obtained for all the study con-structs using this test (2[1] > 3.84 for all pairwise compar-isons), as well as the more stringent procedure suggestedby Fornell and Larcker (1981) (see Table 2).

    RESULTS

    Descriptive statistics for the scales are provided inTable 2. The standard deviations indicate a substantialamount of variance in the responses.6 More important, thelarge standard deviations for the three endogenousconstructsCooperation (1.18), Trust in Coworkers(1.37), and Organizational Commitment (1.13)suggestthat each of these constructs has considerable amount ofvariance to be explained. In addition, most means arewithin one-half point of the scale centers. While the meanfor the Cooperation Scale is 5.38, the dispersion of thisvariable is also reasonably high, indicating that the sampleincludes both cooperative and noncooperative respon-dents (13% of the responses are below the center of thescale). Furthermore, the fact that most of the responses areat the higher end of the Cooperation Scale is not unex-pected. Studies on organizational members commonlyreport similar results (e.g., Chatman and Barsade 1995;Eby and Dobbins 1997). One explanation for this patternof results lies in the very notion of the organization.Organizations exist because individuals come together towork for a common purpose. Some level of cooperation is

    therefore necessary for sustained membership in theorganization.

    Table 3 reports goodness-of-fit indices and standard-ized parameter estimates for the structural model. Theoverall chi-square statistic is significant, 2(1,275) = 2530.6,p < .01, as is expected given the large sample size (Bagozziand Yi 1988). All other goodness-of-fit indices are withinthe acceptable ranges (CFI = .93, GFI = .88, RMSEA =.042, SRMR = .051). Taken collectively, these resultsshow that the hypothesized structural relationships fit thedata well. Overall, the hypothesized structural relation-ships explain 45 percent of the observed variance in coop-eration. In addition, 11 of the 15 hypothesized paths aresupported, and at least one factor from each of the fourantecedent categories exerts significant influence on sales-person cooperation.

    Also included in Table 3 are the parameter estimatesand associated test statistics of the hypothesized relation-ships adjusted for common method variance. Given thatthe same informants provided the data for most of theexogenous and endogenous constructs in our model, thepossibility exists that common method variance may haveinflated or deflated the magnitudes of the parameter esti-mates for the hypothesized paths. Thus, it is necessary toassess the degree of this form of bias in our results. Theadjusted estimates in Table 3 are obtained after partialingout the portion of variance that is common across all ourobserved variables obtained from the same source (i.e.,salespeople), using the procedure in MacKenzie,Podsakoff, and Paine (1999).

    As shown in Table 3, the overall pattern of significantrelationships in the sample is not affected much by com-mon method variance. Of the 11 paths that are significantin the unadjusted analysis, 10 are significant in theadjusted analysis, with the path from collectivist organiza-tional norms to cooperation dropping just slightly to thepoint of being nonsignificant at the traditional .05 level.More important, given that the adjusted estimates havemuch greater standard errors because of the inclusion of anadditional common method factor in the model andfewer degrees of freedom, the absolute sizes of the coeffi-cients should be the primary basis of comparison, not thesignificance levels. Note that the magnitudes of theadjusted path coefficients in our results are very close tothe magnitudes of the unadjusted estimates,7 and the corre-lation between the two sets of estimates is .93 (p value .05), thus indicating lack of support for Hypothesis 3a.Regarding the impact of operational compatibility on stra-tegic performance ( = .25, p < .05), the path coefficientis statistically significant; however, the sign is reversed,thereby failing to support Hypothesis 3b.8

    Hypotheses 4a, 5a, and 6a respectively hypothesize thatresource complementarity will positively affect mutualtrust, reciprocal commitment, and bilateral informationexchange. The results reflect interesting differences in theimpact of resource complementarity on various relationship-capital constructs. Contrary to expectations, resourcecomplementarity is not significantly related to trust ( =.07, p > .05) or bilateral information exchange ( = .01, p >0.05), thereby failing to support Hypotheses 4a and 6a.However, resource complementarity has a significant rela-tionship with reciprocal commitment ( = .27, p < .05),supporting Hypothesis 5a.

    Hypotheses 4b, 5b, and 6b respectively suggest thatcultural compatibility enhances relationship capital. Asexpected, cultural compatibility is significantly related tomutual trust ( = .40, p < .05), reciprocal commitment ( =.42, p < .05), and bilateral information exchange ( = .39,p < .05), thus supporting all three hypotheses.

    Hypotheses 4c, 5c, and 6c respectively hypothesize thepositive impact of operational compatibility on trust, com-mitment, and bilateral information exchange. Asexpected, operational compatibility is significantly relatedto trust ( = .37, p < .05), and commitment ( = .19, p .05), therebyfailing to support Hypothesis 6c.

    Hypotheses 7a and 7b state that trust will be positivelyassociated with both project performance and strategicperformance. Trust is significantly related to project per-formance (= .17, p < .05), thereby supporting Hypothesis 7a.However, contrary to expectations, trust is not signifi-cantly related to strategic performance ( = .15, p > .05),

    thereby failing to support Hypothesis 7b. Commitment issignificantly related to both project performance ( = .39,p < .05) and strategic performance ( = .30, p < .05),thereby supporting Hypotheses 8a and 8b. Furthermore,results indicate that reciprocal information exchange is notsignificantly related to project performance ( = .07, p >.05), thereby failing to support Hypothesis 9a. However,reciprocal information exchange is positively related to

    Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 367

    TABLE 1Measurement Model

    Construct Itema Loading

    Resource Both firms needed each others resources to accomplish their goals and responsibilities .66Complementarity The resources contributed by both firms were significant in getting the bid .91

    Resources brought into the venture by each firm were very valuable for the other .93Cultural The organizational values and social norms prevalent in the two firms were congruent .77

    Compatibility Executives from both firms involved in this project had compatible philosophies/approaches to business dealings .76The goals and objectives of both firms were compatible with each other .86The chemistry was right between the two firmsb

    Operational Technical capabilities of the two firms were compatible with each other .89Compatibility The organizational procedures of the two firms were compatible .82

    Employees of both firms had similar professional or trade skills .67Mutual Trust Both firms were generally honest and truthful with each other .83

    Both firms treated each other fairly and justly .80Both firms found it necessary to be cautious in dealing with each other (R) .80Relying on each other was risky for both firms (R)b

    Reciprocal There was frequent communication between the two firms (e.g., visits to each others firms, meetings, writtenInformation and telephone communications) .81Exchange Exchange of information in this relationship took place frequently and informally .91

    Making contact with people from the other firm was hard for both firms (R) .69Decisions regarding the project were made unanimously in joint meetings with managers from both firmsb

    Reciprocal Both firms were willing to dedicate whatever people and resources it took to make this project a success .83Commitment Both firms provided experienced and capable people to the project .90

    Both firms were committed to making this project a success .89Strategic The collaboration provided a very effective medium of learning .68

    Performance Collaborating with this partner was a wise business decision .91Our strategic objectives going into the venture were achieved .85

    Project The owners objectives (in terms of specifications, schedule, quality) were met .68Performance A quality job was done on the project .80

    Overall, the project was efficiently carried out .84The venture was profitable for our firm .71

    NOTE: (R) Indicates items that were reverse-coded.a. Scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).b. Items that were deleted after initial tests.

    TABLE 2Internal Consistency, Square Roots of Average Variance Extracted, and Correlation Matrix

    Construct Internal Consistency 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

    1. Resource Complementarity .88 .842. Cultural Compatibility .84 .26 .803. Operational Compatibility .84 .11 .65 .804. Mutual Trust .85 .22 .66 .64 .815. Bilateral Information Exchange .85 .09 .38 .24 .31 .816. Reciprocal Commitment .91 .40 .61 .49 .48 .37 .877. Project Performance .86 .45 .54 .48 .53 .33 .67 .768. Strategic Performance .84 .29 .50 .17 .23 .38 .50 .38 .82

    NOTE: The diagonal (in italics) shows the square root of the average variance extracted for each construct.

  • strategic performance ( = .18, p < .05), thereby providingsupport for Hypothesis 9b.

    Indirect effects. Preliminary tests suggested that condi-tions stipulated by Baron and Kenny (1986) for a media-tion model were satisfied,9 indicating that relationship-capital variables mediated the relationship between part-ner characteristics and alliance performance. We examinedthe contribution of the relationship-capital variables to theexplanatory power of the model. Specifically, we exam-ined the increase in R2s of the alliance performance con-structs when the relationship-capital variables wereincluded. The R2 of project performance increases from.45 to .55, while that of strategic performance increases

    from .24 to .41. The increases for both project performance,F(3, 61) = 4.52, Fcrit = 2.76, and strategic performance,F(3, 61) = 6.20, Fcrit = 2.76, are significant at p < .05, thusindicating that relationship-capital variables contributesubstantially to the explanatory power of the model.

    Next, based on the approach suggested by Baron andKenny (1986), we assessed the mediation by examiningthe size and significance of the indirect effects. Interest-ingly, for both performance constructs, all three antecedentsnamely, resource complementarity, cultural compatibility,and operational compatibilityhad significant indirecteffects. The standardized effect sizes were small tomedium (J. Cohen 1988), ranging from .06 to .22. Taken in

    368 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

    TABLE 3Effect of Interfirm Diversity on Relationship Capital: Standardized PLSa Coefficients

    Hypothesized StandardizedDependent Variable Independent Variable H0 Sign CoefficientMutual trust (R2 = .51) Resource complementarity H4a + .07

    Cultural compatibility H5a + .40***Operational compatibility H6a + .37***

    Reciprocal commitment (R2 = .45) Resource complementarity H4b + .27***Cultural compatibility H5b + .42***Operational compatibility H6b + .19***

    Bilateral information exchange (R2 = .15) Resource complementarity H4c + .01Cultural compatibility H5c + .39***Operational compatibility H6c + .02

    NOTE: H = hypothesis.a. PLS = Partial Least Squares.*** Denotes significance at p < .05.

    TABLE 4Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects of Interfirm Diversity

    on Performance: Standardized PLSa CoefficientsStandardized Coefficient

    Dependent Variable Independent Variable H0 Hypothesized Sign Direct Indirectb Totalc

    Project performance (R2 = .55) Interfirm DiversityResource complementarity H1a + .22*** .10 .32Cultural compatibility H2a + .03 .22 .22Operational compatibility H3a + .13 .13 .13

    Relationship capitalMutual trust H7a + .17*** NA Reciprocal commitment H8a + .39*** NA .41Bilateral information exchange H9a + .07 NA

    Strategic performance (R2 = .41) Interfirm DiversityResource complementarity H1b + .09 .08 .08Cultural compatibility H2b + .50*** .20 .70Operational compatibility H3b + .25*** .06 .19

    Relationship capitalMutual trust H7b + .15 NA Reciprocal commitment H8b + .30*** NA Bilateral information exchange H9b + .18*** NA .21

    NOTE: NA = not applicable.a. PLS = Partial Least Squares.b. Only statistically significant indirect effects were included in the computation.c. Only statistically significant effects (direct or indirect) were included.*** Significant at p < .05.

  • tandem, the statistical significance of the increase in R2sand the significant indirect effects together indicate theimportant role of relationship capital in explaining therelationship between partner characteristics and perfor-mance. Also, the presence of these indirect effects sug-gests that any omission of these variables from a theoreti-cal model could lead to an underestimation of the totaleffects of partner characteristics on performance. Spe-cifically, resource complementarity affects both projectperformance ( = .10) and strategic performance ( = .08),cultural compatibility affects both project performance ( =.22) and strategic performance ( = .20), and operationalcompatibility seems to influence both project performance( = .13) and strategic performance ( = .06) through rela-tionship capital.10 In summary, the results indicate theimportance of considering both direct and indirect effectson alliance performance, thus giving further credence tothe theoretical rationale behind integrating both structuraland relational perspectives into an explanation of allianceperformance.

    DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS

    Recent scholarship on international alliances has artic-ulated the need for more research partner selection issues(Hitt et al. 2000), especially because of their impact onalliance performance (Johnson et al. 1996; Saxton 1997).Although strategic alliances have proliferated throughoutthe world, a substantial proportion of these underperform(Madhok and Tallman 1998). In examining determinantsof alliance performance, we focus on a unique aspect asso-ciated with the characteristics of partners involved in analliance, namely, interfirm diversity (Parkhe 1991). Wesuggest that performance is likely to be enhanced whenfirms are able to manage the paradox involved in choosinga firm that is different, yet similar. Complementaryresource and capability profiles enhance the value gener-ated in alliances, as do similarity in the social institutionsof the partners. We therefore focus on three constructsrelated to interfirm diversity. Drawing on Parkhes (1991)work, we develop a multidimensional treatment ofresource complementarity, cultural compatibility, andoperational compatibility. We integrate extant interna-tional alliance literature that has traditionally examinedthe structural and sociopsychological aspects of alliancesseparately and develop a theoretical framework that sug-gests that the diversity-related characteristics of partnersaffect performance directly and indirectly through theireffects on relationship capital or sociopsychological vari-ables that are the focus of interactive theorists (Cullen et al.2000; Heide and Miner 1992). Our results provide aunique contribution to the understanding of how partnercharacteristics affect alliance-related performancebecause they suggest that various types of interfirm

    diversity differentially affect performance and that modelsthat integrate both structural and relationship-capital vari-ables have greater explanatory power over the vexingquestion of alliance performance. We now summarize themain implications of what this research has found.

    With regard to complementarity, our analysis indicatesthat the synergy that results when alliance partners pooltogether complementary resources and capabilitiesenhances performance. First, it enhances the economicefficiency and qualitative effectiveness of the task beingjointly carried out both directly and indirectly. While thedirect effect is stronger, there is a substantive indirecteffect, primarily through reciprocal commitment. It thusappears that when firms can partner with firms that cancomplement their weaknesses, not only is there a directeffect on project performance, but it also has the addedeffect of increasing the commitment of each partner to therelationship wherein they are willing to invest requisiteresources into the relationship to make it a success. Thisserves as a powerful signaling mechanism that reduces thethreat of opportunism, aligns incentive structures, and pro-vides a host of efficiencies. Second, although comple-mentarity does not have a direct effect on strategic perfor-mance (i.e., a perception that the relationship iscompetency enhancing and strategically beneficial), itdoes have an indirect effect, although weak, through themediating effect of relationship-capital variables. Thus,Type I interfirm diversity appears to have important impli-cations for performance. It enhances the efficiency andeffectiveness of the joint task being performed, or the com-mon benefits from the alliance, as well as more strategicprivate benefits that a focal firm may take out from therelationship. Furthermore, while its effect on the commonbenefits is primarily direct, its effect on private benefits ismediated through relationship capital. This appears plau-sible since mere complementarity may not lead to learningand knowledge transfer, which requires a certain depth ofinteraction and relationship quality for tacit know-how tobe transferred.

    The effects of Type II diversity, namely, cultural andoperational compatibility, on performance are intriguing,thus highlighting the dilemma and complexity surround-ing partner selection issues. Cultural compatibilityenhances both project and strategic performance. It affectsproject performance indirectly through relationship-capi-tal variables and influences strategic performance bothdirectly and indirectly (with the direct effect being stron-ger). Also, the total effect on strategic performance isstronger than on project performance, while relative tocomplementarity, cultural compatibility has a strongereffect on strategic performance than on project perfor-mance. The results suggest that when partners in an alli-ance share similar organizational cultures, they are likelyto enjoy a better quality of relationship, which in turn willfacilitate an effective intermingling of skills and

    Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 369

  • competencies and ensure that the project is efficiently andeffectively carried out. On the other hand, the strategiclearning that a focal firm can achieve from its partner or theprivate rents that it generates from the focal alliance isdirectly affected by the cultural congruence of thepartners.

    On the other hand, operational compatibility has a posi-tive indirect effect on project performance, implying thatcompatibility in procedural capabilities enhances the qual-ity of the relationship and thus increases the efficiency andeffectiveness of the project execution. However, it has asurprising negative direct impact on strategic perfor-mance, which is partly mitigated by a weaker positiveeffect through the relational mediators. This counter-iintuitive result is intriguing in that it suggests the follow-ing: although common benefits accruing from an allianceare enhanced when levels of professional skills, technicalcapabilities, and operational procedures of two partnersconverge, private benefits from the alliance may be ham-pered. We offer a speculation based on the learning raceanalogy of Hamel (1991). Operating at similar levels oftechnology and skills (albeit the specific technology,skills, and capabilities may be different) is likely toincrease the absorptive capacity of a firm and enhance itsability to recognize, assimilate, and commercialize exter-nal information (W. Cohen and Levinthal 1990) from itspartner. Accordingly, to protect itself from redundancy, afocal firm may be wary of passing information and know-how that it considers critical to partners that possess highlevels of absorptive capacity. In response to the perceivedthreat, they may clamp down with procedures that limittransfer of information and know-how beyond what isimmediately relevant for project execution to their part-ners. Thus, although operational compatibility translatesinto better project management and execution, it has a neg-ative effect on strategic performance. However, thisintriguing result needs to be examined in future research.Interestingly, this research suggests that different types ofcompatibility related to the social institutions of the part-ners could have very different effects on various aspects ofalliance performance.

    Furthermore, the indirect effects suggest the impor-tance of the mediating relationship-capital variables.Taken individually, the results suggest that reciprocalcommitment and mutual trust enhance the level of directcommon benefits from an alliance, while reciprocal com-mitment and bilateral information exchange increase theprivate benefits that accrue from an alliance. At a moremacro perspective, however, our research suggests that theissue of partner compatibility, which is influenced by orga-nizational routines related to partner selection, by itself can-not maximize benefits that can emerge from an alliance.The results indicate the strong impact of relationship-

    capital variables on alliance performance and thus high-light the importance of alliance management capabilities.In other words, the performance-enhancing effect of com-patibility is enhanced even further when firms make con-scious efforts to create relationship capital and embed therelationship within certain sociopsychological statesthrough an interaction process designed to specificallyimprove the quality of the relationship.

    The findings from this study have important manage-rial and theoretical implications. From a normative per-spective, the results suggest the relative importance of var-ious aspects in choosing the appropriate alliance partner.In particular, allying with firms with complementaryresources is likely to ensure success of the particular pro-ject (or shared benefits), while finding partners with simi-lar cultural norms is more important to achieve strategicprivate benefits. Thus, criteria used in partner choice canbe guided by specific firm objectives in forging cross-bor-der alliances. From a theoretical perspective, our findingsclarify the relative importance and interrelationshipsbetween partner characteristics and relational aspects inexplaining alliance performance. Much of the priorresearch has generally considered these two aspects asalternative ways to improve alliance performance. Ourfindings, by examining the direct and indirect effects ofpartner characteristics on performance, highlight the needto examine both simultaneously.

    While the study makes important contributions to thealliance