Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course...

download Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b3b9b55feef11f106f6 a357d31f1e7c49a98f0eccfce164cb62 %2522Learning With Half

of 12

Transcript of Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course...

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    1/12

    Learning with Half a Brain

    David D. Lent,1 Marianna Pinter,2 Nicholas J. Strausfeld1,3

    1Arizona Research Laboratories Division of Neurobiology, The University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona

    2 Department of Molecular and Cellular Biology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona

    3 Center for Insect Science, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona

    Received 15 August 2006; revised 16 October 2006; accepted 12 December 2006

    ABSTRACT: Since the 1970s, human subjects thathave undergone corpus callosotomy have provided impor-

    tant insights into neural mechanisms of perception, mem-

    ory, and cognition. The ability to test the function of each

    hemisphere independently of the other offers unique

    advantages for investigating systems that are thought to

    underlie cognition. However, such approaches have been

    limited to mammals. Here we describe comparable experi-

    ments on an insect brain to demonstrate learning-associ-

    ated changes within one brain hemisphere. After train-

    ing one half of their bisected brains, cockroaches learn

    to extend the antenna supplying that brain hemisphere

    towards an illuminated diode after this has been paired

    with an odor stimulus. The antenna supplying the nave

    hemisphere shows no response. Cockroaches retain this

    ability for up to 24 h, during which, shortly after train-

    ing, the mushroom body of the trained hemisphere alone

    undergoes specific post-translational alterations of micro-

    glomerular synaptic complexes in its calyces. ' 2007 Wiley

    Periodicals, Inc. Develop Neurobiol 67: 740751, 2007

    Keywords: insect; memory; mushroom bodies; PCamK;

    cockroach

    INTRODUCTION

    Research into mechanisms underlying learning and

    memory usually relies on comparisons between

    trained and nave subjects. However, assessing the

    results of tests between individuals is complicated by

    the fact that it is almost impossible to ensure that they

    have shared the same life experience before being

    tested. Even such genomic model systems as the fruit

    fly are unable to provide true control material. Indi-

    viduals are tested against other individuals withoutknowing if these individuals have actively learned

    and memorized environmental events before being

    tested for molecular changes that might have

    occurred as a consequence of memory acquisition.

    Here we use an insect model system that overcomes

    this by using the same individual for the test and con-

    trol. The experiments described here were performed

    on Periplaneta americana, the brain of which is not

    significantly different from that of Drosophila but is

    far more robust, allowing defined surgical interven-

    tion. The two supraesophageal hemispheres of the

    brain can be entirely separated while still eliciting

    unilateral motor actions from either hemisphere. Theuse of the cockroach as a model for learning and

    memory is evidenced by studies demonstrating both

    Pavlovian-type associative memory as well as mam-

    malian-like place memory (Mizunami et al., 1998;

    Watanabe et al., 2003; Kwon et al., 2004; Lent and

    Kwon, 2004; Pinter et al., 2005). In part, such studies

    have been enabled by making use of specific behav-

    ioral repertoires and relying on the brains resilience

    to chronic recording and lesioning (Mizunami et al.,

    Correspondence to: D.D. Lent ([email protected]).Contract grant sponsor: National Science Foundation; contract

    grant number: NSF IBN0411958.

    ' 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.Published online 21 February 2007 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).DOI 10.1002/dneu.20374

    740

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    2/12

    1998). The cockroach has also been used to demon-

    strate learning-dependent gene expression (Pinter

    et al., 2005).

    Here we use a modification of an established para-

    digm in which directional movements of the cockroach

    antennae (the antennal projection response: APR (Lent

    and Kwon, 2004) provide information about the progressand establishment of a memory trace (Lent and Kwon,

    2004). Unilateral conditioning of the APR is demon-

    strated by a learned response localized to one half of the

    split brain whereas the antenna serving the other half,

    when tested with the same conditioned stimulus, remains

    entirely unresponsive. This allows for one brain hemi-

    sphere to serve as the test subject and the other half as

    the control. Microscopic learning-associated alterations

    of the mushroom bodies, which are the assumed learning

    and memory centers of the insect brain (Heisenberg,

    2003; Davis, 2005), have been explored using an anti-

    body specific to autophosphorylated neuronal calmodu-

    lin kinase (PCamK), a protein associated with memory

    acquisition in mice and Drosophila (Miller and Ken-

    nedy, 1986; Silva et al., 1992; Griffith et al., 1993).

    When compared with the nave half, the trained brain

    half of the mushroom bodys calyx reveals an increase

    in the size of PCamK-positive active domains within the

    microglomeruli and in the neurites that supply Kenyon

    cell dendrites that form synaptic complexes at this level

    (Yasuyama et al., 2002).

    MATERIALS AND METHODS

    Animals

    Experiments were conducted on adult male P. americana

    from a laboratory colony. Animals were maintained on

    IAMS cat food and water and kept as described by Lent and

    Kwon (2004). Animals with any external damage (e.g.

    missing antennal segments or legs) were discarded.

    Split-Brain Preparation

    In preparation for surgery, animals were anesthetized by

    cooling to 48C. Cockroaches remained anesthetized

    throughout surgery by fixing them to a cold plate, whichconsisted of a plastic dish with an ice-filled center. To split

    the brain as far as its esophageal foramen, a single antero-

    posterior incision was made from above into the middle of

    the head capsule, to a depth of 2 mm and between 11.5

    mm long. The incision was then sealed with a low melting-

    point wax and the animal was isolated in a small chamber

    with access only to water. It was allowed 48 h to recover.

    Following recovery animals were monitored for general

    health and behavioral abnormalities. Animals that demon-

    strated forward locomotion and normal (but not coordi-

    nated) antennal movements were used for experiments.

    These animals comprised the split-brain group.

    Control surgeries, which consisted of an incision

    through the cuticle without contacting the brain, were done

    in parallel with split-brain surgeries. Animals that under-

    went control surgery comprised the intact brain group used

    in behavioral experiments. As with split-brain animals only

    those that demonstrated normal motor activity were used

    for learning and memory experiments. Results were eval-

    uated after postmortem histology demonstrated that a brain

    had been bisected as far as the esophageal foramen. Brains

    that had suffered asymmetric lesions were not included in

    the analysis.

    Training Protocol

    Restricted Training. In this procedure only one half of the

    head receives sensory information during training. Follow-

    ing recovery from surgery animals were anesthetized with

    CO2, restrained in a small plastic tube, and placed in the

    middle of a circular arena (Fig. 1). The wall of the arenahad a single green LED in the left hemifield and a single

    green LED (CS) in the right hemifield both positioned 58

    from midline of the roach. A food odor source (US) was

    released through a syringe immediately adjacent to each of

    the green LEDs. The protocol (Fig. 1) consisted of six pre-

    training trials of the conditioning stimulus (green light cue

    of 2 s duration) presented three times to each side of the

    roach to test the preconditioned response. Following pre-

    training, the eye and antenna on one side, (side randomly

    selected), were respectively occluded with black wax and a

    narrow plastic tube which was then sealed at both ends so

    as to totally isolate the olfactory receptor array. The

    occluded side belonged to the \nave" half. The unoccluded

    eye and antennae other belonged to the \trained" half. Fol-lowing pretraining, the animal was presented with five

    training trials to the trained half. The light cue and odor

    were onset simultaneously at each trial: the light was pre-

    sented for 2 s and the odor was presented for 1 s. Six test

    trials were performed randomly, three to the trained half

    and three to the nave half, in which only the CS was pre-

    sented. Inter-trial intervals (ITIs) were 1 min. Cockroaches

    that elicited APRs towards the CS following training were

    considered trained.

    Nonrestricted Training. After recovery from surgery, ani-

    mals were trained using the same stimulus protocol but

    without sensory input blocked to the designated nave half.

    This group of animals was used to analyze structural differ-

    ences between nave versus trained brain hemispheres. The

    protocol provided sensory input to each brain half, the

    trained half receiving paired stimulation and the nave half

    receiving unpaired stimulation of the eye and antenna.

    Immunoblot

    Sodium dodecylsulfate polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis

    (SDS-PAGE) was performed according to Laemmli (1970)

    Learning with Half a Brain 741

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    3/12

    in 4% stacking and 7.5% separating gel. Brains of adult

    male animals were dissected in phosphate buffered saline,

    pH 7.4 (PBS), transferred into Laemmli loading buffer and

    homogenized. Five percent of one brain corresponding to

    10 lg protein was loaded per lane. Protein concentration

    was determined according to Bradford (1976) using Protein

    Assay Dye Reagent (BioRad). Blotting was performed as

    described by Towbin et al. (1979) except for the use of

    0.05% Tween-20 in Tris buffered saline throughout the

    washing steps. Electrotransfer was checked by staining with

    0.1% Ponceau S in 5% acetic acid. Blocking solution

    (Roche) in maleic acid buffer was used to quench non-spe-

    cific binding. Anti-PCamK (sc-12886-R, Santa Cruz Bio-

    tech) primary antibody was added to the blocking solutionat 2000 times dilution. Horseradish peroxidase conjugated

    secondary antibody combined with SuperSignal West

    Femto substrate (Pierce) was used to visualize immunoreac-

    tivity. For preabsorption experiment anti-PCamK antibody

    was diluted 200 times in PBS and a five-fold excess (by

    weight) of the blocking (sc-12886 P) or substrate peptide

    (sc-24562) was added. After incubation for 2 h at 208C, the

    mixture was diluted 10 times in blocking solution buffered

    with maleic acid and the standard immunodetection proto-

    col was performed.

    Immunohistochemistry

    Split-brain animals were decapitated 30 min after training.

    The heads were placed into fixative (2% paraformaldehyde

    dissolved in PBS) and incubated overnight at 68C. Brains

    were dissected in phosphate buffered saline, pH 7.4 (PBS),

    embedded in gelatin/ovalbumin and postfixed in 10% neu-

    tral buffered formalin solution overnight at 68C. Frontal

    sections of gelatin embedded brains were sectioned at 45

    lm with a Leica vibratome. Immunostaining was per-

    formed on free-floating sections at room temperature. Sec-

    tions were equilibrated with 0.5% Triton X-100 dissolved

    in PBS (PBSTx), blocked with 5% normal goat serum

    (NGS) in PBSTx, and incubated simultaneously with the

    primary antibodies added to the blocking solution for at

    least 24 h. The primary antibodies were rabbit anti-PCamK

    and mouse anti-tubulin (MAB3408, Chemicon Int.)

    employed at 1:500 and 1:4000 dilutions, respectively. Sec-

    tions were washed with PBSTx and incubated for approxi-

    mately 16 h with the secondary antibodies; Alexa Fluor 488

    F(ab0) fragment of goat anti-rabbit IgG (Molecular Probes),

    and Cy5-conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG (Jackson) diluted

    in PBSTx containing 1% NGS to 1:400 and 1:500, respec-

    tively. After washing with PBSTx and rinsing with PBS,

    Figure 1 The experimental protocol. Upper left: diagram of the training arena with the cock-

    roach placed at its center with the two sets of spatially coincident cues (odor source and green

    diode) labeled A and 1, each offset 58 from the midline. Upper right: Training protocol. The para-

    digm employs two randomized sets of three pretraining trials, five training trials, and two random-

    ized sets of three testing trials. Lower left: micrograph of a surgically split brain after fixation and

    removal from the head capsule. Scale bar 1 mm.

    742 Lent et al.

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    4/12

    sections were mounted on slides and coverslipped under

    Elvanol (Rodriguez and Deinhardt, 1960). Images were

    acquired with a Zeiss Pascal confocal microscope.

    Measurements of Microglomeruli andKenyon Cell Fiber Bundles inSplit-Brain Cockroaches

    Given that the split extends through the supraesophageal

    ganglion the two halves of any one brain are linked

    throughout treatment by its subesophageal ganglion. This

    allows the tissue from each brain half to be fixed, embed-

    ded, and sectioned at the same level and, therefore, imaging

    of tissue belonging to the nave or trained hemispheres to

    be under the exact same conditions of excitation and image

    capture. Confocal images were reconstructed from three se-

    rial optical sections each with a total depth of 4 lm. Flat-

    tened stacks were processed using Adobe Photoshop. For

    microglomeruli analysis, images of the calyx were collected

    anteriorly above the apex of the vertical lobe. Merged

    images of PCamK and tubulin immunoreactivity were usedto measure differences of microglomerular dimensions at

    equivalent locations of the calyces of each brain hemi-

    sphere. The maximum edge-to-edge widths of identified

    microglomeruli were measured, as were center-to-center

    distances between microglomeruli. Between 105 and 180

    microglomeruli were measured from 2 to 3 areas from the

    lateral and medial calyces in each brain half. For calculat-

    ing PCamK immunoreactivity in bundles of Kenyon cell

    neurites, images were collected from calyces at a level ante-

    rior to the apex of the vertical lobes and from the calyces at

    a level that is immediately posterior to the mushroom body

    pedunculi. Sections of these parts of the calyx allowed

    imaging of neuropil and clusters of neuronal cell bodies

    within the calyx cup. The number of neurite bundles immu-nostained with a-PCamK were calculated as a percentage

    of all fibers stained with a-tubulin.

    Statistical Analysis

    For behavioral data, nonparametric statistics were used as

    described by Lent and Kwon (2004). Friedman ANOVA

    (ANOVA v2) was used to compare differences between

    repeated measures within a group. The Sign test (Z) was

    used compare values within the repeated measures within a

    group. The MannWhitney U test (U) was used to test dif-

    ferences between two groups. Values shown represent

    responses\

    0"

    or\

    1"

    in percentages; therefore, standarddeviations are not depicted. Confocal images of microglo-

    meruli and neurite fiber bundle were measured and scored in

    a blind test by an observer without knowledge of the identity

    (nave or experimental) of the images being measured or of

    the behavioral data. Differences were analyzed using two-

    tailed Students t-test. Corrections for small sample size

    were made when calculating the Tvalues of the fiber differ-

    ences. Using a correction for n 10 the 95% confidence

    interval is set as Xavg6 2.262r/N1/2. Differences were con-

    sidered statistically significant using a p value of 0.05.

    RESULTS

    Classical Conditioning with RestrictedSensory Input in Intact andSplit-Brain Cockroaches

    The antennal projection response (APR) of restrainedcockroaches were conditioned and measured during

    pretraining, training, and testing in cockroaches with

    intact brains (control surgery group; n 51) and split

    brains (split surgery group; n 48). In both groups

    input was restricted to either the left or right side

    [Figs. 1 and 2(a)]. Restricted input was used to ensure

    that the behavioral observations and comparisons

    made between intact and split brains were due to sen-

    sory input being associated unilaterally and not con-

    founded by interactions of bilateral sensory input.

    Both groups of animals performed APRs in less than

    10% of the pretraining trials, with no significant dif-

    ference between the two groups (n 51, 48; U 1197.00, p 0.85). There was also no significant

    difference between the two groups during training, in

    which the odor stimulus was coupled with the green

    light cue (n 51, 48; U 3363.50, p 0.9027).

    However, in both groups after training, the side of the

    animal exposed to paired stimulation by the CS and

    US exhibited vigorous APRs that were significantly

    different from pretraining (intact brain, n 51, Z

    5.2223, p 1.7669e-7; split brain, n 48, Z

    5.0289, p 4.932e-7). Both groups exhibited

    greater than 60% APRs to the green LED presented

    alone 5 min after training and were not significantly

    different from each other (n 51, 48; U 1201.50,

    p 0.8748). This indicated that split-brain cock-

    roaches actively sample the olfactory milieu as do

    animals that are intact and that, at least with regard to

    antennal motor actions, surgery does not adversely

    affect behavior. Testing the nave half of intact and

    split-brain animals yielded different results. Present-

    ing the conditioned stimulus to the nave side of the

    split brain after training elicited APRs that were not

    significantly different from pretraining (n 51, Z

    0.5000, p 0.6171). However, at 5 min, APRs

    of the nave half of animals with intact brains were

    significantly different from both pretraining (n

    51,Z 2.7735, p 0.005546) and from the APRs eli-

    cited from the trained half (n 51, Z 3.590662, p

    0.00033). Using a 6 min testing window 5 min af-

    ter training and randomizing the stimulus between

    the trained and nave sides of the brain of intact ani-

    mals provided some insight into these results. Com-

    parison of APRs of the nave half at different times

    during the test (irrespective of whether the nave half

    was tested before or after the trained half) revealed

    Learning with Half a Brain 743

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    5/12

    that if APRs were tested in the latter half of the 6 min

    testing window they were significantly increased

    compared with pretraining (n 26, Z 2.5981, p

    0.009375), but if testing occurred during the early

    stages of the testing window APRs were not signifi-

    cantly different from pretraining (n 25, Z

    1.2247, p 0.2207). This suggests that informa-tion acquired by the trained half of the brain is trans-

    ferred or made accessible to the nave brain half nei-

    ther during nor immediately after training, but after a

    delay of minutes.

    To further investigate the dynamics of this apparent

    memory transfer, APRs of the trained and nave half of

    intact brains were tested over a period of 24 h

    [Fig. 2(b)]. APRs of the trained half at 5 min, 1 h, 3 h,

    6 h, and 12 h after training were significantly differentfrom pretraining (n 33, ANOVA v2 23.5946, p

    0.0026) but not different from one another (n 33,

    ANOVA v2 2.3158, p 0.6779). As described,

    APRs from the nave half at 5 min after training were

    significantly different from APRs before training and

    different from the trained half at 5 min. However, APRs

    of the nave half at 1, 3, 6, and 12 h were not signifi-

    cantly different from each other (n 33, ANOVA v2

    2.8868, p 0.40942) or different from the trained half

    (n 33, ANOVA v2 3.4656, p 0.83885) at the

    same time intervals. While the trained half received

    both the US and CS during training, and thereafter had

    access to the learned association, these results show

    that access by the nave half to the learned association

    was delayed. Once the memory was transferred, the

    Figure 2

    Figure 2 Comparisons of APRs of intact (non-split) and

    split-brain cockroaches. (a) No significant difference is

    seen between the APRs of intact (n 51) and split-brain (n

    48) cockroaches during pretraining, training, and testing

    of the trained half. However, there is a significant differ-

    ence in APRs of the two groups between testing of the na-

    ive half and pretraining. There is no difference between the

    pretraining and the testing of the nave half of the split-brain (n 48). There is a significant difference between

    pretraining and the testing of the nave half of the intact

    brain (n 51) indicating access of the information by the

    nave half (see Results). (b) APRs of the trained half and

    nave half in intact cockroaches for 24 h. The APRs of the

    trained half at 5 min, 1 h, 3 h, 6 h, and 12 h are not signifi-

    cantly different (n 33). The APRs of the nave half at 1,

    3, 6, and 12 h are not significantly different from each other

    and not different from the trained half at the same time

    intervals (n 33). The APRs of both the trained half and

    nave half at 24 h are not significantly different from pre-

    training (n 33). This suggests that memory is transferred

    from the trained to the nave half after 5 min and that after

    24 h the memory decays. (c) APRs of the trained half andnave half in split-brain cockroaches for 24 h. The APRs of

    the trained half at 5 min, 1 h, 3 h, 6 h, and 12 h are not sig-

    nificantly different (n 33). The APRs of the nave half at

    5 min, 1 h, 3 h, 6 h, 12 h, and 24 h are not significantly dif-

    ferent from pretraining, suggesting no learning (n 33).

    The APRs of both the trained half and nave half at 24 h are

    not significantly different (n 33). This suggest that mem-

    ory is not transferred from the trained to nave half after 5

    min due to splitting of the brain and that after 24 h the

    memory decays in split-brains as it does in non-split.

    744 Lent et al.

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    6/12

    Figure 3 Two examples (ag & hl) of differences of PCamK-labeled microglomeruli in the

    calyces of the mushroom bodies of trained versus nave brain halves of split-brain P. americana 30

    min after the last training trial. Left: optical sections from the na ve brain half (a,c,e) are comparedwith equivalent locations in the lateral calyx in the trained brain half (b,d,f). Tubulin-immunoreac-

    tivity extends between microglomeruli that are revealed by an antibody against PCamK. g. The

    range of PCamK immunoreactive microglomerular dimensions is compared. Microglomerular

    PCamK domain/size distribution shifts towards higher values in the trained compared with the na-

    ve brain half. Right, h, i: two optical stacks (depth 12 lm) showing structural differences of micro-

    glomeruli between trained and nave brains (enlarged in j, k). Maximum diameters are compared in

    panel l. Scale bar for panels a-f and j, k 20 lm.

    Learning with Half a Brain 745

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    7/12

    Figure 4

    746 Lent et al.

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    8/12

    cockroach responds equally well to the conditioning

    stimulus presented to either the left or right visual field.

    The learned response was maintained for 12 h, after

    which there was a steady decay of the response until at

    24 h there was no significant difference of APRs eli-

    cited by either side from pretraining APRs (trained half,

    n 33, Z 1.5119, p 0.1306; nave half, n 33,Z 17678, p 0.0771).

    APRs of the trained and nave sides of split-brain

    cockroaches were also examined for a period of 24 h

    to determine whether any transfer might occur via

    pathways that linked one side of the brain to the other

    through subesophageal pathways such as a poststo-

    modeal commissure linking the tritocerebra (Gundel

    and Penzlin, 1995) [Fig. 2(c)]. APRs from the trained

    half measured at 5 min, 1 h, 3 h, 6 h, and 12 h were

    significantly different from pretraining (n 33,

    ANOVA v2 32.4324, p 0.00001), but not each

    other (n 33, ANOVA v2 3.7949, p 0.4345).

    APRs from the nave half at 5 min, 1 h, 3 h, 6 h, 12 h,

    and 24 h were not significantly different from pre-

    training (n 33, ANOVA v2 0.8696, p 0.9724).

    This indicates that the nave half remained nave

    throughout the 24 h period after training. However,

    after 24 h, APRs of the trained half were not signifi-

    cantly different from pretraining (n 33, Z

    0.0000, p 1.0000) suggesting that a decay of reten-

    tion, as indicated by APRs, occurs between 12 and 24

    h when the trained brain hemisphere was disassoci-

    ated from the untrained one.

    Classical Conditioning withNon-Restricted Sensory Input inSplit-Brain Cockroaches

    In a third experiment, split-brain cockroaches were

    trained under the same protocol previously described,

    but the eye and antenna on both left and right side were

    exposed to sensory stimulation. This non-restricted pro-

    tocol was necessary to control for possible alterations

    of structures or molecular changes that might be

    incurred by sensory exposure alone rather than learned

    sensory associations. Prior to utilizing the split-brain

    animals for analysis of morphological change it wasnecessary to compare the behavioral responses and

    observe if providing bilateral sensory input impacted

    the previously described learning. Comparisons of

    APRs elicited from the trained and the nave half fol-

    lowing training from split-brain animals conditioned in

    this non-restricted sensory condition were not signifi-

    cantly different from split-brain animals that were

    trained with sensory information restricted (n 48, 48,

    trained half, U 961.50, p 0.2516; n 48, 48, nave

    half, U 519.00, p 0.5134). This experiment pro-

    vided paired olfactory and visual cues that were associ-

    ated in the trained half and unpaired olfactory and vis-

    ual cues that were not associated in the nave half;

    unlike that demonstrated in split-brains that have been

    trained under sensory restricted conditions, which only

    received paired olfactory and visual cues. Training

    under the described non-restricted sensory condition

    could be used for effectively controlling observed dif-

    ferences due to sensory stimulation. Animals trained

    under these conditions were used for the analysis of

    PCamK positive dimensions of microglomeruli and the

    Kenyon cell neurites.

    CamK Autophosphorylation is Elevatedin the Mushroom Body Calyces ofTrained Brain Halves

    A body of evidence has suggested that mushroom

    bodies support learning and memory either at the

    level of their afferent supply in the calyces, or within

    Figure 4 Comparison of PCamK immunoreactivity in Kenyon cell neurites. (ad) Comparable

    regions of the lateral calyces in nave and trained hemispheres labeled with anti-PCamK (panels a, b)

    and anti-tubulin (panels c, d) of the same animal reveal differences in the number of Kenyon cell neu-

    rites that show double labeling (examples arrowed in panels e, f). This increase in PCamK immunor-

    eactivity is further manifested by the thickness of the layer (bracketed in e, f) of Kenyon cell neurites

    projecting over the inner surface of the calyx bowl. Images represent 4 lm thickness. Scale bar 20 lm.(g) Percentages of the PCamK-labeled fiber bundles, co-labeled in the tubulin-immunoreactive bundles

    were calculated from both the nave and trained brain halves of five split-brain trained animals. Fiber

    counts were performed in a blind test using composite images of PCamK and tubulin staining. Counts

    were made from frontal images of the medial (n 10), lateral (n 9), anterior (n 10) and posterior

    walls (n 9) of both the medial and lateral calyces. Statistics are corrected to small sample size. Error

    bars Standard deviation. h. Immunoblot characterization of the specificity of anti-PCamK. Anti-

    PCamK recognizes one discrete band of approximately 50 kD of roach brain homogenate (left col-

    umn). The blocking peptide with phosphorylated Thr286 inhibits staining when anti-PCamK is preab-

    sorbed with it (second column: block). The substrate peptide with a non-phosphorylated Thr286 has

    no effect on staining when anti-PCamK is preabsorbed with it (right column: subs).

    Learning with Half a Brain 747

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    9/12

    the lobes, or at both levels (Hammer and Menzel,

    1998; Zars et al., 2000; Heisenberg, 2003). We have

    investigated calycal organization, comparing this

    neuropil in the trained brain half versus the nave

    brain half of split-brain animals. The focus on the

    calyces is due to their distinctive composition of

    measurable synaptic complexes called microglomer-uli (Yasuyama et al., 2002; Frambach et al., 2004)

    and because changes in the lobes, if they occur,

    would be difficult to discern among the approxi-

    mately 200000 parallel fibers. Microglomeruli com-

    prise synaptic complexes that are formed between the

    boutons of presynaptic terminals from the ipsilateral

    antennal lobes, ipsilateral GABAergic interneurons

    from the protocerebrum and dendritic spines of Ken-

    yon cells, the axons of which supply the mushroom

    body lobes (Farris and Sinakevitch, 2003). These syn-

    aptic complexes underlie divergence from calycal

    afferents to intrinsic pathways within the lobes.

    Changes of the calyces between the trained and nave

    halves of split-brain animals were analyzed by label-

    ing the brain with an antibody raised against anti-

    PCamK, which is specific to the autophosphorylated

    amino acid P286Thr that renders human CamKIIa

    constitutively active (Miller and Kennedy, 1986). Im-

    munohistochemical staining was performed on brain

    tissue collected 30 min after training (see Fig. 2). The

    size of the PCamK immunoreactive microglomeruli

    and spacing in the trained versus nave calyces of

    split-brain cockroaches were analyzed using double

    labeling with anti-PCamK and anti-tubulin antibodies

    (Fig. 3).

    PCamK immunoreactive microglomeruli are bor-

    dered by tubulin-stained neuronal processes [Fig.

    3(e,f)]. Comparison of trained and nave halves of

    split-brain cockroaches (n 5) showed significant

    alterations in the sizes of autophosphorylated micro-

    glomerular domains in the trained half. Measure-

    ments of the maximum widths of the domains (n

    105180) showed increases of widths by 4.85,

    7.85, 8.47, 12.3, and 16.2%, respectively, in the

    trained halves compared with the nave halves. These

    differences were significant in all animals measured

    (t 4.374430, 5.468498, 8.357868, 10.412, 13.43387;

    p

    2.1722e-5, 2.045e-7, 1.7304e-14, 8.971e-18,6.6199e-29). Plots of the normal distributions of the

    PCamK immunoreactive microglomerular domains in

    the trained and nave half of a single cockroach where

    there was a 16.2% increase in size is shown in Figure

    3(g) and the corresponding labeled sections shown in

    Figure 3(af). As a control, measurements of the

    number of glomeruli and the center-to-center distance

    between glomeruli were also compared. No signifi-

    cant differences were found between trained and

    nave halves (n 25, t 1.4285, p 0.1660; n 25,

    t 0.1432, p 0.8897).

    Differences were also resolved in immunolabeling

    of bundles of Kenyon cell neurites leading from the

    cell body layer above the calyx to dendrites supplying

    the calyces [Fig. 4(ag)]. An increase of PCamK

    immunoreactivity, which is here interpreted to reflectmobilization of PCamK, was revealed in calyces of

    the trained brain half [Fig. 4(b)] compared with the

    nave half [Fig. 4(a)]. An anti-tubulin antibody was

    employed to reveal bundles of Kenyon cell neurites

    that were not stained for PCamK [Fig. 4(c,d)]. Per-

    centages of PCamK-labeled fibers calculated from

    equivalent areas of the lateral calyx of the nave and

    trained brain half [Fig. 4(g)] showed a greater than

    50% increase in labeling in the calyx of the trained

    half. These increases were observed in equivalent lat-

    eral areas of the medial (n 10, t 8.795, p

    0.0000103) and the lateral (n 9, t 13.093, p

    0.0000011) calyces and at equivalent anterior (n

    10, t 8.893, p 0.00000941) and posterior (n

    9, t 9.112, p 0.0000169) areas [Fig. 4(g)].

    Immunoblot characterization of anti-PCamK shows

    that it recognized only one *50 kD band in roach

    brain homogenate ([Fig. 4(h)], first panel). Immuno-

    reactivity was quenched if the antibody was preincu-

    bated with an excess of the antigen blocking peptide

    containing P286Thr, which corresponds to autophos-

    phorylated CamKIIa of human origin ([Fig. 4(h)],

    second panel). Immunoreactivity was unaffected

    when anti-PCamK was preincubated with an excess

    of the unphosphorylated substrate peptide containing

    286Thr corresponding to nonphosphorylated CamKIIa

    of human origin ([Fig. 4(h)], third panel).

    DISCUSSION

    The robust nature of the cockroach allows chronic

    surgical intervention without diminishing antennal

    motor action by the restrained animal. Split-brain

    preparations can therefore be used to compare behav-

    ioral and structural differences within circumscribed

    brain regions after training only one half of the brain.

    In the present experiments, in which half of a surgi-cally split-brain P. americana was conditioned to as-

    sociate a light cue with an odor source, the learned

    association, which is restricted to the half that

    received paired visual and olfactory information,

    decays within 24 h. This compares to intact animals

    tested with the same association paradigm in which

    memory was still present at 72 h (Lent and Kwon,

    2004). Interestingly, animals with an intact brain

    also showed reduced memory at 24 h when the pre-

    748 Lent et al.

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    10/12

    sentation of the CS and US is restricted to only the

    trained side by sheathing the contralateral antenna

    and covering the contralateral eye (Fig. 2). These

    findings suggests that in intact animals the dynamics

    of long term memory involve more than simple asso-

    ciation of the conditioned and unconditioned stimu-

    lus by one side of the brain but require unimpededsampling of the sensory surroundings by the opposite

    side.

    Interestingly, following unilateral conditioning,

    cockroaches with intact brains demonstrated that the

    visual-olfactory memory trace is either localized in

    both halves or has been made accessible to both

    halves after a delay of several minutes. This suggests

    that the memory trace develops rapidly, possibly

    within the mushroom body, in the trained half of the

    brain and is then transferred, or is made accessible to,

    the mushroom body on the contralateral side. These

    results in the cockroach provide for a comparison of

    memory transfer dynamics in the honey bee. Analysis

    of the olfactory memory trace in honey bees, using

    proboscis extension response (PER) conditioning, has

    shown that there is also a transfer of learned informa-

    tion between sides, but after a significantly longer

    delay. It was only when honey bees were tested with

    longer retention times of 3 or 24 h that bilateral mem-

    ory could be demonstrated (Sandoz and Menzel,

    2001). Sandoz and Menzel (2001) suggest that a

    short-term unilateral olfactory memory trace may be

    established in the antennal lobe whereas the mush-

    room bodies are involved in the consolidation pro-

    cess. In Drosophila, experiments have supported this

    proposition in that the initial phases of olfactory

    memory are represented, to some extent, by changes

    in the representation of odors by the projection neu-

    rons (Yu et al., 2004). There are, however, critical

    differences between the conditioning paradigms of

    the cockroach and honey bee that may account for

    the observed differences in memory transfer dynam-

    ics. The sensory modalities that are involved in the

    conditioning procedures are likely to be reflected by

    which areas of the brain are involved in particular

    memory processes. PER conditioning involves the

    integration of gustatory and olfactory information.

    The convergence of these modalities in the deutocere-brum, at a level prior to their representation relayed

    to mushroom bodies, may result in delayed involve-

    ment or recruitment of the mushroom bodies in the

    formation of the memory trace because antennal

    lobes themselves are capable of supporting short-

    term memory. Conditioning of the APR involves the

    integration of converging visual and olfactory infor-

    mation. Because such convergence is know to occur

    at the level of the mushroom bodies (Li and Straus-

    feld, 1997) an immediate involvement of the mush-

    room bodies might be expected to form short-term

    memory and support the longer memory trace. Thus,

    like that demonstrated from honey bees, bilateral

    transfer of a memory trace in the cockroach is time-

    dependent, but on a much faster scale than in the hy-

    menopteran system with the modalities employed.The present experiments demonstrate that the phe-

    nomenon of bilateral transfer is probably not uniform

    across different modalities but appears also to be mo-

    dality dependent.

    The present split-brain preparation has the singular

    advantage in that it allows a single animal to be both

    the control and test subject. The split-brain paradigm

    eliminates individual variation, thereby offering an

    unprecedented advantage for molecular studies of

    learning and memory (Pinter et al., 2005). The pres-

    ent results reveal the sites at which there are

    increased levels of autophosphorylated CamK 30 min

    after training as a consequence of sensory association.

    It has been shown in mice that the CamK signaling

    pathway is critical for both explicit and implicit

    memory (Mayford et al., 1996). How the increase in

    PCamK relates specifically to associative memory

    and general memory processes in the cockroach is

    unknown. Here the observed increases in PCamK

    have been used as a correlative marker of sensory

    association and not a direct marker of learning or

    memory formation. Differences within the calyces

    specifically relate to synaptic complexes, called

    microglomeruli, that consist of Kenyon cell dendritic

    spines clustered around, and postsynaptic to, termi-

    nals of afferent neurons (Yasuyama et al., 2002; Far-

    ris and Sinakevitch, 2003; Frambach et al., 2004).

    Such changes suggest either upregulation or translo-

    cation of PCamKII (Lai et al., 1987; Gaertner et al.,

    2004) to synaptic sites (Shen and Meyer, 1999), a

    phenomenon also reported from studies of mamma-

    lian hippocampus (Gleason et al., 2003). The present

    results also show a corresponding increase in consti-

    tutively activated CamK in neurite bundles of Ken-

    yon cells in the trained brain half during the early

    phases of memory. Previous in vivo studies describe

    CamK translocation to postsynaptic densities after

    Ca2

    /calmodulin activation and after activation-induced autophosphorylation stabilize its synaptic

    association (Gleason et al., 2003). We propose that

    during sensory association, and persisting for some

    minutes thereafter, CamK is constitutively activated

    within Kenyon cell neurites, and within dendrites and

    their postsynaptic spines, resulting in increased size

    of active domains within the microglomeruli.

    Transient learning-induced changes of CamKII

    have been demonstrated in the hippocampus of rats

    Learning with Half a Brain 749

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    11/12

    following inhibitory avoidance training. Following

    one trial learning there was an increase of between

    2575% of both total and calcium-independent activ-

    ities of CamKII in the hippocampus of rats 0 min af-

    ter training, but there was no observable difference

    after 120 min. Additionally, an increase of 24% in the

    in vitro phosphorylation of both the alpha and betasubunits of CamKII was observed in the synaptoso-

    mal membranes of trained rats 30 min after training,

    but not 120 min after training (Cammarota et al.,

    1998). Comparisons of these significant increases

    observed in the hippocampus to those of the mush-

    room bodies within minutes of training demonstrate

    that those increases quantified in the mushroom

    bodies, while quite large, are actually on the conserv-

    ative side compared with those identified in the hip-

    pocampus. These changes in the mushroom bodies

    are also assumed to be transient and not to lead to

    persistent structural alterations of the microglomeruli.

    Comparable increases are not seen in sampled indi-

    viduals that are confronted on a daily basis by sensory

    associations. Microglomerulus size and number how-

    ever, are subject to other environmental and develop-

    mental events, such as rearing conditions and changes

    of behavioral repertoire (Farris et al., 2001; Groh

    et al., 2006), as in the transition from nurse to forager

    honey bee. The presence of f-actin within Kenyon

    cell spines of microglomeruli (Frambach et al., 2004)

    is a further indication that, as demonstrated during

    learning by the hippocampus (Fukazawa et al., 2003),

    specific structures within the insect mushroom bodies

    reveal learning-related plasticity.

    DDL was supported under a National Science Founda-

    tion Graduate Research Fellowship Award. The authors

    thank Norman T. Davis for advice in immunohistochemis-

    try, Elizabeth Riordan for behavioral tests, Chhitij Bashyal

    for performing the blind measurements, and Dr. Katalin

    Gothard for valuable discussion.

    REFERENCES

    Bradford MM. 1976. A rapid and sensitive method for the

    quantitation of microgram quantities of protein utilizing theprinciple of protein-dye binding. Anal Biochem 72:248

    254.

    Cammarota M, Bernabeu R, De Stein ML, Izquierdo I, Me-

    dina JH. 1998. Learning-specific, time-dependent increases

    in hippocampal Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein ki-

    nase II activity and AMPA GluR1 subunit immunoreac-

    tivity. Eur J Neurosci 10:26692676.

    Davis RL. 2005. Olfactory memory formation in Drosoph-

    ila: From molecular to systems neuroscience. Annu Rev

    Neurosci 28:275302.

    Farris SM, Robinson GE, Fahrbach SE. 2001. Experience-

    and age-related outgrowth of intrinsic neurons in the

    mushroom bodies of the adult worker honeybee. J Neuro-

    sci 21:63956404.

    Farris SM, Sinakevitch I. 2003. Development and evolution

    of the insect mushroom bodies: Towards the understand-

    ing of conserved developmental mechanisms in a higher

    brain center. Arthropod Struct Dev 32:79101.

    Frambach I, Rossler W, Winkler M, Schurmann FW. 2004.

    F-actin at identified synapses in the mushroom body neu-

    ropil of the insect brain. J Comp Neurol 475:303314.

    Fukazawa Y, Saitoh Y, Ozawa F, Otha Y, Mizuno K, Ino-

    kuchi K. 2003. Hippocampal LTP is accompanied by

    enhanced f-actin content within the dendritic spine that is

    essential for late LTP maintenance in vivo. Neuron

    38:447460.

    Gaertner TR, Putkey JA, Waxham MN. 2004. RC3/Neuro-

    granin and Ca2

    /calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II

    produce opposing effects on the affinity of calmodulin

    for calcium. J Biol Chem 279:3937439382.

    Gleason MR, Higashijima S, Dallman J, Liu K, Mandel G,

    Fetcho JR. 2003. Translocation of CaM kinase II to syn-

    aptic sites in vivo. Nat Neurosci 6:217218.

    Griffith LC, Verselis LM, Aitken KM, Kyriacou CP, Danho

    W, Greenspan RJ. 1993. Inhibition of calcium/calmodu-

    lin-dependent protein kinase in Drosophila disrupts be-

    havioral plasticity. Neuron 10:501509.

    Groh C, Ahrens D, Rossler W. 2006. Environment- and age-de-

    pendent plasticity of synaptic complexes in the mushroom

    bodies of honeybee queens. Brain Behav Evol 68:114.

    Gundel M, Penzlin H. 1995. Anatomy of neurons crossing

    the tritocerebral commissures of the cockroach Peripla-

    neta americana (Blattaria). J Morphol 223:225242.

    Hammer M, Menzel R. 1998. Multiple sites of associative

    odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections

    of octopamine in honeybees. Learn Mem 5:146156.

    Heisenberg M. 2003. Mushroom body memoir: From maps

    to models. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:266275.

    Kwon H-W, Lent DD, Strausfeld NJ. 2004. Spatial learning

    in the restrained American cockroach Periplaneta ameri-

    cana. J Exp Biol 207:377383.

    Laemmli UK. 1970. Cleavage of structural proteins during

    the assembly of the head of bacteriophage T4. Nature

    227:680685.

    Lai Y, Nairn AC, Gorelick F, Greengard P. 1987. Ca2+/cal-

    modulin-dependent protein kinase. II. Identification of

    autophosphorylation sites responsible for generation of

    Ca2

    /calmodulin-independence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    84:57105714.Lent DD, Kwon H-W. 2004. Antennal movements reveal

    associative learning in the American cockroach, Peripla-

    neta americana. J Exp Biol 207:369375.

    Li Y, Strausfeld NJ. 1997. Morphology and sensory modality of

    mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the brain of the cock-

    roach Periplaneta americana. J Comp Neurol 386:120.

    Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang Y, Wang L, Hawkins RD,

    Kandel ER. 1996. Control of memory formation through

    regulated expression of a CAMK II transgene. Science

    274:16781683.

    750 Lent et al.

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu

  • 7/29/2019 Https Bb-App.csufresno.edu Bbcswebdav Internal Courses BIOL248!03!35209-2133 Wikis Course 656de2d1a2764b

    12/12

    Miller SG, Kennedy MB. 1986. Regulation of brain type II

    Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase by autophos-

    phorylation: A Ca2+-triggered molecular switch. Cell

    44:861870.

    Mizunami M, Weibrecht JM, Strausfeld NJ. 1998. The

    cockroach mushroom body: Participation in place mem-

    ory. J Comp Neurol 402:520537.

    Pinter M, Lent DD, Strausfeld NJ. 2005. Memory consoli-

    dation and gene expression in Periplaneta americana.

    Learn Mem 12:3038.

    Rodriguez J, Deinhardt F. 1960. Preparation of a semiper-

    manent mounting medium for fluorescent antibody stud-

    ies. Virology 12:316317.

    Sandoz J-C, Menzel R. 2001. Side-specificity of olfactory

    learning in the honeybee: Generalization between odors

    and sides. Learn Mem 8:286294.

    Shen K, Meyer T. 1999. Dynamic control of CaMKII trans-

    location and localization in hippocampal neurons

    by NMDA receptor stimulation. Science 284:162

    166.

    Silva AJ, Paylor R, Wehner JM, Tonegawa S. 1992. Im-

    paired spatial learning in a-calcium-calmodulin kinase II

    mutant mice. Science 257:206211.

    Towbin H, Staehelin T, Gordon J. 1979. Electrophoretic

    transfer of proteins from polyacrylamide gels to nitrocel-

    lulose sheets: Procedure and some applications. Proc

    Natl Acad Sci USA 76:43504354.

    Watanabe H, Kobayashi Y, Sakura M, Matsumoto Y, Mizu-

    nami M. 2003. Classical olfactory conditioning in the cock-

    roach Periplaneta americana. Zool Sci 20: 14471454.

    Yasuyama K, Meinertzhagen IA, Schurmann FW. 2002.

    Synaptic organization of the mushroom body calyx in

    Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Neurol 445:211226.

    Yu D, Ponomarev A, Davis RL. 2004. Altered representa-

    tion of the spatial code for odors after olfactory classical

    conditioning; memory trace formation by synaptic

    recruitment. Neuron 42:437449.

    Zars T, Fischer M, Schulz R, Heisenberg M. 2000. Local-

    ization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science

    288:672675.

    Learning with Half a Brain 751

    Developmental Neurobiology. DOI 10.1002/dneu