Gender Differences

42
1 RESEARCH REPORT 5 The health of subjects: evidence from examinations entries Susannah Wright Research Officer, University of Oxford October 2006

description

 

Transcript of Gender Differences

Page 1: Gender Differences

1

RESEARCH REPORT 5

The health of subjects: evidence from examinations entries

Susannah Wright

Research Officer, University of Oxford October 2006

Page 2: Gender Differences

2

The health of subjects: evidence from examinations entries Susannah Wright

1. Introduction

This paper presents one aspect of the work of the Nuffield Review of 14-19 education and

training. While there are disagreements over to what extent the school curriculum should be

organised around subject disciplines (or around learners), most educational institutions serving

14-19 year olds, and the work of government agencies such as Ofsted and QCA, are de facto

organised on the basis of subjects. It was therefore decided to review evidence on the ‘health’ of

different subjects.

Data on entries to different subjects at GCSE and A-Level since 1994 and 1993 respectively has

been used as a starting point for this analysis. As Walford (2000, p.303) notes, “The number of

students annually taking a subject at examination level is not a complete representation of the

subject’s health, but it does represent a visible and valuable recurring indicator in relation to both

the status and popularity of the subject in schools.” Examination statistics have been quoted

(sometimes selectively) by the media. They have been used by government bodies like the DfES

and QCA for monitoring purposes and also to inform funding and resource allocation decisions,

and by subject associations for monitoring purposes and to support arguments for additional

resources for their subject. It was decided to select a limited number of subjects – chemistry,

physics, mathematics, geography, and modern foreign languages – in order to investigate these

issues in some detail.

This focus on GCSE and A-Level examinations data does not cover all aspects of a ‘healthy’ 14-19

curriculum and learning experience. Vocational qualifications, extra-curricular activities, and

informal learning are also important. The availability of data is a key issue here. Publicly available

administrative data on vocational qualifications is not as detailed as that on GCSE and A-Level and

therefore cannot be analysed in the same way. Administrative data exists on subjects covered for

GNVQ/AVCE and also for frameworks within the apprenticeship programme, but frequent changes

in data collection methods and in the design of the programmes themselves make it difficult to

construct time series data. Other vocational qualifications including BTEC are only counted in the

main administrative datasets as part of the total number of entry Level/Level 1/Level 2/Level 3

qualifications obtained. These data are not disaggregated by type of qualification, or by subjects

Page 3: Gender Differences

3

chosen.1 Also, there is very little quantitative data on extra-curricular activities and informal

learning. Part of the remit of the Review is to assess the quality of the evidence available and to

identify significant gaps: the paucity of data for qualifications other than GCSE and A-Level and

for extra-curricular activities and informal learning is one of these gaps. Further research on

vocational qualifications and a review of qualitative evidence available on informal learning and

extra-curricular activities would be valuable but not possible within the time and resource limits of

the review.

1 “Subject” take-up in vocational qualifications could be traced through privately-owned datasets such as PLASC and ILR. Time and resource limits preclude this sort of analysis for the time being.

Page 4: Gender Differences

4

2. The data

This paper will now present and comment on time-series data for GCSE and A-Level examination

entries. Examination entry data is an imperfect proxy for subject take-up, as some pupils may

have studied on a GCSE or A-Level course but not been entered for the examination. Nevertheless

these data give a useful indication of changing patterns of decision-making among 14-19 year

olds and changing patterns of curriculum provision and organisation among schools and colleges

Before the data are presented it is important to comment on the data in terms of quality, how

they have been collected and collated, and how they have been used.

First, there is no single source of data on examination entries and attainment. Examination Board

data (collated by Joint Council of Qualifications from 2001 and its predecessors before then) are

the most detailed, broken down by country and by gender, but give only provisional figures. A

number of subject associations have also collated data, for their own subject and sometimes for

others too, but these data cannot be used for cross-subject comparisons over time. This paper

analyses DfES entry and achievement data: for A-Levels data collated in UK Statistics of Education

(published annually) and for GCSE time series data for England presented in a Statistical First

Release (DfES 2005).2 This use of different datasets was a deliberate strategy. While this makes

direct comparison between GCSE and A-Level data impossible, it has the benefit of illustrating

some of the potential pitfalls of the data available and the difficulty of making comparisons over

time.

Second, the existence of different datasets which collect and organise data in different ways

makes the construction of time series datasets problematic. This difficulty is compounded by

changes in methods of data collection over time, even within the same dataset. For example, the

A-Level entry statistics in DfES Statistics of Education are for all ages before 1997, and only 16-18

year-olds after this date. Nevertheless, the numbers taking the qualification outside the age

cohort are small and therefore should have a minimal impact on the data.

Third, changes in the post-14 curriculum and the structure of post-14 assessment mean the

nature of GCSEs and A-Levels has changed over time. Boundaries between subjects are dynamic:

for example, elements of science and technology have merged with other subjects (Bell 2001).

The content of syllabuses has also changed. For example, there has been a shift from physical

geography towards social geography in GCSE and A-Level geography syllabuses (Birnie 1999),

and a shift from ‘pure’ to applied areas in the topics covered at mathematics A-Level (Hoyles et al.

2 Time series data from 1994 to 2004 are in Table 26 of the SFR presented as an additional excel file.

Page 5: Gender Differences

5

2001, Kahn and Hoyles 1997). Such changes make it difficult to interpret time series data: we

may not be comparing like with like over time.

For these reasons it is important to treat these data with caution. They should be seen as a crude

measure. Also, the examination entry data analysed is disaggregated by gender, but cannot be

disaggregated by region, ethnicity, or type of school. More detailed data – for instance

disaggregated by region, by towns or boroughs within regions, type of school, or even by

individual schools – has been collated by subject associations or individual researchers for

particular subjects, but cannot be used for cross-subject comparisons.

Page 6: Gender Differences

6

3. Changing patterns of subject take-up

3.1 Time series data – GCSE

Fig 1. GCSE entries (England) 1994-2004

0

100000

200000

300000

400000

500000

600000

700000

1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

ChemistryPhysicsDouble Award ScienceMathematicsGeographyFrenchGermanSpanish

Fig 2. GCSE entries (England) 1994-2004 as % total entries

0.00

2.00

4.00

6.00

8.00

10.00

12.00

14.00

1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

year

% to

tal G

CSE

ent

ries Chemistry

PhysicsDouble Award ScienceMathematicsGeographyFrenchGermanSpanish

Page 7: Gender Differences

7

Fig 3. GCSE (England) entries 1994-2004 % of all 15-year olds attempting the subject

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

year

% o

f 15

year

-old

s

ChemistryPhysicsDouble Award ScienceMathematicsGeographyFrenchGermanSpanish

Table 1. 15 year-old pupils and total entries (England) 1994-2004

Year Total GCSE entries Number of 15 year old

pupils

1994 3,729,427 532,273

1995 4,092,696 578,197

1996 4,194,872 594,035

1997 4,155,907 586,766

1998 4,141,083 575,210

1999 4,229,612 580,972

2000 4,251,811 580,393

2001 4,422,251 603,318

2002 4,843,625 606,554

2003 4,872,690 622,122

2004 4,952,980 643,560

Time series data for GCSE and for A-Level have been calculated as 1) the raw number of entries;

2) the percentage of total entries; and 3) the percentage of the age cohort attempting the

Page 8: Gender Differences

8

subject. The raw number of entries is important - as this affects staffing decisions among other

matters – but the percentage of total entries is required to get a clearer sense of trends over time

the percentage of total entries is required. It is also important to investigate how changes in

entry figures relate to the size of the age cohort.3

Mathematics has been compulsory to age 16 through the whole period. It has also been

compulsory through the period for all pupils to take some science to age 16. It is left to schools

whether to offer single science subjects or science double award or single award GCSEs.

Geography has been optional at Key Stage 4 (KS4) throughout this period. Nevertheless,

geography, like other options subjects, has been affected by changes to the national curriculum in

1995 whereby the time allotted to core areas of English, mathematics and science was increased,

thereby limiting the time available for optional subjects. MFL became part of the compulsory

national curriculum for pupils up to age 16 in 1989. However, from 1998 MFL and design and

technology could be disapplied for some pupils, and in September 2004 MFL was downgraded

from statutory to entitlement status at KS4.

Key trends in the time series data for GCSE entries are as follows:

1. Low and relatively static numbers entered for physics and chemistry – consistently less than

10% of total GCSE entries and of the age cohort. The majority of pupils are taking Double

Award science with a small increase between (72% of 15 year-old pupils in 1994 and 75% in

2004).

2. There has been a rise in the number of entries for mathematics and double award science

(from 479,084 in 1994 to 606,002 in 2004 for mathematics and from 380,893 in 1994 to

479,591 in 2004 for double award science). The proportion of 15 year-old pupils taking these

subjects also rose over the period examined, from 72% in 1994 to 75% in 2004 for double

award science, and from 90% in 1994 to 94% in 2004 for mathematics. However, in 2004

both subjects constituted a slightly smaller proportion of the total number of GCSE

examination entries than in 1994. Entries for mathematics counted for 12.85% of all GCSE

entries in 1994 and 12.24% in 2004, while the corresponding figures for Double Award science

were 10.21% of all GCSE entries in 1994 and 9.68% in 2004.

3. Entries for geography have fallen (with the exception of 2001), from a high of 270,091 entries

(6.44% of all GCSE entries) in 1996 to 197,123 entries (3.98% of all GCSE entries) in 2004. This 3 These data are based on the cohort information in the DfES Statistical First Release. However, population statistics have been amended since this SFR was published so some of the calculations presented may be erroneous.

Page 9: Gender Differences

9

downward trend continued in 2005, with GCSE entries down by 4.9% from 2004 figures (QCA

2005).

4. Entries for French GCSE rose from 295,211 in 1994 to 321,207 in 2001, but have declined since

(289,756 in 2004). However, as a percentage of all GCSE entries there was a slow decline from

7.92% in 1994 to 7.26% in 2001, and a much faster decline from 2001 to 5.85% in 2004.

Entries for German GCSE rose (with some fluctuation) from 110,446 in 1994 to 130,627 in 2001,

but have declined since (there were 116,347 entries in 2004). As a percentage of all GCSE entries

German entries rose from 2.96% in 1994 to a high of 3.07% in 1999, but have declined since

(to 2.35% in 2004). Entries in Spanish, on the other hand, have risen steadily both in raw

numbers and as a percentage of total entries, but from a very low base (from 28,000 (0.75% of

all GCSE entries) in 1994 to 53,539 (1.08% of all GCSE entries) in 2004). The timing of the sharp

decline in the number of entries for French and German suggests that it may be associated

with the disapplication of MFL for some pupils since 1998. However, no definite causal

relationship can be claimed from this data alone.

3.2 Time series data – GCSE disaggregated by gender

GCSE time series data disaggregated by gender are displayed in Appendix 1. They reveal

important trends which are not evident in the aggregate data:

1. Chemistry has consistently been taken by more males than females, and this gap has not

narrowed significantly between 1994 and 2004. There is a similar pattern for physics, with the

gap between males and females being possibly slightly wider than for chemistry.

2. Entries for double award science do not reveal a significant gender gap but still more males

than females are entered for GCSE examination. This gap appears larger for the percentage of

total GCSE entries than for the number of entries.

3. The numbers of males and females entered for GCSE mathematics are close but there are

more male than female candidates. Mathematics is a compulsory subject for all pupils till 16,

and overall there are more female than male GCSE entries. This suggests that a higher

proportion of male pupils than female pupils are entered for examination. Further research

would be needed to establish the reasons for this difference. Moreover, there is a sharp dip in

mathematics entries as a percentage of total entries for both males and females in 2002 which

is not visible in figures 1-3 but evident in these graphs owing to the large scale.

Page 10: Gender Differences

10

4. More females than males are consistently entered for French, German and Spanish GCSE.

Whilst for French the gap between males and females has become slightly wider since 2000,

the gap has narrowed for German. This gap existed even when one MFL GCSE was

compulsory for all students. Perhaps it can be explained by more females than males taking a

second MFL GCSE as one of their optional subjects.

5. Geography has been optional throughout the period, and has been consistently taken by more

boys than girls. QCA (2005) suggest a figure consistently close to 45% girls.

Page 11: Gender Differences

11

3.3 Time series data – A-Level

Fig. 4. A-Level/SCE Higher entries (UK) 1993-2004

0

10000

20000

30000

40000

50000

60000

70000

80000

90000

100000

1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries Chemistry

PhysicsMathematicsGeographyFrenchGermanSpanish

Fig. 5. A-Level/SCE Higher entries (UK) 1993-2003 as % total entries

A-Level/Higher entries as % total entries

0.00

2.00

4.00

6.00

8.00

10.00

12.00

1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

ChemistryPhysicsMathematicsGeographyFrenchGermanSpanish

Page 12: Gender Differences

12

Fig. 6 A-Level/SCE Higher entries (UK) as % age cohort

A-Level/Higher entries as % of age cohort

0.00

2.00

4.00

6.00

8.00

10.00

12.00

14.00

1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004

year

% a

ge c

ohor

t

ChemistryPhysicsMathematicsGeographyFrenchGermanSpanish

These data are for the UK as a whole and include entries for Scottish Highers as well as A-Levels.4

At A-Level (unlike at GCSE) no subjects are compulsory: all subjects have the same statutory

status within the curriculum. Therefore, examination entry data for A-Level reflects, more than

for GCSE, students’ decisions and preferences. Nevertheless, students are constrained in their

decisions by what subjects and courses institutions offer.

Key trends in the time series data for A-Level/Higher entries are as follows:

1. The main trend in chemistry, physics, geography, French and German between 1993 and 2004

has been one of decline (in the total number of entries, in the percentage of total entries, and

in the percentage of the age cohort entered for the subject). This decline has been most

marked in French.

2. Mathematics has consistently had the highest take-up of all these subjects. The number of

entries rose (with some fluctuation) between 1993 and 1996 but has declined thereafter. The

4 The dataset used here does not disaggregate between A-levels and SCE Highers. This dataset was deemed relevant because young people in Scotland might apply to HEIs in England and Wales, and young people in England and Wales might apply to HEIs in Scotland.

Page 13: Gender Differences

13

percentage of total entries has been relatively stable at between 9 and 10 percent till 2001,

with a decline to just over 8% since then. Entries as a percentage of the age cohort follow a

similar pattern to the number of entries but the fluctuations are greater.

3. Spanish is the exception to the general pattern of decline in Languages. Entries have risen (as

raw numbers, a percentage of total entries and as a percentage of the age cohort entered for

examination in the subject). Indeed, the graphs suggest that entries for German have

declined at the same time as entries for Spanish have risen: the two appear to be converging.

4. Data for physics, chemistry and geography reveal a general trend of declining entries with

some minor fluctuation. Entries for chemistry, however, rose between 2003 and 2004.

3.4 Time series data – A-Level/SCE disaggregated by gender

A-Level/SCE data disaggregated by gender is displayed in Appendix 2. With the exception of

chemistry, gender gaps are greater than they are for GCSE. Some researchers (e.g. Stables and

Stables 1997) suggest that once students have freer choice over which subjects to take, their

decisions are influenced by gender stereotypes to a greater extent than at GCSE when the

National Curriculum constrained choice. Such patterns could, however, also be influenced by what

subjects and courses educational institutions offer, option blocks, and timetabling constraints.

1. For chemistry, the number of entries for males was greater than that for females in the mid-

90s. However, towards the end of the decade numbers converged and from 2002 onwards

more females than males have been entered for advanced level chemistry. Entries for

chemistry A-Level/SCE remain higher as a percentage of all male entries than as a percentage

of all female entries, though the gap has narrowed.5

2. Entries for mathematics have fluctuated between 11% and 14% of total entries for boys, and

between 6% and 7% of total entries for girls. The gap between male and female entries has

reduced slightly, but has changed relatively little during the period examined.

3. The gap between male and female entries is largest for physics. As with mathematics the gap

has reduced slightly, but has remained relatively consistent over time.

5 The gap between male and female entries almost closed in 2001 (male Chemistry entries made up 5% of all male A-Level entries and female entries 4.89% of all female entries) but has been wider since 2002.

Page 14: Gender Differences

14

4. The number of female entries for MFL has been substantially higher than the number of male

entries throughout the period. The gap is more substantial for all languages at A-Level/SCE

than it was for GCSE. For French the gap in entries narrowed slightly: in 1994 25,000 females

and about 10,000 males were entered for French A-Level/SCE, while in 2004 the

corresponding numbers were about 13,000 female and 6,000 male. This shows that most of

the numerical decline in entries was among females, but the proportionate decline for males

and females was similar. A similar pattern can be observed for German: a consistently greater

number of female entries than male entries, the steeper numerical decline being in female

entries, and a relatively consistent gap between female and male entries (which has perhaps

decreased very slightly). In Spanish there is a relatively consistent gap between male and

female entries. However, unlike French and German, this is within the context of an overall

rise in entries for both males and females.

5. Geography A-Level, like geography GCSE, has consistently been taken by more males than

females, within an overall trend of declining entry. The gap between male and female entries

has remained relatively constant, although it narrows slightly after 2001. Geography entries

in 2004 were 55% male, 45% female (QCA 2005).

3.5 Additional data

The analysis of time series data can be supplemented by data collated by subject associations and

researchers. Some of this these additional data pre- or post-date that in the time series, allowing

the trends observed in the time series data to be contextualised within a longer time span. In

addition, survey and case study data point to differences between localities, types of school,

individual schools which are masked by overall trends in the aggregate data.

Geography

Some of the additional data available regarding Geography pre-date those in the time series. In

an analysis of geography examinations entry data for the Royal Geographical Society, Grimble and

Mansell (2004) note that geography GCSE entries have declined since 1988, with the overall

number of candidates falling by 77,743, except for a brief revival between 1994 and 1997. This is

in the context of a consistent increase in total GCSE entries year on year. For A-Level they note a

more erratic pattern, with a steady increase of entrants from 1989 to 1994, and a general pattern

of decline (but with fluctuations) since then. Walford (2000) found that for most of the twentieth

century geography entries at public examinations aged 16 and 18 improved both in absolute

numbers and in numbers in relation to other subjects year by year, peaking in 1994-96.

Page 15: Gender Differences

15

There are also significant differences between schools. QCA (2005) note that entry figures for

individual schools range from 0% to 80% of the cohort, that entries within an individual school

can fluctuate over time, and that geography appeared to maintain or strengthen its position in

more “academic” schools or schools with strong geography departments, even in the context of an

overall downward trend in entries 2000-04. Weeden (2005) similarly found differences between

schools in their rates of entry for geography GCSE.

MFL

Much of the additional data on Modern Foreign Languages examination entries is provided by the

CILT, the National Centre for Languages. For instance, the CILT website gives access to data on

A-Level entries between 1975 and 2003. These data indicate a 36.4% decrease in entries for

French, an 11% decrease for German, but a 124% rise in entries for Spanish (though from a very

low base). Within this overall time-frame, entries for all three languages increased between 1985

and 1995. These data also report A-Level and Scottish Highers separately and so reveal

differences between England and Scotland which are masked in the time series data (for instance

Higher entries for German and French in Scotland have risen since 2001 – after the introduction of

a new Higher in 2000 – while A-Level entries fell).6 This suggests that entry data for A-Level

alone would reveal starker declines in French and German entries than are evident in the DfES

dataset used.

In addition, the CILT Language Trends survey reveals regional and local differences, and

differences between types of school, beneath the aggregate national data. For instance, the 2004

survey found that of the schools surveyed those in the north of England were more likely to make

MFL optional than those in the south. The same survey also found variation between types of

schools, with languages at KS4 being compulsory in 97% of the independent schools but only

30% of the state schools surveyed. It also found that the independent schools surveyed reported

a strong growth in Spanish while 44% of the maintained schools surveyed reported a decrease in

Spanish provision. (CILT 2004c). Finally there is evidence of significant differences between

individual schools in their interpretation of ‘entitlement’, with some schools guiding only top sets

to do languages, while others see languages as open to all (CILT 2004c, QCA 2004c).

Mathematics and science

6 See http://www.scilt.stir.ac.uk/Languages_in_Scotland/index.htm and http://www.cilt.org.uk/research/statistics/education/index.htm#secondary [both accessed 4 October 2006].

Page 16: Gender Differences

16

Additional data on mathematics contextualises that in the time-series with a longer time-scale.

For example, London Mathematical Society reported that the number of A-Level mathematics

candidates fell from c.85,000 in 1989 to 53,000 in 2002, in the context of an increase in

participation in post-16 education and training over this period (LMS 2002). Institute of Physics

data demonstrates a rise in the proportion of female candidates for Mathematics A-Level from

33.6% in 1991 to 35.9% in 1998 (IoP 1999).

Institute Physics data (1999) also indicates that the low proportion of female candidates for A-

Level physics has been almost constant from 1988 to 1999 (at around 22% of physics

candidates), and also important differences between England and Scotland (where females made

up about 31% of physics Highers candidates in these years). Data for chemistry, on the other

hand, indicates a narrowing (and even a closing) of the gap in the number of A-Level entries for

males and females between 1985 and 2004.7

With mathematics and science being compulsory subjects in the national curriculum, it is

unsurprising that there is less additional evidence of differences between schools in entry rates

than there is for MFL and geography. Nevertheless, according to data on the Institute of Physics

website, single science subjects are more likely to be offered in independent than in state schools.

7 http://www.iop.org/Our_Activities/Science_Policy/Statistics/

Page 17: Gender Differences

17

4. Interpreting the trends

This section analyses how subject experts and relevant research literature have interpreted the

trends in examination entries outlined above. Evidence examined includes reports by government

agencies (particularly QCA and Ofsted), literature from subject associations, major reports on

particular subjects (e.g. Smith 2004), and research literature on subject choice, subject

preference, and pupil motivation.

4.1 The evidence base

Much of this evidence does not set out directly to explain trends in public examination entries in

these subjects, but offers useful insights into a number of concerns and issues behind the trends

observed. QCA and Ofsted reports aim to assess the general condition of the subject in question,

with examination entry data being only one factor considered. Nevertheless, they offer useful

insights into government curriculum policy, the subject curriculum and assessment, and the

teaching workforce which might influence trends in examination entries. Subject associations

provide both useful data and evidence on the views and experiences of their practitioner

members. However, the role of subject associations as “preservers” of their own subject and as

lobbying bodies should be borne in mind.

The research evidence is patchy. First, much of the literature does not deal directly with

examination entry patterns or even with subject choice. Studies on pupil attitudes and motivation

in the classroom, or of subject preference (as distinguished from choice) are more common, but

motivation and preference may not correlate with actual decisions over examination entry.

Nevertheless, insights into motivation and preference can offer useful insights which could,

potentially, help us understand the static or declining entry levels observed in most of these

subjects. Second, researchers can interpret concepts such as motivation or preference in different

ways. As Lord (2003) observes, some researchers investigate enjoyment, others positive

attitudes, others liking or not liking, others preference. This makes it difficult to develop a

cumulative picture of research findings.

4.2 Interpreting the trends in examination entries

A range number of issues have been raised in the evidence examined which might help us

understand the trends in examinations entries in geography, chemistry, physics, mathematics and

MFL.

Page 18: Gender Differences

18

4.2.1 Teaching and learning

For all the subjects included in this paper, downward or static trends in examination entries have

been attributed to poor teaching and a shortage of specialist teachers (see below). In some cases

this has been seen as a vicious circle, with a shortage of specialist teachers leading to poor

teaching and declining examination entries, which limits the supply of those qualified to become

specialist teachers in the future (e.g. Lipsett 2004).

Geography

Ofsted reports reveal that geography has performed poorly at KS3 compared with other subjects

in terms of teaching and student achievement, with most lessons being judged ‘satisfactory’

(Ofsted 2004a). Problems with teaching have been linked to problems with teacher recruitment,

leading to teaching by non-specialists particularly at KS3 and below and a lack of continuing

professional development opportunities for geography teachers (Lambert 2005; QCA 2004a; QCA

2005; Weeden 2005). QCA (2004a) also found a tendency to teach for coverage of examination

specification content rather than for the deep learning of ideas, leading to “safe” lessons rather

than motivational teaching (QCA 2004a). Research on pupils’ perspectives on geography lessons

also reveals that too much passive learning and a lack of variety in teaching approaches could put

pupils off geography at KS3 and KS4, with pupils appreciating more proactive, practical and varied

learning activities (Bidduph and Adey 2003; Biddulph and Adey 2004; Norman and Harrison 2004;

QCA 2005).

Westaway and Rawling (2001) suggest that the quality of teaching affects student choices at KS4,

that ‘satisfactory’ lessons at KS3 may not be enough to convince students to take geography at

GCSE. These authors also note that, despite improvements between KS3 and KS4, geography is

‘outperformed’ by history in terms of quality of teaching and student progress at KS4. Moreover,

as noted already, GCSE entry levels vary widely between schools. Factors identified as

contributing to higher levels of entry include: a reputation for successful teaching and good

examination results (the “school grapevine effect”), and specifications and/or approaches to

learning which allowed topical content to be covered and provided good quality fieldwork

experiences (QCA 2005; Weeden 2005; Westaway and Rawling 2001).

MFL

Teacher shortages, particularly for languages other than French, and problems with teacher

retention, have also been identified as serious issues for MFL (e.g. Aplin 1991; Fisher 2001; CILT

Page 19: Gender Differences

19

2005).8 Yet there is evidence that a good teacher, and a good student-teacher relationship, can

be a powerful influence on student attitudes. For instance, Clark and Trafford (1995) found that a

positive relationship with the teacher was more important than any other variable in influencing

pupils’ attitudes to the subject. Pupils appreciated qualities such as friendliness, professionalism

and an ability to make lessons lively. Similarly, Fisher (2001) found that, among the A-Level

linguists in her study, the teacher was of vital importance to their enjoyment or otherwise of the

GCSE course. Fisher suggests that the importance of pupil-teacher relationships is so important

in languages because of the communicative nature of the subject, which requires the teacher to

be able to communicate with and draw communication from pupils.

Research on subject preferences, however, suggests that languages are often among the least

preferred subjects (e.g. Stables and Wikeley 1999) and are, according to Williams et al. (2002)

seen as neither interesting or enjoyable. Fisher (2001) suggests that the issue is not so much that

pupils actively disliked languages, but more that other subjects were more enjoyable. She also

found that even when pupils liked the teacher, they often did not like what they were required to

do. These perceptions, it is suggested, could have a direct influence on both motivation and

subject choice (Fisher 2001, Williams et al. 2002).

Mathematics

Severe shortages of specialist teachers in mathematics have been widely reported (e.g. LMS

2004; Mathematical Association 2002a; Ofsted 2004b; Smith 2004; Watson 2005). For instance,

in a survey of about 400 schools QCA (2004b) found that about 20% of mathematics teachers did

not have a qualification in mathematics, and also that temporary contracts and vacancies were

common in a number of schools. This lack of specialists has been linked with ineffective

mathematics teaching. Furthermore, there have been criticisms of uninspiring and unimaginative

teaching. Ofsted (2004b) contend that teaching and learning styles in mathematics are limited

compared with other subjects, and that teachers often dictate too much instead of encouraging

pupils to find things out for themselves. Indeed, subject associations suggest that students can

be put off taking the subject at A-Level by rote learning and repetition in mathematics teaching up

to 16 (QCA 2004b).

Physics and Chemistry

There is a shortage of specialist teachers of physics and chemistry in many schools. Lipsett

(2004) sees this shortage of specialist teachers as a problem not only for A-Level teaching which

explicitly requires specialist knowledge, but also for GCSE even in the majority of schools which 8 In a debate in the House of Lords on 24 January 2005, Baroness Walmsley cited the most “recently available figures” which indicated that 380 secondary teachers of modern foreign languages recruited to the service in 1997 had left the service by 2003.

Page 20: Gender Differences

20

offer double award science. The physics and chemistry components of the double award GCSE

are, Lipsett argues, often taught by non-specialists who lack detailed subject knowledge and also

lack the enthusiasm necessary to inspire pupils to take the subject further.

4.2.2 Perceived difficulty

A second issue identified in the literature examined is that of perceived difficulty of these subjects.

Declining examination entries have been attributed to perceptions that geography, MFL,

mathematics, physics and chemistry are ‘difficult’ compared to others.

Geography

Evidence regarding the difficulty or otherwise of geography is mixed.9 On the one hand, there are

reports of anecdotal evidence that geography is perceived as the ‘easiest’ humanities subject at

GCSE (Westaway and Rawling 2001; QCA 2005). On the other hand, higher rates of entry for

Geography GCSE are found in “academic” schools which gain high GCSE results (QCA 2004a;

Weeden 2005). Also, quantitative evidence based on GCSE results (Stott et al. 1997) and

qualitative case study evidence from one school (Weeden 2005) suggests that the cohort which

opts for geography is weighted towards high achievers. Elements of difficulty identified by

teachers and students include the quantity of work involved, the requirement to think

independently and express opinions, and the need to use geographical terminology and concepts

(Biddulph and Adey 2003; QCA 2005; Weeden 2005).

Furthermore, it appears that elements of ‘difficulty’ such as the need to think independently may

put some pupils off, but appeal to others as a positive challenge (Biddulph and Adey 2003; QCA

2005). Thus the evidence available does not allow a firm conclusion on whether the perceived

difficulty of GCSE geography discourages students from pursuing the subject further at A-Level

(Westaway and Rawling 2001)

MFL

There is a much evidence that both teachers and students perceive languages as difficult.

Research reveals that pupils feel that innate ability in the subject is important, that you have to

be “good at languages” to succeed (Graham 2002; Stables and Stables 1995). Furthermore,

students have argued that a GCSE in a foreign language is more demanding than other GCSE

subjects, particularly in demands on concentration and memory (Clark and Trafford 1995), with

even the highest attaining students, including those who went on to take languages at A-Level,

finding MFL difficult compared with other subjects at GCSE (Fisher 2001). Researchers (Fisher

2001; Graham 2002) have also found a discrepancy between pupils’ internal perceptions of

9 This evidence on the perceived difficulty of geography relates mainly to GCSE.

Page 21: Gender Differences

21

progress or ability and their predicted or actual GCSE grade. Even pupils with a good grade at

GCSE did not feel competent in the language, perhaps owing to unrealistic expectations of fluency

(Graham 2002).

Perceptions of difficulty appear to discourage students from taking languages at GCSE and

beyond. There is evidence that less academic students have been discouraged from taking

languages at GCSE and beyond, either through their own lack of confidence, or through school

timetabling and options arrangements and/or the careers advice they receive (Aplin 1991; Atlas

2003a; Atlas 2003b; CILT 2004c; QCA 2004c; Smith 2004). Difficulty was the most common

reason which the Year 11 students in Graham’s (2002) study gave for not wanting to continue

studying French beyond GCSE. Fisher (2001) suggests that students who may have considered

pursuing a language post-16 were “warned of the difficulty of the subject” by older students

already engaged in A-Level study, while Graham (2002) also found that year 12 pupils perceived

a big jump between elements of the GCSE course and A-Level study.

Mathematics, Physics and Chemistry

There have been persistent claims about the relative difficulty of mathematics and physical

science from pupils, teachers and professional associations. What is the research evidence for

these claims? Bell, Malacova and Shannon (2003) contend that mathematics AS and A-Level has

increasingly become the preserve of pupils with overall high levels of attainment (as represented

by GCSE results). Fitz-Gibbon (1999) argues, on the basis of a statistical analysis of A-Level

grades linked with prior attainment at GCSE, that it is more difficult to get a high grade in

Mathematics and science (and MFL) than in most other subjects, and that this indicates the

relative difficulty of these subjects. However, Goldstein and Cresswell (1996) highlight difficulties

in assessing “standards” and particularly in making comparisons between subjects and over time.

Whatever the technical issues over measuring standards and comparing the ‘difficulty’ or

otherwise of different subjects, many teachers and pupils evidently perceive mathematics and

science as particularly difficult. Pupils gave lack of self-confidence in ability in these subjects as a

key reason for not liking the subject at GCSE (Gallagher et al. 1997; Stables and Stables 1995;

Stables and Wikeley 1997), while students who disliked arts subjects tended to describe them as

boring and pointless rather than difficult (Gallagher et al. 1997). There is also a gender

dimension to these perceptions: for instance, some of the girls in Gallagher et al.’s (1997) study

perceived physics as a ‘male’ subject which boys were better at than girls. On the other hand,

QCA (2004b) attribute negative views of mathematics to the design of the GCSE qualification,

Page 22: Gender Differences

22

particularly the quantity of work involved and the three-tier structure,10 more than issues of ability

per se.

Perceptions of difficulty have been linked to declining or static A-Level entries in these subjects.

With reference to mathematics, QCA (2004b) suggest that students find the higher tier GCSE

harder than GCSE papers in other subjects which are not divided into tiers, and that this puts

them off progressing to A-Level. Furthermore, it has been argued that examination entries for

mathematics and science have suffered because pupils, sometimes with encouragement from

their teachers, have opted for ‘easier’ subjects in order to maximise A-Level examination scores

(e.g. ACME 2004; Fitz-Gibbon 1999, LMS 2003). On the other hand, Garratt (1986) found that the

perceived level of difficulty was the factor which had least influence on A-Level subject choice for

students taking science A-Levels.11

4.2.3 Curriculum and assessment design

Geography

Dated and content-heavy examination specifications, and a lack of curriculum time to cover these

specifications, have been identified by teachers as important factors in declining numbers (QCA

2004a). GCSE and AS/A-Level geography courses have not been subject to a full curriculum

review since the mid-1980s, and commentators (e.g. Rawling 1991) note that exciting

developments in the academic subject of geography in universities have not filtered down to the

school curriculum. A new geography GCSE with focus on cultural, social and economic geography,

and active learning approaches is currently being piloted (QCA 2005).

What light does research on pupils’ attitudes and decisions shed on this issue? Many researchers,

as noted above, see teaching processes as the most important factor in forming pupils’ attitudes

to geography. Norman and Harrison, on the other hand, note that “subject content, as much as

the mode of delivery,” is important to students (Norman and Harrison 2004, p.11)12 There is also

evidence that pupils dislike the tendency of GCSE and A-level courses to repeat topics from KS3

(QCA 2005).

10 QCA has now introduced a two-tier structure for mathematics GCSE, for first teaching in 2006, on a similar model to two-tier GCSEs in other subjects such as English and science. Students entered for the Foundation Tier can achieve Grades C-G, those entered for the Higher Tier Grades A*-C. QCA (2006). 11 It should be noted that this study was conducted before examination league tables and the UCAS point system were introduced. 12 This comment relates to Year 9 students.

Page 23: Gender Differences

23

Findings from the pilot GCSE are instructive. While most teachers welcomed the new style of

teaching and learning, there were concerns over the risks involved. Some teachers preferred to

stick to the well-resourced and familiar traditional specifications. There is a potential conflict,

therefore between pupils’ desire for new content, and teacher’s desire for continuity and

consistency of results (QCA 2005). The same report also noted a danger that exam papers and

assessment tasks could move away from the intended innovative content and approaches to

learning.

MFL

The content of the GCSE language curriculum has been criticised. Research indicates pupils’

frustration at a repetitive curriculum and the lack of a cultural dimension, and a feeling that what

they learned was unrealistic and mundane. Teachers also criticized a vocabulary-driven and

packed syllabus which left little time to practice new language or to use language for genuine

communication. As Clark and Trafford put it: “The communicative approach underpinning the

GCSE syllabus may all too often have given pupils merely an unsystematic, repetitive

acquaintanceship with undemanding, perhaps anodyne, transactional language” (Clark and

Trafford 1995, p.321; Fisher 2001).

Mathematics

There is a consensus in the literature examined that much mathematics teaching has been geared

to preparing students for examinations (Ofsted 2006). The emphasis on external assessment in

the subject has been seen to limit opportunities for enrichment or ensuring that pupils understand

and are able to manipulate mathematical concepts (LMS 2003; Watson 2004; Ofsted 2006), and

to undermine pupils’ enthusiasm for the subject (ACME 2005). Moreover, particular problems

have been identified with the three-tier design of mathematics GCSE. There is evidence that

schools can sometimes ‘play safe’ by entering some pupils who might have been capable of the

higher tier paper for the middle tier. This strategy ensures that, for school league table purposes,

the pupil can achieve a ‘good’ GCSE pass and avoids the risk of failure in the higher tier.

Nevertheless, some pupils could be stopped from progressing to A-Level. (e.g. Smith 2002; LMS

2003; QCA 2003).13

Physics and Chemistry

Ofsted (2004d) suggest that pupils are not motivated by the “heavily examination-oriented”

science GCSE curriculum. This finding is reinforced by research by Parkinson et al. (1998) found

that 13-14 year old pupils who had enjoyed practical work in science lessons felt less motivated

13 C is the top grade available for the middle tier paper.

Page 24: Gender Differences

24

when the level of practical work declined at KS4. The Applied Science GCSE, with a greater

practical focus, could potentially be more motivating (Ofsted 2004d).

4.2.4 Public image of the subject

Geography

The evidence evaluated highlights problems with the public image of geography. Lambert (2005)

and Rawling (2001) have noted a public perception of geography as a traditional, utilitarian

subject, involving low level learning about countries and natural landscapes, while Brown (2001)

suggests that geography lacks “street cred” with potential undergraduates. Moreover, the

evidence suggests that pupils see the utility of geography in a narrow way, limited to reading

maps, knowledge about countries that can be used when traveling. Pupils also link geography to

careers in a naïve way, seeing it as useful only for jobs in the travel industry (Adey and Biddulph

2001; Biddulph and Adey 2003; Biddulph and Adey 2004).

MFL

As with geography, the literature examined highlights pupil perceptions that languages are not

useful in employment, and misconceptions about the kind of employment opportunities open

through the study of languages (Atlas 2003a; Aplin 1991; Blenkinsop et al. 2006; Clark and

Trafford 2005; Graham 2002; Stables and Wikeley 1999; Williams et al. 2002). Misconceptions

are attributed to inadequate guidance from careers advisors or parents (Aplin 1991; Fisher 2001).

Graham (2002) also found that a perceived usefulness was among the most often mentioned

reasons given for not continuing with French post-16 (Graham 2002). Other studies, however,

found that students were aware of the importance of languages for employment, but saw other

subjects as more important for career progression (e.g. Fisher 2001; ATLAS 2003a; ATLAS

2003b).

Another possible indication of a public image problem is the declining popularity of MFL over time

in subject preference studies (Colley, Comber and Hargreaves 1994a; Colley and Comber 2003;

Stables and Wikeley 1997; Wikeley and Stables 1999). There are also gender differences in

attitudes, with boys often found to be more negative than girls. Some researchers suggest that

boys may be put off by lack of male role models (Clark and Trafford 1995; Williams et al. 2002)

Mathematics

The influential Smith Report (Smith 2004) argued that young people were often perceived maths

as “boring and irrelevant”, and were unaware of the importance of mathematical skills for future

career options and advancement. QCA (2004b) concur that pupils do not see their mathematical

skills and knowledge as useful in themselves. Nevertheless, there is evidence that pupils are

Page 25: Gender Differences

25

aware that mathematics GCSE is important for progression to a university place or to employment

(Blenkinsop et al. 2006; QCA 2004b; Stables and Wilkeley 1999).

Physics and Chemistry

The evidence reviewed suggests that students perceived the career usefulness of physics and

chemistry, but are less aware of the significance of science in their everyday lives (Blenkinsop et

al. 2006; Parkinson et al. 1998). The research also suggests gender differences in attitudes.

Parkinson et al. (1998) found that boys had more positive attitudes to science than girls. The girls

who took part in research by Gallagher et al. (1997) and Garratt (1986) felt that boys were better

at physics than they were, and also that physics was appropriate for careers suited to boys more

than careers suited to girls.

The issues discussed so far have been pertinent to all subjects covered by this paper. The issues

which follow relate only to some subjects.

4.2.5. Government curriculum policy

Geography

The evidence reviewed suggests that geography, along with other subjects decoupled from the

compulsory national curriculum after 1995, have been squeezed as the time devoted to core

subjects has been increased to 70%. Brown (2001) argues that “geographers everywhere are alarmed

that this vital discipline is under attack, not as a deliberate policy, but by default, because the government’s

priorities are elsewhere.” Norman and Harrison (2004) suggest that geography is marginalized even before

KS4, with the emphasis on literacy and numeracy at KS3. These curriculum policies are seen as problematic

not so much in themselves but in combination with options and timetabling procedures in schools

(Walford 2000; QCA 2005). Nevertheless, we lack the evidence which allows us to argue that changes in

curriculum policy in themselves lead to changes in examination entries.

It has also been argued that the uptake of geography, along with other optional subjects at GCSE,

has been affected by the rise in the number of alternative subjects (Grimble and Mansell 2004;

QCA 2004a; Weeden 2005; Rawling 2001). This has been seen as a Bell’s (2001) quantitative

analysis of examination entries, however, reveals that students generally took more subjects in

1997 than they did in 1984. Westaway and Rawling (2001) suggested that the impact of pre-16

vocational qualifications (such as Part 1 GNVQ leisure and tourism) which could potentially attract

students who otherwise might have taken geography GCSE was probably limited, as the take up

of these vocational programmes was low. The greater opportunities to take alternatives to GCSE

through the Increased Flexibility Programme could potentially have a greater impact on GCSE

entries for geography and other optional subjects.

Page 26: Gender Differences

26

MFL

There has been a sharp decline in numbers taking GCSEs in French and German within a few months since

MFL was made an entitlement subject in September 2004. Teachers have suggested that making MFL

entitlement subject sent out negative message about its status (CILT 2005; QCA 2004c). Stables and

Wikeley (1999), however, noted a worsening of attitudes of 14-15 year old pupils in relation to subject

preference, and no real improvement in attitudes as to career usefulness, between 1984 and 1996 when MFL

was compulsory under the national curriculum.

4.2.6. Modular structure of post-16 qualifications

It has been argued that the modular structure of post-16 qualifications is a disadvantage for

subjects which are linear and cumulative in nature, such as mathematics and modern foreign

languages. Mathematics has been described as “cumulative and interconnected” and “intrinsically

interlinked”, and subject associations have argued that modular assessment tests only fragments

and does not provide the opportunities for practice and reinforcement which students need (LMS

2003; LMS 2005; Mathematical Association 2003c). This view was also expressed by mathematics

tutors who took part in focus groups conducted at HEIs as part of the work of the Nuffield Review.

Similarly, MFL has been described as a “sequential subject” (Clark and Trafford 1995). Although

modularity has been noted as a hindrance to effective learning in these subjects, there is no clear

evidence that this has a direct impact on examination entries. However, LMS (2005) suggest that

under the UCAS tariff system (where subjects are of equal worth), modular structure is damaging

for sequential subjects which depend on “second passes” and reinforcement more than other

subjects do.

4.2.7 Double Award science and progression to A-Level

The vast majority of pupils now take double award science at GCSE rather than single science

subjects. The advent of double award science means that many females now take chemistry and

science in a significant way to age 16 (subjects which were previously heavily male dominated)

(e.g. IoP 1999). However, problems with progression from Double Award GCSE to A-Level

science subjects have been noted. Science teachers have argued that the step in content from

GCSE to AS level was too big, especially for non-mathematicians (QCA 2004d). This view was

echoed by HE admissions tutors in our focus groups. That HE admissions tutors are concerned

about this could, if these views influence admissions decisions, prove a barrier for social inclusion

and widening participation, as take-up of single science is concentrated in selective schools.

4.2.8 Problems with the design of GCSE and AS-Level specifications

For some of the subjects examined, problems with the design of GCSE and AS-Level specifications

have been identified as a barrier to further progression in that subject. In the case of geography,

Page 27: Gender Differences

27

schools have expressed concerns that in the original AS specification under Curriculum 2000 there

was too much content to cover in the time available, and that this discouraged pupils to continue

to A-Level (QCA 2004a). For MFL, it has been argued that ‘teething problems’ with the revised

GCSE specifications, which were revealed in the sharp drop in the proportion of students getting

A*-C for the first cohort examined on the basis of the new specifications in 2003, was linked to

the sharp decline in GCSE entry levels in 2004. It has also been argued that the difficulty of AS

specifications could discourage students taking AS from progressing to A-Level. (QCA 2004c).

Criticisms of mathematics examination specifications have been particularly severe (and highly

publicized). The sharp decline in the number of A-Level candidates in 2002 was attributed to initial

problems with the design of the AS and A-Level mathematics specifications under Curriculum

2000. (Mathematical Association 2002a; QCA 2004b; Ofsted 2004b; Smith 2004). (Although

entries rose slightly in 2003 and 2004 they are still far below their 2001 level.) Much criticism has

been directed at AS specifications. ACME (2005) have argued that a rushed AS experience which

left students no time to consolidate or practice new techniques or to understand new concepts, as

reflected in poor results and high failure rates, deterred some students from taking the subject on

to a second year (See also Mathematics Association 2002b). Part of the problem also seems to be

with the transition between GCSE and AS/A-Level; it has been argued that the gap is too large

and that GCSE offers insufficient foundation in important techniques and concepts such as algebra

(Smith 2004; QCA 2004b). Also, as noted previously, it appears that students entered for the

middle tier exam of the three-tier mathematics GCSE may have trouble progressing to higher

levels of study in the subject.

Page 28: Gender Differences

28

5. The effects of changing patterns of subject take-up

What have commentators perceived to be the effects of the changing patterns of subject take-up

observed? These perceived effects are varied: from an impact on HE, to an impact on the ‘status’

of the subject as a whole, to an impact on the national economy and international

competitiveness, to social polarization.

Impact on higher education

There are complex economic and political reasons for the closure of university departments, but a

decline in the supply of students with appropriate qualifications has been seen by some

commentators as a major contributory factor in the closure of degree courses and university

departments (e.g. Smith 2006; McLeod 2004a; McLeod 2004b; Curtis 2005). Furthermore, focus

groups discussions conducted as part of the work of the Nuffield Review in a range of HEIs

revealed that chemistry, physics, and languages departments, even in highly selective

institutions, were often in ‘recruitment’ mode and admissions tutors said they found it hard to

attract candidates with the qualifications or other attributes which they desired.

Loss of the specific contribution to understanding the world which the subject could

make

QCA noted regarding geography that the decline in GCSE and A-Level entries could potentially

affect “the level of geographical understanding in the nation as a whole.” (QCA 2005 p.9) Brown

(2001) argued in a similar vein that declining take-up of the subject potentially depriving citizens

of skills and knowledge vital to an understanding of the world and for meeting future

environmental and political challenges. In the case of MFL it has been argued that a decline in

languages could lead to an inability to appreciate cultural diversity which is vital in a multi-cultural

society and a globalised world (e.g. NfER 2003).

Damage to the economy

Concerns have been expressed about the potential damage which a decline in numbers taking

mathematics and science subjects at Level 3 and above could inflict on the national economy.

This has been a mantra in successive government documents, for example the SET for success

report of 2002 (Roberts 2002). Similarly, it has been argued that a decline in numbers taking

languages will have a negative impact on country’s economic output and ability to do business

(Fisher 2001; QCA 2004c) and on the competitive position of individual applicants within a

European labour market.14

14 Language specialists and non-specialists alike raised this particular concern in focus groups with HEI admissions staff conducted for the Nuffield Review.

Page 29: Gender Differences

29

Social polarisation

Fears of polarisation have been noted particularly in relation to MFL. Given the evidence that MFL

is most likely to be optional in state schools, especially those in deprived areas, several

commentators are concerned that languages will again become elitist subjects and lose the broad

base which they attained under the national curriculum (QCA 2003; CILT 2004c; CILT 2005). In

addition, admissions tutors have expressed a preference for single subject science GCSEs over

double award for the purposes of progression to HE (Wilde et al., 2006). This could be a cause for

concern, as single science is concentrated in selective schools.

Page 30: Gender Differences

30

Conclusion

Three main points can be made in conclusion.

First, this examination has highlighted the importance of looking not only at the raw numbers of

examination entries as an indication of change over time. Clearly, headlines which only report the

number of examination entries in a subject (without reference to the total number of GCSE or A-

Level entries or the age cohort) could give a misleading impression.

Second, there are serious weaknesses in the evidence base which limit the strength of the

conclusions that can be drawn. Inconsistencies in methods of data-collection over time are

problematic for the construction and analysis of time-series data.

Third, the evidence examined has not provided robust causal explanations of declining or static

examination entries. This investigation has revealed common issues across the different subjects

examined. Nevertheless, many of the recurring issues raised (a restrictive curriculum and

assessment regime dominated by content rather than process, narrow teaching methods) have

been raised as concerns in relation to subjects generally perceived to be in a ‘healthier’ state

(including history and English). Some of these concerns appear to be part of the ‘system’ as a

whole, and are not peculiar to these subjects.

Page 31: Gender Differences

31

References

Adey, K. and Biddulph, M. (2001) The influence of pupil perceptions on subject choice at 14+ in

geography and history, Educational Studies, 27 (4), 439-50.

Aplin, R. (1991) Why do pupils opt out of foreign language courses? A pilot study, Educational Studies,

17 (1), 3-13

ATLAS Project (2003a) Survey summary results. At: http://www.ucl.ac.uk/calt/atlas/atlas-

focusgroups.doc. Accessed 30 August 2005.

ATLAS Project (2003b) Focus groups. At: http://www.ucl.ac.uk/calt/atlas/atlas-questionnaire-report.doc.

Accessed 30 August 2005.

Bell, J. F. (2001) Patterns of subject uptake and examination entry 1984-1997, Educational Studies, 27

(2) 201-19.

Bell, J. and Forster, M. (2001) Patterns of uptake of modern foreign language examinations in England

1984 to 1999, Language Learning Journal, 2001, 24, 48-52.

Bell, J., Malacova, E. and Shannon, M. (2003) The changing pattern of A-level/AS uptake in England.

Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association Annual Conference, Edinburgh,

September 2003.

Biddulph, M. and Adey, K. (2003) Perception v. reality: pupils’ experiences of learning in history and

geography at Key Stage 4, The Curriculum Journal, 14 (3), 291-303.

Biddulph, M. and Adey, K. (2004) Pupil perceptions of effective teaching and subject relevance in history

and geography at Key Stage 3, Research in Education, 71, 1-8.

Birnie, J. (1999) Physical geography at the transition to higher education: the effect of prior learning,

Journal of Geography in Higher Education, 23 (1), 49-62.

Blenkinsop, S., McCrone, T., Morris, M. and Wade, P. (2006) How do young people make choices at age

14 and age 16. DfES Research Report RR773. Nottingham: DfES.

Brown, P. (2001) The erosion of geography, Education Guardian, 20 November.

CILT (2004c) Language trends 2004: Languages in Key Stage 4. At:

http://www.cilt.org.uk/key/Language%20Trends%202004.pdf. Accessed 6 September 2005.

CILT (2005) House of Lords 24 January 2005. At: http://www.cilt.org.uk/key/trends2004_lords.htm.

Accessed 30 August 2005.

Clark, A. and Trafford, J. (1995) Boys into modern languages: An investigation of the discrepancy in

attitudes and performance between boys and girls in Modern Languages, Gender and Education, 7

(3), 315-25.

Colley, A., Comber, C., Hargreaves, D. (1994a) Gender effects in school subject preferences: a research

note, Educational Studies, 20 (1), 13-19.

Colley, A. and Comber, C. (2003) School subject preferences: age and gender differences revisited,

Educational Studies, 29 (1), 59-67.

Page 32: Gender Differences

32

Curtis, P. (2005) Languages cull at Oxford Brookes, Education Guardian, 11 July.

Davies, P., Telhaj, S., Hutton, D., Adnett, N. and Coe, R. (2004a) Institutional and social background

effects on the probability of taking an examination subject at age 16. IEPR Working Paper

2004/14. At: http://www.staffs.ac.uk/schools/business/iepr/publications/working-papers.html.

Accessed 4 Jan 2005.

Davies, P., Telhaj, S., Hutton, D., Adnett, N. and Coe, R. (2004b) The myth of the bog standard

secondary school: a school level analysis of students’ choice of optional subjects. IEPR Working

Paper 2004/13. At: http://www.staffs.ac.uk/schools/business/iepr/publications/working-

papers.html. Accessed 4 Jan 2005.

DfES (2005) GCSE and equivalent results and associated value added measures for young people in

England 2003/04 (Final). DfES Statistical First Release SFR25/2005.

Fitz-Gibbon, C. T. (1999) Long-term consequences of curriculum choices with particular reference to

mathematics and science, School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 10 (2), 217-252.

Gallagher, T., McEwen, A., Knife, D. (1997) Science education policy: a survey of the participation of

sixth form pupils in sciences over a 10 year period, 1985-1995, Research Papers in Education, 12,

121-42.

Garratt, L. (1985) Factors affecting subject choice at A-Level, Educational Studies, 11, 127-32.

Garratt, L. (1986) Gender differences in relation to science choice at A level, Educational Review, 38, 67-

77.

Graham, S. (2002) Experiences of learning French: a snapshot at years 11, 12 and 13, Language

Learning Journal, 25, 15-20.

Goldstein, H. and Cresswell, M. (1996) The comparability of different subjects in public examinations: a

theoretical and practical critique, Oxford Review of Education, 22 (4), 435-442.

Grimble, L and Mansell, J. (2004) Analysis of the 2004 examination results and the current status of

Geography in England, Wales and Northern Ireland. At:

http://www.rgs.org/category.php?Page=maineducation. Accessed 30 August 2005.

Grimwade, K. (1997) Part 1 GNVQs: what implications do they have for geography? Teaching Geography,

22, 140-42.

Hendley, D. and Parkinson, J. (1995) Gender differences in pupil attitudes to the National Curriculum

foundation subjects of English, Mathematics, Science and Technology in Key Stage 3 in South

Wales, Educational Studies, 21 (1), 85-97.

Hoyles, C., Newman, K. and Noss, R. (2001) Changing patterns of t4ransition from school to university

mathematics, International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 32 (6),

829-45.

Institute of Physics (1999) GCSE and A-Level, 8.7 Physics Statistics 1 October 1999. At:

http://www.policy.iop.org/Policy/8/7.doc. Accessed 30 August 2005.

Page 33: Gender Differences

33

Kahn, P.E. and Hoyles, C. (1997) The changing undergraduate experience: a case study of single

honours mathematics in England and Wales, Studies in Higher Education, 22 (3), 349-62.

Lambert, D. (2005) Why subjects really matter. Paper presented to the Nuffield Review 18 February

2005.

Lipsett, A. (2004) Its a Downward spiral of decline, Times Higher Education Supplement, 17 December.

LMS (2005) Letter to the Secretary of State, 26 January 2005. At: http://www.lms.ac.uk. Accessed 15

August 2005.

Lord, P. and Hartland, J. (2000) Pupils’ experiences of the National Curriculum: Research review.

Lord, P. (2001) Pupils’ experiences and perspectives of the National Curriculum: Updating the research

review

Lord, P. (2002) Pupils’ experiences and perspectives of the National Curriculum: Updating the research

review

Lord, P. (2003) Pupils’ experiences and perspectives of the National Curriculum: Updating of the research

review 2002-2003

McLeod, D. (2004a) Senate backs Exeter closure plans, Education Guardian, 2 December.

McLeod, D. (2004b) Keele closes German department, Education Guardian, 29 December.

Mathematical Association (2002a) Press release in response to the report of the Post 14 Inquiry

Mathematical Association (2003c) League Tables and Targets. Why are they a particular problem for

mathematics? At: http://www.m-

a.org.uk/whats_new/Post_14_inquiry_league_tables_and_targets. Accessed 30 August 2005.

Miller, D., Parkhouse, P., Eagle, R. and Evans, T. (1999) Pupils and the core subjects: A study of the

attitudes of some pupils aged 11-16, Paper presented at the British Educational Research

Association Annual Conference, University of Sussex, Brighton, September 1999.

Norman, M. and Harrison, L. (2004) Year 9 students’ perceptions of school geography, Teaching

Geography, 29 (1), 11-15.

Ofsted (2004a) Standards and quality 2002/03. The annual report of Her Majesty’s Chief Inspector of

Schools. London: Ofsted.

Ofsted (2004b) Ofsted subject reports 2002/03. Mathematics in secondary schools. London: Ofsted.

Ofsted (2006) Evaluating mathematics provision for 14-19 year-olds. London: Ofsted.

Parkinson, J., Hendley, D., Tanner, H. and Stables, A. (1998) Pupils’ attitudes to science in Key Stage 3

of the National Curriculum: A study of pupils in South Wales, Research in Science and

Technological Education, 16 (2), 165-76.

QCA (2004a) Geography. 2003/04 annual report on curriculum and assessment. London: QCA.

QCA (2004b) Mathematics. 2003/04 annual report on curriculum and assessment. London: QCA.

QCA (2004c) Modern Foreign Languages. 2003/04 annual report on curriculum and assessment. London:

QCA.

QCA (2004d) Science. 2003/04 annual report on curriculum and assessment. London: QCA.

Page 34: Gender Differences

34

QCA (2005) Geography. 2004/5 annual report on curriculum and assessment. London: QCA.

QCA (2006) Changes to GCSE mathematics: A two-tier model. London: QCA.

Rawling, E. (2001) Changing the Subject. The impact of national policy on school geography 1980-2000.

London: Geographical Association.

Roberts, G. (2002) SET for Success: The supply of people with science, technology, engineering and

mathematical skills.

Smith, A. (2004) Making Mathematics Count. The report of Professor Adrian Smith’s inquiry into post-14

mathematics education. London: HMSO.

Smith, A. (2006) Plummeting student numbers jeapordise chemistry, Education Guardian, 16 August

2006.

Stables, A. and Stables, S. (1995) Gender differences in students’ approaches to A-level subject choices

and perceptions of A-level subjects: a study of first-year A-level students in a tertiary college,

Educational Research, 37 (1), 39-51.

Stables, A. and Wikeley, F. (1997) Changes in preference for and perceptions of relative importance of

subjects during a period of educational reform, Educational Studies, 23 (3) 393-403

Stables, A. and Wikeley, F. (1999) From bad to worse? Pupils’ attitudes to modern foreign language at

ages 14 and 15, Language Learning Journal, 20, 27-31.

Stott, T., Howard, R. and Linnett, R. (1997) What influence students’ choice of geography at GCSE?,

Teaching Geography, 22, 192-93.

Watson, A. (2005) Maths 14-19: its nature, significance, concepts and modes of engagement. Paper

presented to the Nuffield Review 18 May 2005.

Watson, A (2004) Red herrings: Post-14 ‘best’ mathematics teaching and curricula, British Journal of

Educational Studies, 52 (4), 359-76.

Weeden, P. (2005) Subject choice at 14. The role and influence of subject leaders. Paper presented at

BERA Conference, 14-17 September 2005.

Westaway, J. and Rawling, E. (2001) The rises and falls of geography, Teaching Geography, 26 (3), 108-

11.

Wilde, S., Wright, S., Hayward, G., Johnson, J. and Skerrett, R. (2006) Nuffield Review Higher Education

Focus Groups: Preliminary report. [online] At: http://www.nuffield14-19review.org.uk

Wikeley, F. and Stables, A. (1999) Changes to school students’ approaches to subject option choices: a

study of pupils in the West of England in 1984 and 1996, Educational Research, 41 (3), 287-99.

Williams, M., Burden, R. and Lanvers, U. (2002) ‘French is the language of love and stuff’: student

perceptions of issues related to motivation in learning a foreign language’, British Educational

Research Journal, 28 (4), 503-28.

Page 35: Gender Differences

35

Appendix 1: GCSE entries disaggregated by gender

Chemistry GCSE entries (England)

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE Chemistry entries as % of total entries

0.00

0.20

0.40

0.60

0.80

1.00

1.20

1.40

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Physics GCSE entries (England)

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE Physics entries as % of total entries

0.00

0.20

0.40

0.60

0.80

1.00

1.20

1.40

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Page 36: Gender Differences

36

Double Award Science GCSE entries (England)

0

50000

100000

150000

200000

250000

300000

350000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

Mathematics GCSE entries (England)

0

50000

100000

150000

200000

250000

300000

350000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE Mathematics entries as % of total entries

10.50

11.00

11.50

12.00

12.50

13.00

13.50

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

malefemale

GCSE Double Award Science entries as % of totalentries

11.00

11.50

12.00

12.50

13.00

13.50

14.00

14.50

15.00

1994199519961997 199819992000 2001200220032004

year

male

female

Page 37: Gender Differences

37

French GCSE entries (England)

0

20000

40000

60000

80000

100000

120000

140000

160000

180000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE French entries as % of total entries

0.00

1.00

2.00

3.00

4.00

5.00

6.00

7.00

8.00

9.00

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

German GCSE entries (England)

0

10000

20000

30000

40000

50000

60000

70000

80000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE German entries as % of total entries

0.00

0.50

1.00

1.50

2.00

2.50

3.00

3.50

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Page 38: Gender Differences

38

Spanish GCSE entries (England)

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

35000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE Spanish entries as % of total entries

0.00

0.20

0.40

0.60

0.80

1.00

1.20

1.40

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Geography GCSE entries (England)

0

20000

40000

60000

80000

100000

120000

140000

160000

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

GCSE Geography entries as % of total entries

0.00

1.00

2.00

3.00

4.00

5.00

6.00

7.00

8.00

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

smale

female

Page 39: Gender Differences

39

Appendix 2: A-Level/SCE Higher entries disaggregated by gender

Chemistry A-Level/Higher entries

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

35000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

A-Level/Higher Chemistry entries as % of total entries

0.00

1.00

2.00

3.00

4.00

5.00

6.00

7.00

8.00

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Physics A-Level/Higher entries

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

35000

40000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

A-Level/Higher Physics entries as % of total entries

0.001.002.003.004.005.006.007.008.009.00

10.00

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Page 40: Gender Differences

40

A-Level/Higher Mathematics entries as % of total entries

0.00

2.00

4.00

6.00

8.00

10.00

12.00

14.00

16.00

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Mathematics A-Level/Higher entries

0

10000

20000

30000

40000

50000

60000

70000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

French A-Level/Higher entries

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

A-Level/Higher French entries as % of total entries

0.00

1.00

2.00

3.00

4.00

5.00

6.00

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Page 41: Gender Differences

41

German A-Level/Higher entries

0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

6000

7000

8000

9000

10000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

A-Level/Higher German entries as % of total entries

0.00

0.50

1.00

1.50

2.00

2.50

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Spanish A-Level/Higher entries

0

500

1000

1500

2000

2500

3000

3500

4000

4500

5000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

A-Level/Higher Spanish entries as % total entries

0.000.100.200.300.400.500.600.700.800.901.00

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% to

tal e

ntrie

s

male

female

Page 42: Gender Differences

42

Geography A-Level/Higher entries

0

5000

10000

15000

20000

25000

30000

35000

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

num

ber o

f ent

ries

male

female

Geography A-Level/Higher entries as % of age cohort

0.001.002.003.004.005.006.007.008.009.00

10.00

1993

1994

1995

1996

1997

1998

1999

2000

2001

2002

2003

2004

year

% a

ge c

ohor

t

male

female