EVALUATING TIME OF DAY INFLUENCE ON ACHIEVEMENT, AN ...

106
EVALUATING TIME OF DAY INFLUENCE ON ACHIEVEMENT, ENGAGEMENT, AND BEHAVIOR FOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS: AN INVESTIGATION OF EFFECTIVE INSTRUCTIONAL STRATEGIES A Dissertation Presented to The Faculty of the Education Department Carson-Newman University In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Education By Rachel V. Brouillette June 2020

Transcript of EVALUATING TIME OF DAY INFLUENCE ON ACHIEVEMENT, AN ...

EVALUATING TIME OF DAY INFLUENCE ON ACHIEVEMENT,

ENGAGEMENT, AND BEHAVIOR FOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS:

AN INVESTIGATION OF EFFECTIVE INSTRUCTIONAL STRATEGIES

A Dissertation

Presented to

The Faculty of the Education Department

Carson-Newman University

In Partial Fulfillment

Of the

Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Education

By

Rachel V. Brouillette

June 2020

Copyright © 2020 Rachel Victoria Brouillette

All Rights Reserved.

iii

Dissertation Approval

Student Name: Rachel Victoria Brouillette

Dissertation Title:

EVALUATING TIME OF DAY INFLUENCE ON ACHIEVEMENT,

ENGAGEMENT, AND BEHAVIOR FOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS:

AN INVESTIGATION OF EFFECTIVE INSTRUCTIONAL STRATEGIES

This dissertation has been approved and accepted by the faculty of the Education Department,

Carson-Newman University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree, Doctor of

Education.

Dissertation Committee:

Dissertation Chair: Dr. Steve A. Davidson

Methodologist Member: Dr. P. Mark Taylor

Content Member: Dr. Tony L. Dalton

Approved by the Dissertation Committee: June 30, 2020

iv

Abstract

This qualitative research study investigated the phenomenon of differences in student

engagement, achievement, and behavior at different times of the day. The purpose of the study

was to identify best instructional practices used by highly qualified educators that were most

effective in promoting positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior between

morning and afternoon classes. The participants were three ninth grade, college-preparatory

level teachers who taught either Biology 1, Algebra 1, or English 1. The researcher conducted

two lesson observations on the same day for each participant, one morning class and one

afternoon class for the same lesson. After concluding lesson observations, the researcher

interviewed each participant, and then led a focus group with all three participants. The overall

findings of the study suggested there are four ways educators can promote positive student

engagement, achievement, and behavior despite time of the day differences in these areas.

Educators should incorporate close monitoring and frequent circulation throughout their lessons,

give clear and concise directions for each lesson activity, implement multiple formative

assessment strategies with incentives when possible, and adjust lesson pacing between morning

and afternoon to compensate for differences in student attention.

Keywords: chronotype, instructional strategies, increasing academic performance, student

engagement

v

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank my dissertation committee members for helping me through this

long and arduous journey. Dr. Davidson was the perfect dissertation chair for me because he

always calmed my overwhelming anxiety throughout this process with encouragement and made

me feel at ease. I cannot thank him enough for his disposition, encouragement, and assistance

throughout this process. Dr. Taylor was also a huge help in developing the direction of my

research, and helping me through the unchartered waters of qualitative data coding. I would have

never made it to the end without his help. Dr. Dalton was also very encouraging and helpful

throughout this process. He always provided helpful and timely feedback and always offered

encouragement and praise.

I would also like to thank my husband. He has walked through life with me for 13 years

now, and we have grown and changed together so much since we met. He has always been

supportive of any endeavor I take on, and provides support whenever I need him. I would not

have made it through these last three years without his support and encouragement. He is my

rock and the love of my life.

Lastly, I want to thank my parents. From a young age, they have always pushed me to

reach my highest potential. While they may not have initially approved of my decision to

become a teacher, they were always supportive and have seen over the years that it was the best

decision for me because teaching provides more than an income. Teaching gives me fulfillment

and passion, which is something not many people have in their careers. My parents have been

supportive throughout my entire doctoral program, and they have pushed and encouraged me

during the dissertation process. It is because of them that I had the drive and passion for

achieving this doctoral degree.

vi

Table of Contents

Abstract ......................................................................................................................................... iv

Acknowledgments ......................................................................................................................... v

List of Figures ............................................................................................................................... xi

CHAPTER ONE: Introduction ................................................................................................... 1

Statement of the Problem ............................................................................................................ 2

Research Question ...................................................................................................................... 2

Theoretical Foundation ............................................................................................................... 3

Rationale ..................................................................................................................................... 4

Researcher Positionality Statement ............................................................................................ 4

Definitions of Terms ................................................................................................................... 5

Academic achievement. .......................................................................................................... 5

At-risk students ....................................................................................................................... 5

Chronotype .............................................................................................................................. 5

Circadian rhythm .................................................................................................................... 5

College preparatory (CP) ........................................................................................................ 5

Highly qualified teacher status ............................................................................................... 6

High-stakes testing .................................................................................................................. 6

Morningness-eveningness ....................................................................................................... 6

Student engagement ................................................................................................................ 6

vii

Student tracking ...................................................................................................................... 6

Synchrony effect ..................................................................................................................... 6

TN Ready ................................................................................................................................ 7

Organization of the Study ........................................................................................................... 7

Summary ..................................................................................................................................... 8

CHAPTER TWO: Review of the Literature .............................................................................. 9

Biological Clocks ...................................................................................................................... 10

Chronotype and Morningness-Eveningness ............................................................................. 13

Chronotype and Age ................................................................................................................. 16

Chronotype and Gender ............................................................................................................ 18

Chronotype and Student Performance ...................................................................................... 19

Memory and Time of Day ........................................................................................................ 22

Student Motivation and Engagement ........................................................................................ 26

Classroom Activity and Student Behavior ................................................................................ 29

ADHD ................................................................................................................................... 30

ODD ...................................................................................................................................... 31

ASD ...................................................................................................................................... 32

Medications ........................................................................................................................... 33

Summary ................................................................................................................................... 35

viii

CHAPTER THREE: Methodology ........................................................................................... 37

Research Question .................................................................................................................... 37

Population and Sample ............................................................................................................. 37

Description of Instruments ........................................................................................................ 38

Research Procedures and Time Period of Study ....................................................................... 40

Data Analysis Procedures ......................................................................................................... 40

Trustworthiness ..................................................................................................................... 41

Limitations, Delimitations, and Assumptions........................................................................... 42

Limitations.. .......................................................................................................................... 42

Delimitations ......................................................................................................................... 42

Assumptions .......................................................................................................................... 42

CHAPTER FOUR: Presentation of the Findings .................................................................... 44

Research Methodology Applied to Data Analysis .................................................................... 45

Descriptive Characteristics of Participants ............................................................................... 45

Classroom Observations, Interviews, and Focus Group Data Presentation ............................. 46

Study Findings .......................................................................................................................... 53

Academic feedback ............................................................................................................... 53

Student accountability ........................................................................................................... 54

Classroom Management ....................................................................................................... 56

Use of multiple instructional strategies ................................................................................ 57

ix

Short lesson segments ........................................................................................................... 58

Clear and concise directions ................................................................................................. 58

Lesson pacing ....................................................................................................................... 59

Student attention ................................................................................................................... 60

Student behavior ................................................................................................................... 61

Increase in achievement ........................................................................................................ 62

Incentives or rewards ............................................................................................................ 64

Student participation ............................................................................................................. 65

Trustworthiness ......................................................................................................................... 66

Summary ................................................................................................................................... 67

CHAPTER FIVE: Conclusions, Implications, and Recommendations ................................. 68

Statement of Problem ................................................................................................................ 68

Theoretical Approach ............................................................................................................... 69

Conclusions and Summary of Findings .................................................................................... 71

Frequent circulation during lessons ...................................................................................... 71

Chunking lesson into segments with clear, concise directions ............................................. 72

Adjust lesson pacing ............................................................................................................. 73

Use of multiple formative assessment strategies .................................................................. 74

Implications .............................................................................................................................. 75

Limitations ................................................................................................................................ 76

x

Delimitations ............................................................................................................................. 76

Recommendations for Further Research ................................................................................... 77

Summary ................................................................................................................................... 78

References .................................................................................................................................... 80

Appendix A: ................................................................................................................................. 88

Appendix B: ................................................................................................................................. 91

Appendix C: ................................................................................................................................. 94

xi

List of Figures

Figure 4.1 Coding of Raw Data from Classroom Observations, Interviews, and

Focus Group……………………………………………………………………..48

1

CHAPTER ONE: Introduction

Biological processes that cycle in 24-hour periods are daily rhythms. A circadian rhythm

is a daily rhythm influenced by internal and environmental cues. According to Refinetti (2012),

“Many behavioral processes of individual organisms exhibit daily and/or circadian rhythmicity,

including locomotor activity, feeding, excretion, sensory processing, and learning capability”

(para. 2). Researchers James Horne and Olov Ostberg were among the first to examine the

potential differences among human circadian rhythms. They developed the Morningness-

Eveningness Questionnaire to help determine an individual’s peak performance time of day.

Ultimately, they determined that individuals are either “Morning types,” “Evening types,” or

“Afternoon types” (Horne & Ostberg, 1977).

According to Besoluk (2011), “Morningness–eveningness preference is not a fixed

feature but can change during the span of an individual’s life. Evening types are more prevalent

in adolescents and young adults, whereas morning types are more common in children and older

adults” (p. 248). Furthermore, Besoluk (2011) claimed, “Preference for morning or evening

activities is an individual difference in circadian rhythms with potential applications in everyday

life such as optimizing work schedules, sports performance, and academic achievement” (p.

248). Due to the shift in circadian preference (chronotype) to eveningness during adolescence,

students have shown negative consequences because of living in a society that forces them to

perform early in the morning. According to Diaz-Morales and Escribano (2013):

Due to early school starting times, evening-type adolescents experience a greater

misalignment between biological and social rhythms: they sleep less on school days, their

quality of life related to health is worse, they report more school-related problems, and

they achieve lower grades. (p.1232)

2

This study investigated which instructional strategies were most effective in promoting

student engagement and performance to overcome the misalignment of evening-type adolescents

with the traditional early start times for high school.

Statement of the Problem

Existing research on the topic of how time of day affects performance focused on

whether a difference existed in human performance at different times of day (Carrier & Monk,

2000; Fabbri, Natale, & Adan, 2008; Onder, Horzum, & Besoluk, 2012; Preckel et al., 2013;

Randler, 2011; Randler, Bechtold, & Vogel, 2016; Randler, Rahafer, Arbabi, and Bretschneider,

2014; Roenneberg et al., 2007; Vollmer, Potsch, and Randler, 2013; Zavada, Gordijn, Beersma,

Daen, & Roenneberg, 2005). The research established differences in preferred times of the day

and performance (Gelbmann et al., 2012; Biss and Hasher, 2012; Roenneberg et al., 2007; Kim,

Dueker, Hasher, & Goldstein, 2002; Vinne et al., 2015); therefore, it was essential to focus the

current research on how to improve student performance for those enrolled in a class not during

their preferred time of the day. This study focused on identifying best practice instructional

strategies of high school teachers for promoting positive student engagement, achievement, and

behavior to promote equitable performance from all students regardless of time of the day they

were in class.

Research Question

What instructional strategies are most effective for high school teachers in promoting

positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning and afternoon

classes?

3

Theoretical Foundation

The theoretical foundation for this study was constructivism. According to Olusegun

(2015), constructivism suggested that people construct their knowledge and understanding

through experiences and reflecting on those experiences. To accomplish this, people should ask

questions, explore, and assess what they already know. In a classroom setting, constructivism

encourages students to use active learning techniques such as experiments and real-world

problem solving. The basic characteristics of constructivist learning environments included the

sharing of knowledge between students and teachers, shared authority between students and

teachers, heterogeneous small learning groups in the classroom, and the teacher acting as a

facilitator or guide for learning. Some of the goals associated with constructivist learning

environments included students determining how they will learn, student-centered learning,

classroom collaboration, and student reflection on their learning (Olusegun, 2015).

Constructivist theory was best suited for this investigation because it promoted the active

involvement of students in their learning, which promoted higher engagement and overall

academic performance (Saeed & Zyngier, 2012).

In addition to constructivism, best practice strategies to promote student engagement and

motivation were a foundation for this study. The research problem concerned how students

exhibit different levels of engagement, achievement, and behavior at different times of the day.

Identifying best practice strategies for promoting engagement and achievement guided the

researcher in the formation of teacher interview questions and focus group discussion questions.

The researcher needed to familiarize herself with research regarding the most current best

practice strategies for promoting student motivation and engagement in today’s generation of

students. This information was crucial for the foundation of the study, development of the

4

interviews and focus group questions, and provided the researcher with necessary background

knowledge while coding the data. In the constructivist model, students were active participants

in their learning while the teacher facilitated and prompted, mediated, and coached students as

they developed and assessed their understanding of the material.

Rationale

Identifying best instructional practices from high school teachers for promoting student

engagement, achievement, and behavior has many benefits for all high school teachers and

students, especially any practice that might work best for college preparatory (CP) level students

enrolled in classes later in the day. It is often challenging to keep students engaged and on task

at the end of the day due to fatigue and restlessness. Identifying effective practices to overcome

differences in student performance at different times of the day could have a significant impact

on improving teaching practice and student achievement.

Researcher Positionality Statement

The researcher taught at the high school used in the study and has worked in the school

for eight years. The researcher had a good relationship with the majority of teachers in the

building and taught a high-stakes tested subject area. The researcher had anecdotal evidence and

state testing data to support the hypothesis that students typically have higher achievement in

class and on state standardized assessments when enrolled in a course earlier in the day

compared with the last block of the day. There were several biases the researcher had regarding

this study. One bias was that at-risk students were less motivated and engaged than students not

identified as at-risk, and about one-third of students enrolled in CP level classes at the school

used in the study belonged to the at-risk population. The researcher controlled this bias by not

focusing on individual students, but on how teachers accommodate for differences in

5

engagement, achievement, and behavior in their classes overall between morning and afternoon

classes with CP level students. The researcher observed teacher lessons but did not know any

information about the students enrolled in the observed classes.

Definitions of Terms

Academic achievement. “Academic achievement is almost entirely measured with

grades (by course or assignment) and GPA” (York, Gibson, & Rankin, 2015, p.7).

At-risk students. In general, at-risk students were defined as students that were poorly

equipped to meet academic standards. Students often fell into several categories when labeled

at-risk. They may have been low-income, academically underprepared, lacking technology

skills, raised by single parents, poor health, having limited access to technology, and immigrant

status (Bulger & Watson, 2006). These risk factors made students at-risk to not graduate on

time.

Chronotype. Human preferences in the timing of sleep and wakefulness (Roenneberg,

Wirz-Justice, & Merrow, 2003).

Circadian rhythm. Often referred to as the "body clock," the circadian rhythm is the 24-

hour cycle that tells our bodies when to sleep, rise, and eat—regulating many physiological

processes (“Circadian Rhythm,” n.d.).

College preparatory (CP). The school and district used in this study identified courses

as college preparatory to promote college readiness for all students. Students enrolled in CP

courses represented the general population of students, including special education students

(those with 504 plans, IEPs, or identified as gifted).

6

Highly qualified teacher status. To be considered highly qualified, teachers must have

had a bachelor’s degree, full state certification or licensure, and prove they knew each subject

they teach (U.S. Department of Education, 2005).

High-stakes testing. “High-stakes tests are a part of policy design that links the score on

one set of standardized tests to grade promotion, high school graduation and, in some cases,

teacher and principal salaries and tenure decisions” (Au, 2007, p. 258). Stakes on testing were

considered high when the results of standardized testing were used in the ranking and

categorization of schools, teachers, and students, and those results were reported to the public

(McNeil, 2000).

Morningness-eveningness. Chronotypes have been assessed mainly by questionnaires

designed to associate individuals to tendencies coined ‘morningness’ or ‘eveningness’ (Horne

and Östberg, 1976).

Student engagement. Student engagement typically referred to either students’

psychological investment or willingness to invest time in educational behaviors, or a more

general reference to student involvement in educational activities (York, Gibson, & Rankin,

2015).

Student tracking. An instructional practice that involved placing students into different

classes based on their achievement skill levels, readiness, or abilities. The primary reason for

this practice was to create a more homogeneous learning environment so teachers can provide

instruction better matched to students’ needs, and students could benefit from interactions with

their comparable academic peers (Steenbergen-Hu, Makel, & Olszewski-Kubilius, 2016).

Synchrony effect. The idea that individuals perform best when working during their

preferred chronotype (Goldstein, Hahn, Hasher, Wiprzycka, Zelazo, 2007).

7

TN Ready. TNReady was a part of the Tennessee Comprehensive Assessment Program

(TCAP) and was designed to assess true student understanding, not just basic memorization and

test-taking skills (Tennessee Department of Education, n.d.).

Organization of the Study

Chapter 1 of the study introduced how time of the day can influence human performance.

The researcher discussed concepts of chronotype and synchrony effect along with supporting

research regarding the change in chronotype from childhood to adolescence, which indicated

adolescents exhibit higher performance in the afternoon compared to the morning. Chapter 1

also discussed the theoretical framework, key terms, purpose and significance of the study, and

the limitations, delimitations, and assumptions of the study.

Chapter 2 contained the review of literature relevant to the topic of study. The topics

discussed in the literature review were biological clocks, chronotype, morningness-eveningness,

chronotype and age/gender, chronotype and student performance, memory and time of day,

classroom activity and behavior, and effective instructional strategies to promote student

motivation and engagement. Chapter 3 was the methodology of the study. Chapter 3 included

the research design, data collection, coding process, data analysis, and a description of the study

participants and research setting.

In Chapter 4, the researcher presented the research findings, including a visual

representation of the coding process. In Chapter 5, the researcher provided an interpretation of

data presented in Chapter 4 to determine conclusions. Chapter 5 also included how the findings

related to broader theoretical issues, analyzed the research process, described implications for

future studies, and made recommendations for future related studies.

8

Summary

Identifying best practice strategies to promote positive engagement, achievement, and

behavior from high school students in classes despite time of the day can have a positive impact

on students, teachers, and the school. This study aimed to identify these best instructional

practices by selecting three highly qualified, practicing high school educators in high-stakes

tested subject areas of CP level Biology I, Algebra 1, and English 1 as participants. Participants

initially completed two classroom observations, followed by individual interviews. The

researcher observed each participant once in a morning class and again for their afternoon class

to observe any differences in student engagement or behavior. The researcher also observed any

differences in teacher instruction between morning and afternoon classes. The final step in data

collection was a focus group discussion after completion of all classroom observations. The goal

of this study was to identify effective instructional strategies to overcome the discrepancy in

student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning and afternoon classes.

9

CHAPTER TWO: Review of the Literature

There were several areas of previous research to investigate the phenomenon of

differences in student engagement, academic achievement, and behavior among high school

students on a block schedule at different times of the day. Reviewing current research provided

the researcher with the necessary background knowledge to conduct an informed, non-biased

study. The research question focused on identifying best instructional practices by high school

teachers to promote positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning

and afternoon classes. There were several areas of research related to the current study. First, it

was necessary to describe how biological clocks and circadian rhythm worked in animals.

Biological clocks were found to be subject to individual variation. People typically felt most

alert, energetic, and capable at a particular time of the day, which often varied from one

individual to another (Onder, Horzum, & Besoluk, 2012). This preference for a particular time

of the day is an individual’s chronotype.

Next, the researcher reviewed how to identify an individual’s chronotype and the

morningness-eveningness scale of chronotypes. A questionnaire such as the Morninness-

Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) determined an individual’s chronotype. Developed by James

Horne and Olov Ostberg in 1976, the MEQ was a 19-question survey aimed at determining an

individual’s optimal time of day to be active mentally and physically (Zavada, Gordijn, Beersma,

Daen, & Roenneberg, 2005). Concerning chronotype, there were factors that may have

influenced an individual’s chronotype, such as age and gender. In a study conducted by

Gelbmann et al. (2012), it was determined that circadian preference had both genetic and

environmental influences. Gelbmann et al. (2012) also described how chronotype changed with

age. In addition, some studies indicated a slight difference in chronotype between males and

10

females. These studies deduced that gender chronotype differences were most likely due to the

difference in hormone levels between males and females (Duffy et al., 2010; Roenneberg et al.,

2007).

The researcher also reviewed studies related to how chronotype influenced academic

performance in children. According to Onder, Besoluk, Iskender, Masal, and Demirhan (2014),

morning-type students naturally woke up earlier and therefore reached maximum productivity

earlier in the day compared with evening-type students. In their study, morning-type students

performed better on school exams, had a higher grade point average (GPA), and fewer school-

related problems compared with evening-type students. After a thorough review of research

regarding chronotypes, the researcher reviewed research concerning memory and time of day

because memory played a major role in student learning.

Finally, the researcher reviewed research on student motivation and engagement. Many

factors influenced student motivation and engagement other than chronotype. The researcher

needed to be knowledgeable concerning all factors that influence student engagement,

motivation, and behavior to maintain a non-biased study. Saeed and Zyngier (2012) claimed that

student engagement referred to the students’ willingness, need, desire, and compulsion to

participate and be successful in the learning process. Student motivation referred to the amount

of effort given by the student and their focus on learning to achieve academic success. There

were two primary types of motivation—intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. The researcher

included an investigation of all these topics in the following review of the literature.

Biological Clocks

A circadian rhythm is an internally driven phenomenon that occurs in an approximately

24-hour cycle. Scientists have observed circadian rhythms in almost all living organisms, from

11

bacteria to humans. Circadian rhythms control many biological and physiological processes

such as sleep-wake patterns, feeding habits, daily performance patterns, and body temperature in

animals. In plants, circadian rhythms influence flowering, position relative to the sun, and

opening and closing of stomata to perform photosynthesis. In humans and other animals, the

cycles of sleep and wakefulness rely on a biological clock, a mechanism that controls gene

expression and cellular activities. Synchronization of the human biological clock aligns with

cycles of light and darkness. Within the human brain, the hypothalamus contains clusters of

neurons that form a structure known as the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN). The SCN receives

light sensory information from the eyes and controls the release of melatonin, the sleep-inducing

hormone, from the pineal gland within the brain. As the amount of light entering the eyes

decreases, the SCN triggers an increased release of melatonin. As the amount of light increases,

melatonin production decreases. (Urry, Cain, Wasserman, Minorsky, & Reece, 2018, pp.1016,

1092-93). According to Onder, Horzum, and Besoluk (2012), “While melatonin secretion is

high during night, cortisol secretion increases with light. Melatonin and cortisol are important

components of bodies’ time keeping system. Awakening response is associated with cortisol

whereas sleep is associated with melatonin” (pp. 162-163).

Many psychological and physiological processes within the human body vary throughout

the day, typically with peaks and troughs roughly coinciding with light and darkness,

respectively. Biological clocks were subject to individual variation. People felt most alert,

energetic, and capable at a particular time of day, which often varied from one individual to

another (Onder, Horzum, & Besoluk, 2012). This preferred time of day was an individual’s

chronotype. There were two most commonly identified chronotypes, morning-type and evening-

type. Morning-type individuals were larks, while evening-type individuals were owls. Many

12

studies suggested that chronotype varied with age and gender. The ability of an individual to

anticipate peaks in their physical and mental capacities had an important influence on scheduling

and accomplishment of everyday activities such as learning and coursework for students (Onder,

Horzum, & Besoluk, 2012). An individual’s preference for morning or evening activities

appeared to be a non-traditional but promising indicator of academic success for students.

According to Preckel et al. (2013), “Recent research has documented statistically meaningful

relationships between chronotype and academic performance and demonstrated that eveningness

and academic performance are negatively related, whereas morningness and academic

performance are positively related” (p. 115).

In a study performed by Carrier and Monk (2000), they investigated the relationship

between circadian rhythm and human performance. They claimed that maximum performance

should be midday with minimal performance early in the morning and late at night. Carrier and

Monk expanded on this by explaining that working memory tasks showed a maximum around

midday, while the peak for immediate (learning) memory peaked in morning hours. Carrier and

Monk discussed how alertness and performance efficiency were determined by the number of

hours an individual is awake and input from the body’s circadian timing system (CTS). They

claimed performance efficiency for a specific task decreased throughout the day. The

mechanism that controlled internal circadian performance rhythm was the same mechanism that

drove the circadian rhythm of body temperature. This mechanism also drove the levels of

cortisol and melatonin circulating in the blood (Carrier & Monk, 2000). Another important

aspect of Carrier and Monk’s study was the idea of the “post-lunch dip.” They suggested there

was a general increase in the natural tendency or desire to sleep during mid-afternoon hours,

which could aid in the explanation of decreased student performance in classes after lunch.

13

Carrier and Monk reported a short-lived decrease in performance during the mid-afternoon

hours, and a high-carbohydrate lunch intensified this effect. However, they said the effect still

occurred even with no lunch (Carrier & Monk, 2000).

For significance to this study, the researcher needed to understand how physiological

processes such as the biological clock influenced behaviors and performance patterns of

individuals. Increased knowledge concerning the optimal time of day for student performance

and alertness could aid school leaders and educators in course scheduling and improvement of

instruction to meet student needs.

Chronotype and Morningness-Eveningness

Daily rhythms such as temperature and sleep-wake cycles influenced many aspects of

human lives. The changes in amounts of melatonin and cortisol released into the bloodstream in

response to amount of light detection by the SCN within the hypothalamus influenced body

functioning. Most people felt most alert, energetic, and productive at a specific time of the day

(Onder, Hohrzum, Besoluk, 2012). The term chronotype referred to the preferred time of day

variation from one individual to another. According to Oginska and Oginska-Bruchal (2014),

“The term ‘chronotype’ is used to describe relatively stable traits of the subjective diurnal

rhythm of activity. . . . Traditionally, it refers to the subjective morning-evening preference” (p.

2). A questionnaire, such as the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ), determined an

individual’s chronotype. Developed by James Horne and Olov Ostberg in 1976, the MEQ was a

19-question survey aimed at determining an individual’s optimal time of day to be active

mentally and physically. The majority of questions were preferential and multiple-choice based

with a point value associated with each answer. The valued sum of each answer choice ranged

from 16-86, with lower scores corresponding to evening-type individuals (Zavada, Gordijn,

14

Beersma, Daen, & Roenneberg, 2005). The Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) was a

more recent chronotype indicator developed by Till Roenneberg in 2003. The MCTQ asked

individuals to rate themselves as one of seven chronotypes: Extreme Early, Moderate Early,

Slightly Early, Normal, Slightly Late, Moderate Late, or Extreme Late. Subjects also judged

their chronotype at different life stages to determine how their chronotype changed with age

(Zavada et al., 2005).

A study conducted by Randler, Rahafer, Arbabi, and Bretschneider (2014) investigated

the effect of chronotype on students during their first lesson of the school day. They claimed

with supporting research that children were usually morning-oriented while adolescents around

the age of 12-14 years underwent a dramatic shift toward eveningness. At the end of

adolescence, there was a shift back toward morningness. Randler et al. (2014) claimed that

many studies reported a worse school performance in evening-oriented adolescents or evening

types. This relationship was evident in multiple countries, different school types, and among

university-level students. Evening-type adolescents appeared unable to perform at their best

performance during school lessons. A study conducted by Vollmer, Potsch, and Randler (2013)

found that because adolescents had a shift in chronotype toward eveningness, but the school day

started early in the morning, adolescents’ academic performance suffered due to sleep

deprivation and daytime sleepiness. Vollmer et al. (2013) also indicated a relationship between

chronotype and attention peaks at different times of the day. Adolescents’ performance may

have increased during the morning due to societal pressures and the finding that subjective

alertness increased in the morning (Fabbri, Natale, & Adan, 2008). After reaching their

individual peak, performance likely deteriorated, and errors increased at the end of the school

day (Vollmer et al., 2013).

15

In adolescents, the significant change in morningness-eveningness preference was

associated with different aspects of their lives, such as excessive daytime sleepiness, poor

academic performance, and behavioral and emotional problems (Randler, 2011). Randler (2011)

said morning-type individuals fit better into society because school and work schedules oriented

around a morning schedule. Evening-type adolescents were at a higher risk for lower school

performance and academic achievement. Onder, Horzum, and Besoluk (2012) described

evening-type individuals as creative, emotionally unstable, and had difficult social and familial

relations. Morning-type individuals had decreased mood and energy throughout the day, while

evening-type personalities showed the opposite with increased mood and energy throughout the

day. Evening-type individuals showed greater behavioral problems, lower academic

performance, and higher stress rates in family and social lives. Due to sleep deprivation and

their forced shift to perform at their non-preferred chronotype, it was difficult for late chronotype

children to adapt socially and academically. Onder, Horzum, & Besoluk (2012) claimed that one

of the probable reasons for the negative effects of the typical school schedule on evening-type

adolescents was an insufficient amount of sleep. Fixed school start times were typically much

earlier than peak performance time for adolescent evening-types. Early start times benefitted

morning-type individuals but placed evening-type individuals at a significant disadvantage.

School leaders and educators should be aware of the misalignment between student peak

performance and the timing of the school day. Rather than focusing on persuading policymakers

to make the unlikely change of adjusting school start times, it would be more effective for

educators and school leaders to focus efforts on identifying effective instructional strategies to

motivate and engage students despite time of the day to eliminate the concern of chronotype

misalignment for adolescent students.

16

Chronotype and Age

For significance to this study, it was essential to discuss how chronotype varied with age.

Several studies indicated that chronotype changed throughout an individual’s lifetime. In a study

conducted by Gelbmann et al. (2012), it was determined circadian preference had both genetic

and environmental influences. Gelbmann et al. (2012) also described how chronotype changed

with age. Young children showed strong tendencies for morningness, with estimates close to

90% of children age five or younger. This morningness tendency decreased to 46% in four- to

11-year-olds. During adolescence, a significant shift toward eveningness occurred. Gelbmann et

al. (2012) claimed this significant shift in chronotype during puberty was associated with

physical changes, the increase in academic and social demands, increased independence, more

relaxed parental restrictions, and greater involvement in late-night activities. The results of their

study supported pronounced morningness in young children, but they could not confirm the

previously reported significant drop in morningness preference during puberty. Rather, they

found morningness steadily declined as children matured.

Some studies found that the shift to eveningness chronotype in adolescence was due to

hormonal changes that occurred during puberty. According to Biss and Hasher (2012), “These

chronotype differences are thought to be linked to age-dependent changes in the concentration

and timing of certain hormones, including cortisol and growth hormone, which influence the

timing and quality of sleep” (p. 437). A study conducted by Roenneberg et al. (2007) further

supported this claim. Roenneberg et al. proposed that the changes in chronotype with age along

with significant differences observed in chronotypes between males and females between

puberty and menopause indicated that hormones were most likely involved in the age-dependent

changes of chronotype. In adolescents aged 16-25, the secretion of growth hormone reached its

17

maximum and cortisol (the waking hormone) reached its minimum around 1:00 am, which was

about one hour later than in elderly people age 70 or older (Roenneberg et al., 2007).

Furthermore, it was found that the shift toward evening preference appeared to occur at

approximately age 13 (Kim, Dueker, Hasher, & Goldstein, 2002), which was the typical age of

puberty onset and significant hormonal changes.

Most adolescents experienced a shift to eveningness; therefore, they tended to stay up

later in the evenings. However, school start times tended to be earlier for middle and high school

students compared with elementary start times. Adolescents were typically not receiving an

adequate amount of sleep. According to Carskadon (2011), “The reported number of hours slept

on school nights declined from 8.4 hours in the 6th-grade students to 6.9 hours in the 12th

graders” (p. 638). Carskadon proposed that in addition to changes in the circadian rhythms and

chronotype of adolescents, screen time, technology use, and social engagements in the evening

became more available during adolescence. A relationship existed between the use of electronic

devices such as computers, TV, and smartphones in the evening before bedtime, and shorter,

later, and more disturbed sleep. Most electronic devices emit blue-spectrum light to which the

circadian clock may have higher sensitivity and caused individuals to stay awake later than their

natural circadian rhythm (Carskadon, 2011).

The chronotype shift to eveningness in adolescents had important significance for

education. According to Kim et al. (2002), there were two primary ways eveningness of

adolescents affected their school performance. The chronotype shift to eveningness meant

adolescents were more likely to go to bed later but still had to wake up early for school, which

led to sleep deprivation. The result of this sleep deprivation was sleepiness throughout the

school day, which influenced their alertness and engagement. Also, Kim et al. (2002) found that

18

both younger and older adults performed best during their preferred time of day. Younger and

older adults reported better performance of cognitive tasks, less distractibility, and recognition of

newly learned information at their preferred optimal time of the day. The shift in children’s time

of the day preference as they aged indicated their cognitive functioning was likely at its peak in

the afternoon when school was over or almost over. If adolescent students took tested subjects

such as math and English in the morning, they were most likely learning material during their

less-preferred time of day, resulting in decreased academic performance (Kim et al., 2002).

Not all research agreed that there is a definite shift to eveningness among adolescents. In

1999, Callan claimed that about 33% of high school students had no time of day preference, with

about 20% favoring mornings and 30% favoring afternoons or evenings. This indicated that over

half of the adolescents studied had either no preference, or preferred mornings. Another study

by Intons-Peterson, Rocchi, West, McLellan, & Hackney (1998) found that 57% of young adults

demonstrated no preference on the MEQ. In their study, only 6% tested as definitely evening-

types.

Chronotype and Gender

There was a more pronounced relationship between age differences in chronotype than

gender differences in chronotype among existing research. However, some research suggested a

slight difference between male and female chronotype. While both sexes had a shift toward

eveningness beginning around age 13, Roenneberg et al. (2007) claimed women reached their

maximum in lateness around 19.5 years old while males reached their maximum lateness around

age 21. Roenneberg et al. (2007) also said males, on average, maintained a later chronotype

throughout adulthood compared with females. The gender difference in chronotype disappeared

around age 50, which coincided with menopause in females. Roenneberg et al. (2007) used this

19

evidence to support the idea that gender differences in chronotype were primarily due to the

hormone differences between males and females. Duffy et al. (2010) reported,

The shorter intrinsic circadian period we observed in women may be related to their

higher estrogen levels, because it has been shown that continuous administration of

estradiol benzoate results in a significant shortening of [circadian] period in blind,

ovariectomized female hamsters. Our finding of a shorter intrinsic circadian period in

women may therefore be attributable, in part, to the higher circulating levels of estrogen

in women. (p. 4)

For this study, it was essential to be aware of any gender differences in chronotype while

observing student attention and engagement during classroom observations for data collection

purposes. If females appeared to be more attentive than males at different times of day, it could

be due to the slight difference in their preferred time of the day for peak performance.

Chronotype and Student Performance

Various researchers established that adolescents shift their chronotype from morningness

to eveningness around the age of 13; however, school start times become earlier as children enter

middle school and high school. If adolescents developed a chronotype to favor eveningness,

they went to bed later but still woke up early for school. This led to sleep deprivation, which is

not conducive to learning and optimal student performance throughout the school day. In 2015,

Vinne et al. explained that the chronotype of adolescents was later than in all other age groups,

resulting in overall later sleeping times. Students still woke up early for school leading to

chronic sleep deficiency, which was associated with lower school performance. Vinne et al.

(2015) explained, “the condition of chronic sleep deficiency associated with early work or school

hours and late sleep onset has been called social jetlag” (p. 54). Increased social jetlag has been

20

associated with lower academic achievement, and late chronotypes achieved overall lower grades

than early chronotypes (Vinne et al., 2015).

According to Onder, Besoluk, Iskender, Masal, and Demirhan (2014), morning-type

students naturally woke up earlier and reached maximum productivity earlier in the day

compared with evening type students. Their study found morning-type students displayed higher

performance on school exams, had a higher grade point average (GPA), and fewer school-related

problems compared with evening-type students. Onder et al. (2014) hypothesized because

evening-type individuals were less alert early in the day, they may have had difficulty meeting

expectations of teachers and had lower motivation toward learning and achievement. Evening-

type individuals also typically had more daytime sleepiness, which led to decreased alertness and

motivation. Preckel et al. (2013) sought out to investigate why morning-type individuals had

higher performance compared with evening types. They claimed evening-type students went to

bed later than morning-type students did, but they were required to wake at the same time due to

school schedules. This resulted in evening-type students reporting higher rates of daytime

sleepiness, which was associated with lower school achievement. The results of the Preckel et

al. (2013) study indicated that morningness showed a significant positive correlation with

conscientiousness, cognition, and goal mastery. Eveningness showed a significant negative

correlation with conscientiousness, performance goal orientation, and a significant positive

correlation with work avoidance in school. Furthermore, Preckel et al. (2013) found that

students with a tendency for eveningness received significantly lower marks regarding overall

GPA, math-science only GPA, and language GPA. Preckel et al. (2013) determined eveningness

to be a significant negative predictor of overall GPA, math-science GPA, and language GPA

21

even after controlling for variables such as gender, cognitive ability, and achievement

motivation.

Preckel et al. (2013) proposed that factors such as sleep deprivation, and behavioral

problems and work ethic among students with eveningness contributed to their lower

achievement. The researchers claimed that individuals with an eveningness tendency were more

likely to exhibit characteristics negatively related to academic achievement. These

characteristics included a negative attitude towards school, anxiety disorders, lower levels of

conscientiousness, and higher drug consumption. In 2016, Randler, Bechtold, & Vogel found

that morning-type individuals had greater attention during class and were slower and more

considerate when completing exams while evening types were faster and displayed strategies that

were more impulsive during exams. Another study conducted by Hines (2004) established that

performance efficiency steadily declined throughout the day, while speed increased. The

declines in efficiency were more apparent in the afternoon, even though the ability to perceive

stimuli was lower in the morning. Randler, Bechtold, & Vogel (2016) also investigated how

mood and attitude associated with chronotype may influence achievement. They found that

negative mood was higher in evening types, and positive mood was higher in morning types. In

addition, morning types had a higher positive mood during the entire school day compared to

evening types. The tendency of evening types to have a more negative attitude toward school

may have contributed to their lower academic achievement.

In another investigation concerning how time of day impacts classroom achievement,

Wile and Shouppe (2011) described how morning learning was associated with superior

immediate recall (short-term memory) while material learned in the afternoon was more

beneficial to long-term memory recall. In elementary school, teaching core subjects during

22

morning hours before lunch was more conducive to a younger child’s chronotype because young

children typically had a higher tendency for morningness. Random assignment of class

schedules in high school means they may be learning core subjects such as math, science, and

English at times of the day misaligned with their preferred time of day. Vinne et al. (2015)

found that high school students with an early chronotype earned significantly higher grades

during the early and late morning compared with late chronotypes, but this difference

disappeared in the early afternoon, indicating that early and late chronotypes obtained similar

grades in the early afternoon.

Wile and Shouppe (2011) also discussed the commonality observed and heard in teacher

workroom conversations concerning how to motivate students in afternoon classes and the

consistent reports that afternoon classes were harder to teach. Klein (2001) found that levels of

attention for tenth-grade students were lowest in the afternoon and highest in the morning.

Holloway (1999) reported that students scored better during their teacher’s optimal time of day.

This suggested the teacher’s chronotype influenced student learning as much as the student’s

chronotype. As adults, most teachers may shift back towards an early chronotype; therefore,

they may exhibit higher effectiveness and attention to detail during the morning compared with

the afternoon, which could be a contributing factor to the common assumption that students

perform better in the morning.

Memory and Time of Day

The previous sections established that chronotype tended to change in adolescents toward

eveningness rather than morningness. This shift in chronotype resulted in students staying up

later in the evening while still waking early in the morning due to early school start times. This

ultimately led to chronic sleep deficiency among evening-type adolescents, which had a negative

23

impact on their academic achievement. Research concerning how time of day impacts short-

term and long-term memory explained how the brain processes memory and learning at different

times of the day. The research also showed how the shift to eveningness in adolescents followed

by the development of sleep deficiency affected memory and learning. In 1999, Cynthia May

investigated the effects of circadian rhythms on cognitive abilities, specifically, memory,

attention, and decision-making. May claimed that superior cognitive functioning occurred when

testing times synchronized with an individuals’ peak arousal period or preferred time of day

(chronotype). This phenomenon was the “synchrony effect.” In particular, it appeared that

inhibition of distractions or off-task information was most susceptible to synchrony effects. May

(1999) reported that inhibition served three functions related to processing information. First,

inhibition limited access to working memory, which maintained focus to only relevant, task-

oriented stimuli. Second, inhibition suppressed information that was once relevant but no longer

necessary. Finally, inhibition restrained impulsive behavior to promote responsible decision-

making. May (1999) found evidence to suggest each of the inhibitory functions may be impaired

at off-peak times of the day. May claimed individuals were notably susceptible to distractions

during off-peak times of the day. In her study, May did find stable performance throughout the

day for judgment, vocabulary tests, color naming, and generation of sentences. May also found

that younger adults at their peak time of day (preferred chronotype) were highly effective in

suppressing distractions but their ability to avoid distractions worsened during off-peak times.

May (1999) said, “Because suppression of off-task distractions will be particularly difficult at

off-peak times, complex tasks that require focused attention, retrieval of exact information, or

careful control over responses should be completed at peak hours or in a setting in which

distractions are kept to a minimum” (p. 146). May also found that sometimes individuals

24

benefitted from reduced inhibition. For instance, in activities requiring creativity, diminished

inhibition allowed the individual to consider a greater depth of alternatives, which produced a

better product (May, 1999).

Berger (2000) reported that adolescents had a lack of sleep during the school week due to

their tendency to be evening-types, which made them more susceptible to time of day effects on

memory. Berger’s study investigated types of memory tasks best suited for lower levels of

arousal and which ones required higher levels of arousal. The goal of the study was to provide

results to inform school leaders about scheduling certain classes to allow students to learn at their

peak time of day. Berger (2000) reported that a large number of studies showed performance on

tests involving short-term memory was best when given in the morning compared to the

afternoon in adolescents. Berger suggested that the reason for inferior performance in the

afternoon on short-term memory tasks was due to mental fatigue from the student’s accumulated

sleep deficiency. Furthermore, Berger suggested that while performance on tasks involving

short-term memory declined from morning to afternoon, performance on tasks utilizing long-

term memory was generally best in the afternoon. Adolescents were more prone to an evening

chronotype, and their highest level of arousal was in the afternoon. Berger (2000) determined

that high levels of arousal enhanced long-term memory. For complex learning, the finding was

that the level of performance on tasks increased with increased arousal, but beyond a certain

optimal level of arousal, performance fell (Berger, 2000). This could suggest a reason for the

“post-lunch dip” in performance observed among adolescents (Carrier & Monk, 2000). Berger

(2000) found that among high school students, about 40% were early chronotypes, while the

remaining 60% preferred morning-early afternoon. Of the 60% of students who preferred

25

morning-early afternoon, 13% were late chronotypes who exhibited peak learning time in the

evening.

Berger (2000) found short-term memory to have limited capacity and duration.

Rehearsal and repetition of information allowed retention in short-term memory somewhat

longer. While information was in short term memory, it integrated with related information from

long-term memory, which allowed it to enter long-term memory (Berger, 2000). This is why it

is crucial for educators to relate newly learned information to previous knowledge or real-life

experiences, so students can integrate the new information with existing knowledge for new

information to enter long-term memory storage. Information stored in long-term memory may

remain for minutes, hours, days, or potentially a lifetime. Long-term memory recall should

improve throughout the day as arousal increases, while recall of short-term memory was

optimum in the morning (Berger, 2000).

A study conducted by Carrier and Monk (2000) further confirmed these findings of peak

times for short and long-term memory recall. Carrier and Monk reported that immediate

memory peaked in morning hours, working memory showed a maximum for recall about

midday, and long-term memory recall was best in the afternoon. Carrier and Monk also

supported the claim reported in Berger (2000) that beyond a certain period of arousal,

performance fell. Carrier and Monk (2000) found that working memory (part of short-term

memory) recall depended on the size of the working memory load. For a given individual, their

memory load involved with completing tasks affected the timing of the trend over the day. This

could explain why teachers noticed a drop in student performance in afternoon classes, even

though the majority of high school students had peak arousal in the afternoon (Wile & Shouppe,

26

2011). By afternoon, students have reached their maximum working memory load and

experienced mental fatigue (Berger, 2000).

Student Motivation and Engagement

The previous sections discussed how circadian rhythms and chronotype influenced

student achievement. The majority of the research described above supported the idea that

adolescents tend to shift towards an evening-type chronotype, meaning they reached peak

performance ability at some point in the afternoon. As a result, educators need to consider how

they differentiate instruction throughout the day to maintain student engagement at all times

during the day. However, it was important to discuss the many factors that contribute to student

motivation and engagement. While circadian rhythm tendencies may play some part in student

motivation, engagement, and academic performance, many other factors influenced student

motivation and engagement.

In 2012, Saeed and Zyngier investigated how motivation influenced student engagement.

They reported that motivation was a required and necessary element for student engagement in

learning. Student engagement in learning was essential for students to achieve sound academic

outcomes. Engagement occurred when students were involved personally in their work, showed

perseverance in the face of obstacles and challenges, and found joy in accomplishing their work.

Saeed and Zyngier (2012) also claimed that student engagement refers to a student’s willingness,

need, desire, and compulsion to participate and be successful in the learning process. Student

motivation referred to the amount of effort given by the student and their focus on learning to

achieve academic success. There were two primary types of motivation identified—intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation. According to Toshalis and Nakkula (2012), the use of extrinsic motivators

occurred within a behaviorist framework where external stimuli such as social expectations,

27

reward, praise, punishments, or threats produced successful outcomes. The shift away from

behaviorist theory to constructivist theory in education coincided with the growing belief that the

best motivators were intrinsic to the student. Intrinsic motivators were internal and valued by the

individual, whether expressed to others or not. Student motivations tended to be stronger, more

resilient, and better sustained when they emerged intrinsically (Toshalis & Nakkula, 2012).

According to Saaed and Zyngier (2012), intrinsically motivated students had higher

achievement levels, lower anxiety, and higher self-perceptions of competence in learning

compared with students who were not intrinsically motivated. Furthermore, teachers frequently

used extrinsic motivators like rewards, praise, free time, food, or punishment to encourage and

stimulate student motivation. The majority of researchers believed motivation is not exclusively

intrinsic or extrinsic. A balanced approach to motivation in the classroom with a combination of

both types produced the highest success (Saeed & Zyngier, 2012). A study conducted in 2010 by

Zepke and Leach outlined ten proposals for educators to increase student engagement in the

classroom. Zepke and Leach (2010) proposed educators should 1) enhance students’ self-belief

by encouraging them to create their own knowledge, 2) enable students to work autonomously,

enjoy relationships with others and feel they are competent to achieve their own objectives, 3)

recognize that teaching and teachers are central to engagement by maintaining a positive,

approachable attitude, being well prepared, and being sensitive to student needs, 4) create

learning that is active, collaborative and fosters learning relationships by allowing for frequent

peer interaction within lessons, 5) create educational experiences for students that are

challenging, enriching, and extend their academic abilities, 6) ensure institutional cultures are

welcoming to students from diverse backgrounds, 7) invest in a variety of support services such

as educational technology, 8) adapt to changing student expectations, 9) enable students to

28

become active citizens, and 10) enable students to develop their social and cultural capital by

creating an atmosphere of acceptance and promoting active and engaging peer relationships

among all students (Zepke & Leach, 2010). Saeed and Zyngier (2012) suggested that good

teacher-student relationships, clear instructions, group work, student choice, planning engaging

and interesting learning activities, and making learning important and valuable to students

contributed to promoting student motivation and engagement in their learning. Saeed and

Zyngier (2012) also said that “extrinsic motivation should only be used in a way that enhances

intrinsic motivation rather than undermining it, as extrinsic rewards have a positive effect in

situations where intrinsic motivation is not high” (p. 262).

Student engagement was also an important factor in understanding dropout rates among

high school students. According to Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong (2008), engagement was

the primary factor for understanding dropout and was necessary to promote school completion.

Appleton et al. (2008) defined school completion as graduation from high school with academic

and social skills necessary for college enrollment or entering the workforce. Data examined by

Appleton et al. (2008) indicated that sufficient engagement with school did not occur for many

students. “Data from 2003 indicated that 3.5 million youth and young adults (16-25 years old)

had not earned a high school diploma and were not currently enrolled in school. Many dropouts,

by ages 16-24 were not employed” (p. 372). Additionally, male dropouts aged 25-34 received an

annual average income of $22,903 in 2002, which was the poverty threshold for a family of five.

The average annual income of employed female dropouts was $17,114, which made them unable

to keep a family of four above the poverty line. Also, students who did not complete high school

had higher incarceration rates and had a long-term dependency on social services (Appleton,

Christenson, & Furlong, 2008).

29

Understanding factors that influenced student motivation and engagement other than

circadian rhythm preference was essential for educators to learn how to develop curriculum

strategies to motivate and engage students despite misalignment of class times with a student’s

chronotype for peak performance in learning. The goal of the current study was to determine the

most effective instructional methods for promoting student engagement to overcome the

obstacles of student fatigue and sleep deficiency caused by misalignment of class times and

student chronotypes.

Classroom Activity and Student Behavior

The shift in chronotype from morningness to eveningness in adolescents led to sleep

deprivation and social jetlag (Vinne et al., 2015). According to Witkowski et al. (2014),

“Considerable research has examined the effects of total sleep deprivation (TSD) on cognitive

processes such as working memory, executive functioning, learning, and selective attention”

(p.1). Witkowski et al. (2014) described a direct relationship of TSD and a decreased ability to

sustain attention and alertness over long periods. The results of the Witkowski et al. (2014)

study determined there was a direct correlation between cognitive ability, attention, and student

activity with chronotype. In addition to sleep deprivation leading to decreased sustained

attention and alertness throughout the school day, several behavioral conditions affected student

activity and behavior in the classroom. The following sections investigated the effects of

commonly diagnosed behavior disorders in children, such as attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). In

addition, the researcher investigated the types of medications prescribed for these disorders and

their effect on student performance throughout the school day.

30

ADHD. According to the Center for Disease Control, ADHD was one of the most

common mental health disorders of childhood. The symptoms often began in childhood and

continued into adulthood. The symptoms associated with ADHD, such as inattention, impulsive

behavior, and hyperactivity, often led to adverse effects in the academic, family, and social

aspects of their lives (Akinbami, Llu, Pastor, and Reuben 2011). Diagnosis for ADHD involved

a medical exam, a checklist for rating ADHD symptoms completed by the parents, teachers, and

sometimes the child. From 2007 to 2009, the average diagnosis of ADHD in children aged 5-17

was 9.0%. The prevalence of ADHD was higher among boys compared with girls (Akinbami et

al. 2011).

A study performed by Loe and Feldman (2007) investigated the academic and

educational outcomes of children with ADHD. The researchers found that children with ADHD

showed significant academic underachievement, poor academic performance, and other

education problems. Children with ADHD scored significantly lower on reading and math

achievement tests, exhibited an increase in repeated grade levels, used more remedial services,

and had higher placement in special education classes. Children with ADHD were also more

often expelled or suspended. Pre-school children with ADHD or symptoms of ADHD were

more likely to be behind in basic academic readiness skills. Initial symptoms of ADHD were

hyperactivity, distractibility, impulsivity, and aggression. These symptoms tended to decrease in

severity over time but remained present and increased in comparison to controls. Those subjects

followed into adolescence failed more grades, achieved lower grades in all subjects on report

cards, had lower class rankings, and performed more poorly on standardized subject exams

compared with control groups (Loe & Feldman, 2007).

31

ODD. According to Drabick and Gadow (2012), characterization of oppositional defiant

disorder (ODD) included a “pattern of negativistic, hostile, and defiant behavior toward adults

and co-occurs with numerous psychiatric disorders” (para. 1). The disorders that most

commonly co-occur with ODD included ADHD, conduct disorder (CD), and anxiety or mood

disorders. A study conducted by Serra-Pinheiro, Mattos, Regalla, Souza, & Paixão (2008)

investigated the effects of three main behavioral disorder symptoms on academic achievement.

This study examined the effects of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and oppositional-

defiant symptoms on academic achievement among sixth-grade students. Serra-Pinheiro et al.

(2008) found that out of the three symptoms, only inattention was the main predictor for school

difficulties. In their study, oppositional-defiant disorder symptoms (when not co-occurring with

ADHD symptoms) did not correlate with academic difficulties. However, when looking at

children with co-occurring ODD with ADHD, their risk of academic failure heightened

significantly.

In their research regarding strategies for helping children with oppositional defiant and

other conduct disorders, Webster-Sratton (1993) declared that teachers typically encountered at

least two or more children each year that exhibited “high rates of noncompliance and defiance in

response to teacher requests, aggression, cruelty toward peers, destructive acts, lying, stealing,

and cheating” (p. 437). Webster-Stratton (1993) suggested, “4-10% of children in the U.S. meet

the criteria for oppositional defiant disorder and/or conduct disorder” (p. 437). ODD was often

included in the more general description of conduct disorders. When considering the

relationship of ODD/CD and academic performance, Webster-Stratton (1993) claimed that low

academic achievement manifested in early elementary grades in children with ODD/CD and

continued through high school. These academic deficits included reading disabilities, language

32

delays, and attention problems. Webster-Stratton (1993) said, “It is clear that conduct problems

and a lack of reading ability both place the child at high risk for lower self-esteem, continued

academic failure, further conduct problems, and school dropout” (p. 440).

ASD. According to Hedges, Kirby, Sreckovic, Kucharczyk, Hume, and Pace (2014),

“Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by deficits in

social functioning and communication with restricted interests and repetitive behaviors” (p. 65).

Hedges et al. (2014) suggested symptoms of ASD may improve during the teen years, but there

were varying levels of aggression, resistance to change, unacceptable sexual behaviors, and self-

harm behaviors exhibited among this age group. Anxiety and depression were also common

alongside ASD in adolescents. Approximately 33% of ASD students in high school were

included in general education grade-level academic classes. In this study, 67% of teachers

reported they made at least one accommodation for students with ASD. Students with ASD were

less likely to respond orally to questions, give oral presentations to the class, or work in groups

with peers. Accommodations for students with ASD typically included extra time on tests and/or

assignments or alternate assignments/tests (Hedges et al., 2014). Estes, Rivera, Bryan, Cali, and

Dawson (2010) reported “an increasing proportion of children with ASD made significant gains

in intellectual ability and behavioral functioning due to early intervention and as many as 70% of

individuals with ASD were now thought to have intellectual ability in the average to above

average range” (para. 1). Among their 30 study participants with ASD, there was an

identification of a wide range of achievement outcomes from significantly above expectations to

far below expectations based on grade level placement. Estes et al. (2010) found that reading

and spelling were specific challenge areas for students with ASD. Among their participants,

90% showed at least one discrepancy in spelling, reading, or basic numeracy skills.

33

Additionally, 60% displayed lower than predicted achievement scores. The researchers also

found that 55% of children with ASD displayed attention problems, 31% met full criteria for

ADHD diagnosis, and 7% met the criteria for ODD diagnosis (Estes et al., 2010).

Medications. In an article written for the New York Times, Sroufe (2012) claimed there

had been a twentyfold increase in the consumption of drugs for ADHD over the last 30 years.

ADHD medications were typically stimulants that increased short-term concentration. However,

when given to children long-term, they did not improve academic performance or reduce

behavior concerns (Sroufe, 2012). Two of the common medications for ADHD were Ritalin and

Adderall, which were a combination of the stimulants dextroamphetamine and amphetamine.

Stimulants worked to excite the nervous system, making it curious that these medications aided

to calm those diagnosed with ADHD to help them with attention and focusing on tasks. Sroufe

said, “Some experts argued that because the brains of children with attention problems were

different, the drugs had a mysterious paradoxical effect on them” (Sroufe, 2012, para. 10). These

stimulant medications generally had the same effects on children and adults with ADHD. They

enhanced the ability to concentrate, especially on tasks when the individual was feeling fatigued

or bored, but they did not improve broader learning abilities (Sroufe, 2012). In a study

conducted by Punja, Zorzella, Hartling, Urichuk, and Vohra (2013), “Evidence has suggested

that ADHD may be the result of insufficient production of norepinephrine and dopamine in the

prefrontal cortex, resulting in forgetfulness, distractibility, impulsivity, and inappropriate social

behaviors” (p. 2). Stimulant medications increased levels of dopamine and norepinephrine in the

prefrontal cortex, which was why they were prescribed to ADHD patients to restore the

functioning of the prefrontal cortex (Punja et al., 2013).

34

There were two main types of stimulant medications prescribed to patients with ADHD.

There was a short-acting methylphenidate medication taken three times daily for immediate

release or an extended-release methylphenidate, which used a controlled release formula

delivering the medication at a controlled rate throughout the day. While short-acting stimulants

showed in numerous studies vast improvement in ADHD symptoms, the problem with these

short-acting medications was that it was difficult to obtain patient compliance in taking the

medication as directed, especially in young children. While there was more compliance with the

once-daily, extended-release medication, they were priced up to 15 times more than the short-

acting medications (Punja et al., 2013). According to Charach & Fernandez (2013), the benefits

of psychostimulants wore off when patients did not administer the medication every 3-5 hours

for the immediate release formulas. Best practice guidelines recommended that children with

ADHD take the medication as directed all day, every day, to achieve lasting effects. Charach

and Fernandez (2013) found that once-daily, extended-release medications improved the

duration and compliance of stimulant use compared with immediate-release medications among

ADHD patients.

Student behavior was one of the many factors that influenced engagement and

achievement in the classroom. For this study, it was necessary to outline the most common

behavioral conditions diagnosed in children and adolescents, as they influenced student

engagement and achievement. ADHD, ODD, and ASD were the most commonly diagnosed

behavioral disorders in children. If students with ADHD were taking a prescribed stimulant

medication, but did not take the medication as directed, it may not be effective in managing the

behavior. If students taking an immediate-release medication took their medicine in the morning

but forgot to take their afternoon dose, their ability to pay attention and focus could be much

35

different in the morning compared with the afternoon. Therefore, it is possible that if there were

observed differences in behavior between morning and afternoon classes, then it could be due to

ADHD medication effects wearing off from the morning dose because immediate-release

medications lasted only 3-5 hours (Charach & Fernandez, 2013).

Summary

Examination of the literature regarding biological clocks, chronotype, memory and time

of day, student motivation and engagement, and classroom activity and student behavior was

necessary because the current study aimed to investigate the differences in student engagement,

achievement, and behavior among high school students at different times of the day. Based on

anecdotal evidence, and a study conducted by Wile and Shouppe (2011), there was a

commonality observed and heard in teacher workroom conversations concerning how to

motivate students in afternoon classes and consistent reports that students were more challenging

to teach in the afternoon.

The majority of research examined supported that chronotype changed with age. Young

children under the age of 13 were typically morning-type, and then a shift toward eveningness

occurred around age 13, which lasted until around age 21. As adults, there was typically a shift

back toward morningness (Gelbmann et al., 2012). While there were few studies to indicate a

difference between male and female chronotypes, those studies that did identify a difference

found females were more prone to be morning-type than males of the same age (Roenneberg et

al., 2007). The majority of studies reviewed also indicated that among adolescents, there

appeared to be a difference in academic achievement depending on the individual’s chronotype.

Previous research has shown that the majority of adolescents shifted their chronotype from

morningness to eveningness and began to stay awake later in the evening with peak performance

36

in the afternoon. However, schools started earlier as adolescents moved from middle school to

high school, which led to accumulated sleep deficiency and “social jetlag” that impaired their

performance in school (Vinne et al., 2015).

The research regarding student motivation and engagement supported that intrinsic

motivation had the most substantial effects on engagement and achievement. However, to

become permanent in children, intrinsic motivation needed to be developed and nourished.

Educators should use extrinsic motivation techniques to facilitate the development of students’

intrinsic motivation for maximum student benefit. (Saeed & Zyngier, 2012). Student behavior

influenced engagement and achievement. Three of the most common behavior disorders

afflicting adolescents were ADHD, ODD, and ASD. Medications for these behavior disorders

had a positive effect on student engagement and their ability to focus if taken as directed, but

non-compliance was a significant problem for children taking these medications. If the patient

did not follow medication instructions consistently, there were possible adverse effects on their

behavior, which negatively affected their engagement and academic performance (Punja et al.,

2013; Sroufe, 2012; Charach & Fernandez, 2013).

37

CHAPTER THREE: Methodology

The research examined in the literature review (Vollmer, et al., 2013; Gelbmann et al.,

2012; Kim et al., 2002) indicated many adolescents had a shift in their chronotype from

morningness to eveningness beginning around age 13. For high school students, this shift in

chronotype meant they had expectations to be productive during their non-optimal time of day.

Due to most adolescents identifying as evening types, they stayed up late even though they must

wake up much earlier than their biological clocks would prefer because of early school start

times (Gelbmann et al., 2012). While at school, student expectations required them to be alert,

motivated, and provide great effort regardless of their chronotype. This led to fatigue and

accumulated sleep deprivation over time, which affected their memory and overall academic

performance (Vinne et al., 2015). It is unlikely that school systems will delay start times due to

several community and family factors. Therefore, this investigation aimed to identify effective

instructional strategies to promote positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior at all

times of the day in an attempt to counteract the accumulated sleep deprivation of adolescents and

the observed differences in student performance at different times of the day due to their

chronotype preferences.

Research Question

What instructional strategies are most effective for high school teachers in promoting

positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning and afternoon

classes?

Population and Sample

Participants in the study were ninth-grade level teachers at a rural, public high school

with a student population of approximately 2,300 students. The researcher selected three

38

teachers of college-preparatory (CP), state-tested subjects (math, science, or English). All CP

classes at the selected school were composed of general education students within the

performance ability range of special education to gifted. The teacher participants must have also

taught the same course in the morning (1st or 2nd block) and in the afternoon (4th block), so the

researcher could effectively compare student performance and engagement in morning versus

afternoon classes. All participants were highly qualified teachers for their subject area. The

researcher chose teacher participants with CP courses because students enrolled in CP courses

represented the broadest range of student abilities within one class. The researcher chose teacher

participants of subjects with high stakes testing because these courses fulfilled graduation

requirements and had the most significant impact on teacher and school performance

evaluations. Participants taught the same course in the morning (1st or 2nd block) and afternoon

(4th block) because the study investigated differences in student engagement and performance at

different times of the day. Selection of ninth-grade level teachers eliminated the variable of

student age as a factor for student motivation and engagement in the classroom. All teacher

participants signed an informed consent document (See Appendix A) before any collection of

research data.

Description of Instruments

The researcher used three instruments to gather data for this investigation. First, the

researcher observed each teacher during two different class periods on the same day—once in the

morning and again in the afternoon for the same subject class. Due to the researcher also being a

teacher at the school used for the study and not able to be out of class for observations, the

lessons were video recorded and watched later for data collection. In the classroom

observations, the researcher observed any teacher modification of instruction between morning

39

and afternoon courses, and levels of engagement and performance from students in each class.

The researcher created and distributed a video release form (See Appendix B) for students and

parents to sign before recording any lessons because all students observed were under the age of

18. Any students who did not sign and return the video release form or declined being video

recorded were not visible in the recordings. To ensure privacy and confidentiality, the

recordings were only viewed by the researcher for the data coding process and were not posted

anywhere for public viewing.

After completion of classroom observations, the researcher conducted individual

interviews with each teacher participant. The researcher conducted these interviews in person at

a time chosen by the participant—either during their planning period, before school, or after

school. The interview questions (See Appendix C) allowed the researcher to gather data about

teacher perceptions of student learning, performance, and behavior at different times of the day.

Additionally, the interview questions aimed to investigate best instructional practices

implemented by each teacher for promoting maximum student engagement and achievement.

The final instrument for data collection was a focus group involving the researcher and

all three participating teachers. The focus group allowed the researcher to summarize the

findings from the personal interviews and classroom observations, and allowed for member

checking by the teachers in order for them to clarify, explain, or contribute additional

information to the findings. The researcher and participating teachers discussed any instructional

modifications that were most effective to keep students engaged and motivated at all times of

day, regardless of student chronotype.

40

Research Procedures and Time Period of Study

After receiving IRB approval and permission to conduct the study from the District

Central Office, the researcher selected three teachers to participate in the study and made

personal contact with each teacher to ask for their informed consent to participate. Selection of

teacher participants depended on whether they taught ninth grade, CP level classes in a high-

stakes tested subject area (math, science, or English). Once three teachers agreed to participate,

the researcher began scheduling classroom observations for each teacher. The researcher

observed each teacher participant in two different class periods during the same school day. To

conduct observations without missing class, the researcher used an iPad to record the lessons.

The researcher confirmed parent permission for video recording through the submission

of a video release form. To ensure privacy and confidentiality for the teacher participants and

students, only the researcher viewed recordings for the data coding process. After completion of

classroom observations, the researcher scheduled personal interviews with each participating

teacher. The researcher conducted interviews at the preference of the teacher—before school,

after school, or during their planning period. The final step in data collection was the focus

group with the researcher and all three participating teachers.

Data Analysis Procedures

The researcher conducted open coding after each step in the data collection process—

observations, interviews, and a focus group. Open coding allowed the researcher to group data

and begin establishing value and meaning to the data. The researcher conducted axial coding at

two different points in the data collection process—once after completion of personal interviews

to draw connections between open codes of the classroom observations and interviews, then

again after the focus group to identify connections among all data. Finally, after the researcher

41

collected all data and completed axial coding for the data, selective coding helped the researcher

identify the themes from the collected data.

Trustworthiness. The researcher used multiple techniques to ensure the trustworthiness

of the study. The triangulation of data collection supported the credibility of the results. The

researcher used classroom observations, personal interviews, and a focus group to provide three

methods of data collection. After the teacher participant interviews, the researcher used member

checking to allow participants to review open coding notes to ensure the researcher did not

misinterpret or misrepresent participant responses. Member checking also occurred during the

focus group with a discussion of the class observations. Participants had the opportunity to

clarify, explain, or contribute additional information to the findings. These member checks

supported the credibility of the study. The researcher also provided sufficiently thick, detailed

descriptions of the context to support the transferability of the study. The researcher compiled

detailed written and oral descriptions throughout the data collection process to demonstrate an

understanding of the research setting.

After collection of all data, a non-participant, school administrator reviewed the study to

provide peer debriefing to support the dependability of the study. Finally, the researcher

maintained an audit trail to support the confirmability of the study. The audit trail included all

video recordings of lessons, researcher notes on lesson observations, transcripts of participant

interviews and the focus group, and audio recordings of each interview and the focus group.

After the completion of data collection, the researcher compiled a detailed description of the data

collection and analysis process, coding process, and the interpretation of the data.

42

Limitations, Delimitations, and Assumptions

Limitations. The rural classification of the school used in the study was out of the

researchers’ control. The demographics of students and teachers may not be the same in another

school setting; therefore, results and conclusions were only applicable to other rural high

schools. Additionally, any personal student issues that affected student engagement and

academic performance during the time of the study may or may not have influenced the results.

As the researcher had no control over how teachers or students behaved during the video-

recorded class observations, another limitation was participants could have put on a false display

for the camera rather than an authentic representation of a daily lesson.

Delimitations. One delimitation of this research study was the use of one rural high

school setting. The researcher worked as a teacher in the high school involved in the study.

Another delimitation for the study was not to include third block classes because the school

involved in the study had four lunch periods during the third block, which meant classes took

breaks at different times during this period for lunch. This would have added other variables to

the study. The researcher selected teachers and courses involving only ninth-grade students to

control the variable of student age. The results may or may not apply to students outside of ninth

grade. A final delimitation was the selection of participants that taught CP level courses. The

high school involved in the study offered honors-level courses also, but CP level courses had the

broadest range of student abilities within one class, which provided for a broader representation

of the general population.

Assumptions. For this study, the researcher assumed that participating teachers provided

candid and transparent feedback during the interviews and focus group. The researcher also

43

assumed that teachers would demonstrate authenticity in their lesson delivery for the video-

recorded observations.

44

CHAPTER FOUR: Presentation of the Findings

The purpose of this qualitative study was to investigate the phenomenon of differences in

student engagement, achievement, and behavior at different times of the day to determine the

most effective instructional strategies educators use to overcome this phenomenon. The data

gathered, and interpretation of the results, provided insight for all education stakeholders to

improve instructional and curriculum practices in the high school classroom setting. This

chapter outlines the research approach, a detailed description of the participants and research

setting, methodology, and the presentation of the findings. The study involved the collection of

data through classroom observations, participant interviews, and a focus group. The classroom

observations aimed to provide the researcher with evidence of differences in student engagement

and behaviors at different times of the day. Three high school educators participated in

individual interviews. The interview questions aimed to gather educator insight in the areas of

student engagement, achievement, and behavior and how the time of the day influenced those

areas. The focus group participants included the researcher and the three educator participants.

The focus group aimed to gather insight into how educators modified instruction between

morning and afternoon classes. Another goal of the focus group aimed to determine which

instructional strategies were best suited for establishing student engagement and achievement at

any time of the day.

All participants in the study taught the same grade level and course level at the same

school. The triangulation of data collection using classroom observations, personal interviews,

and a focus group and the coding of the raw data led to the development of thematic findings.

45

Research Methodology Applied to Data Analysis

The research methodology applied to data analysis for this study was phenomenology.

This study investigated the phenomenon of students exhibiting differences in engagement and

achievement between morning and afternoon classes and the instructional differentiation

employed by teachers to overcome this phenomenon. The collected data took into account

anecdotal evidence from highly qualified educators and observational evidence by the researcher

to identify common themes among the data to construct a better understanding of the

phenomenon. Identification of common themes among the data allowed the researcher to draw

conclusions that could improve instructional practices for classroom educators in similar high

school settings.

Research question.

1. What instructional strategies are most effective for high school teachers in promoting

positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning and afternoon

classes?

Descriptive Characteristics of Participants

All participants were educators at the same high school located in eastern Tennessee.

Participants in the study were ninth-grade teachers at a rural, public high school with a student

population of approximately 2,300 students. The researcher selected three teachers of college-

preparatory (CP), state-tested subjects (math, science, or English). All CP classes at the selected

school included general education students within the performance ability range of special

education to gifted. The teacher participants must have also taught the same course in the

morning (1st or 2nd block) and afternoon (4th block), so the researcher could effectively compare

student performance and engagement in morning versus afternoon classes. All participants were

46

highly qualified teachers for their subject area. Participant 1 (P1) had 14 years of teaching

experience and highly qualified in the area of grades 7-12 Biology. Participant 2 (P2) had 21

years of teaching experience and highly qualified in the area of grades 7-12 English. Participant

3 (P3) had eight years of teaching experience and highly qualified in the area of grades 7-12

Math.

The researcher chose teacher participants with CP courses because students enrolled in

CP courses represented the broadest range of student abilities within one class. The researcher

chose teacher participants of subjects with high stakes testing because these courses fulfilled

graduation requirements and had the most significant impact on teacher and school performance

evaluations. Participants taught the same course in the morning (1st or 2nd block) and afternoon

(4th block) because the study investigated differences in student engagement and performance at

different times of the day. The selection of ninth-grade level teachers eliminated the variable of

student age as a factor for student motivation and engagement in the classroom. All participants

signed an informed consent form (see Appendix A) before the commencement of any data

collection.

Classroom Observations, Interviews, and Focus Group Data Presentation

The researcher first collected data from classroom observations. The researcher taught at

the high school involved in the study and could not personally perform observations. An iPad

device recorded each lesson. Each participant recorded a morning class and an afternoon class

on the same day to ensure viewing of the same lesson for both classes. The researcher provided

video release permission forms to film students (see Appendix B) because all students were

under the age of 18. No student names or other identifying information was gathered or used in

this study. The researcher collected all video release forms prior to any class observation

47

recordings. Students who did not provide consent or did not submit the video release form were

out of the camera view while recording. To ensure students were authentic in their behaviors,

students did not know when the video recording took place. No students appeared to notice the

recording device during the observations.

The purpose of the class observations was to investigate signs of engagement and other

student behaviors that could affect learning and achievement and compare those signs and

behaviors between students enrolled in morning and afternoon classes for the same subject area.

Additionally, the researcher observed any differences in teacher instruction between morning and

afternoon classes. The researcher conducted observations for P1 on February 3, 2020 for 2nd

block (9:28am-11:00am) and 4th block (1:13pm-2:45pm) Biology 1 classes. Observations for P2

were on February 4, 2020, for 2nd block and 4th block English 1 classes. Finally, observations for

P3 were on February 6, 2020, for 1st block (7:40 am-9:14 am) and 4th block Algebra 1 classes.

The researcher used notes and data gathered from the observations to develop the participant

interview questions and the guiding questions for the focus group.

Upon completion of the classroom observations, the researcher personally interviewed

each educator participant. The researcher developed the interview questions, and the dissertation

committee evaluated and approved the scripted questions. Each participant received the same

questions in the same order. Appendix C contains the questions used during the interviews. The

purpose of the participant interviews was to gain insight into educator perceptions and ideas

surrounding student engagement, achievement, and behavior and how these three factors could

be interrelated. The researcher also hoped to determine which instructional strategies the

participants found most useful to promote higher engagement and achievement in their

classrooms.

48

The researcher used Zoom, an online video conference program, to conduct the focus

group due to school closure from the COVID-19 pandemic. The researcher and the three

educator participants participated in the focus group. The questions for the focus group were

open-ended and based on data gathered from the classroom observations and personal

interviews. The primary purpose of the focus group was to identify which instructional

strategies worked best to promote the highest student engagement at different times of the day,

regardless of the content area. The researcher transcribed the audio texts from the interviews and

the focus group for the coding process to develop themes and identify any common experiences

echoed among participants.

After the collection of all data, the researcher uploaded all notes and transcripts to

MAXQDA for open coding. The researcher started with notes taken from the classroom

observations, grouped the raw data into categories associated with answering the research

question. As the researcher worked through the interviews and focus group transcripts, the

creation of more codes helped categorize the raw data until themes emerged, incorporating all

the data. The researcher worked through each piece of data twice to ensure appropriate coding

of all notes and responses. After grouping the data into similar topics related to answering the

research question, axial coding helped the researcher determine the emerging themes of the data.

After identifying emerging themes, the researcher began to see parallel trends in the data to

develop selective coding (see Figure 4.1).

49

Figure 4.1

Coding of Raw Data from Classroom Observations, Interviews, and Focus Group

50

Figure 4.1 (continued)

Coding of Raw Data from Classroom Observations, Interviews, and Focus Group

51

Figure 4.1 (continued)

Coding of Raw Data from Classroom Observations, Interviews, and Focus Group

52

Figure 4.1 (continued)

Coding of Raw Data from Classroom Observations, Interviews, and Focus Group

53

Study Findings

Based on responses from the interviews and focus group, and notes from the classroom

observations, the following themes emerged from the coding process to answer the research

question: What instructional strategies are most effective for high school teachers in promoting

positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning and afternoon

classes?

1. Academic feedback

2. Student accountability

3. Classroom management

4. Use of multiple instructional strategies

5. Short lesson segments

6. Clear and concise directions

7. Lesson pacing

8. Student attention

9. Student behavior

10. Increase in achievement

11. Incentives/Rewards

12. Student Participation

Academic feedback. The classroom observations, interviews, and focus group data all

indicated that providing academic feedback to students throughout a lesson increased student

engagement and achievement. During the observations, the researcher noticed that when the

teacher circulated around the room to answer questions and provided constructive feedback,

students were more likely to stay on task with their attention on the assignment. During the

54

focus group, P3 discussed how they used the flipped classroom method at the time of the

observations and how that method helped provide academic feedback to students during the

lesson. “In the flipped class, they learn at home, and then we just drill, drill, drill, and drill in

class. So I am able to walk around and then help them . . . [and] I was able to find the mistakes.”

In the interview with P2, they discussed how using the student Chromebooks every day helped

with academic feedback by saving instructional time. “I enjoy using technology in the

classroom. We use it every day. I like that it gives me, most of the time, immediate feedback so

I can look immediately and see who’s completed assignments.” When students received

continuous feedback throughout a lesson, it kept them focused on the lesson and held them more

accountable.

While providing feedback is imperative at all times of the day, based on the classroom

observations it was even more critical in afternoon classes to keep them on task and engaged. In

all three of the afternoon class observations, students were more talkative, fidgety, and harder to

keep focused and engaged. To overcome this change in behavior in the afternoon, all

participants circulated around the class more to correct behavior and provide immediate feedback

on assignments, which kept students on task.

Student accountability. A vital role of educators is to teach students how to hold

themselves accountable for their actions and behaviors. However, it is important not to be too

authoritarian. Students need to know their teachers are correcting behaviors and actions out of

genuine care about them and the desire for them to be successful. When asked about student

behavior differences between morning and afternoon classes, P1 said:

Second block, not as many problems. They seem to be self-moderating better. So fourth

block, I do not think kids have the same self-control, the ability to think, ‘Well, I’ve got

55

to get through this class too,’ so they end up going to sleep or whatever . . . if I try to do

very harsh treatment, very austere and just be authoritarian, that doesn’t go very well.

Based on the classroom observations, interviews, and focus group data, it appeared the

best way to hold students accountable was to monitor them closely throughout the lesson.

Teachers should correct any behaviors or actions that could affect their learning or the learning

of those students around them. All participants agreed during the focus group that circulating

and consistently monitoring/correcting student behavior held them more accountable. Student

accountability relates to engagement and achievement because if students were allowed to act

and behave how they chose in class without accountability for their actions, it did not take long

for them to begin off-task or disruptive behaviors.

Even though high school students should have more self-accountability than elementary

age students should, they are still immature and need guidance from teachers on how to manage

their actions to reach their fullest academic potential. The researcher noted evidence for this

when assigning independent work time for students without close monitoring to keep students on

task. During the morning class observation for P1, students had the last thirty minutes of class to

work on two different assignments. After going over their instructions, the teacher sat down at

their desk. Five minutes into working independently, fifteen students were off task not working

on their assignment. The teacher circulated a few times after noticing off-task behaviors, but did

not consistently circulate and several students remained off task. However, in the afternoon

class, P1 started circulating immediately after giving student instructions and students displayed

more focus on their assignment. When the teacher continued to circulate and give feedback to

students throughout the independent work, students showed higher engagement and the teacher

held them more accountable. In the morning class observations, there were fewer behavior

56

issues from students and they remained mostly on task, which did not require the teacher to walk

around prompting or correcting off task behavior. In the afternoon class observations, there were

more off task behaviors from students and the participants responded to this by circulating to

prompt or correct the behaviors and kept students on task and accountable.

Classroom Management. Effective classroom management to regulate student behavior

is an essential skill for any educator. If student behavior is out of control, learning is less likely

to occur. During the classroom observations, distractions for students came primarily from the

behavior of their peers, so teachers needed to monitor behavior consistently and appropriately

deal with problem behaviors. The participants agreed that the key to classroom management was

consistency, teacher-student relationships, and close monitoring by the teacher. When describing

how building a personal connection with students often led to fewer behavior problems and a

stronger work ethic, P3 affirmed, “Well, if they don’t feel like they’re separated from the class or

if they feel some sort of personal connection to me, they’re more apt to give me what I want.

They’re more apt to try harder.” During the focus group, P1 also discussed the importance of

forming relationships with students to increase engagement. P1 said, “You got to have some

informal connection socially with your kids. That tremendously helps engagement. [When

students realize] ‘Oh, he really is on my side. . . . He really wants me to succeed. Okay. I’ll work

and I’ll work harder.’”

In addition to forming positive relationships with students to decrease behavior problems

and increase engagement, consistent student monitoring was key for classroom management and

maintaining student engagement and focus, especially in afternoon classes. During the focus

group, all participants agreed that teacher circulation was effective in helping regulate classroom

discipline. In all three class observations, as soon as the teacher stopped circulating, the chatter

57

started, and it would quickly build. Participants also agreed that classroom management and

student behavior were more problematic in afternoon classes, and more frequent monitoring was

required in the afternoon compared to the morning.

Use of multiple instructional strategies. When participants used multiple instructional

strategies throughout their lesson, it kept the attention of the students, which increased their

engagement. Using multiple instructional strategies seemed to be equally effective at all times of

the day. During the focus group, P2 said:

You don’t want to spend the entire period reading. You want to break it up. You do the

DGP, you read a little, you answer some questions, you have a video, or you have

something else in there. So I think maybe changing three times during a class period or

mixing it up a little bit. . . . Because they can’t be doing the same work, their brain working

the same way the whole period.

When asked about instructional strategies that promoted higher achievement, P2

indicated, “I can’t make every lesson a game or a group or a station type of thing, but don’t do

the same thing every single day the same way because then they get bored.” During the focus

group, P3 added to the discussion on using multiple instructional strategies throughout a lesson

saying, “So I guess I never thought of it, but I guess I always have tried to have at least three

separate slots of time for something in class.” The research participants agreed that it was

important to use different instructional strategies during class, and all agreed that three to four

different activities probably worked best. Participants also agreed that if teachers tried to do too

many different activities, the purpose of the activities/assignments might get lost because

students need enough time to work with and process the information provided in each

assignment.

58

Short lesson segments. In addition to using multiple instructional strategies in a lesson,

it was evident from the lesson observations that educators should keep each lesson segment as

short and concise as possible to maintain student attention and engagement. Short lesson

segments appeared to be especially important for maintaining engagement in afternoon classes.

When asked about how student behavior related to engagement, P1 explained, “I think behavior-

wise, I just can’t do it for too long. It’s got to be short. . . . Especially my regular kids, I got to do

something that’s fast.” When asked about how they differentiated between morning and

afternoon classes, P2 talked about the importance of taking short breaks between lesson

segments in afternoon classes. “But fourth period, they need to get up. They need to stretch.

They need a drink of water. They need to go to the bathroom more. They need to move. So I

try to give them that opportunity.” The importance of providing short breaks in the afternoon to

keep students engaged was also evident in the classroom observations.

After watching all the lesson observations, it was evident there was more fatigue and

sleepiness in the afternoon classes. It also became apparent after watching the lesson

observations that the length of each lesson segment was necessary for maintaining student

attention and engagement. Based on notes taken from the observations, students could mostly

focus their attention on an assignment or activity for about 25 minutes before they became

distracted or participated in off-task behaviors. The attention span decreased greatly in afternoon

classes. Overall, students in the afternoon classes were able to focus on a task for about half the

time of students in morning classes. Because of this observation, shorter lesson segments could

be especially useful in afternoon classes for promoting higher student engagement.

Clear and concise directions. Students in the lesson observations combatted several

distractions throughout the lessons. Their peers in the class, something passing by the window,

59

visitors, teacher phone ringing, intercom announcements, etc., distracted students. Due to these

significant distractions, it was essential that the participants provided clear, concise directions

and repeated them multiple times for students to comprehend the instructions and expectations.

P2 elaborated on the implementation of clear, concise directions at different times of day stating,

“Well, afternoon classes need the directions repeated several more times.” P1 also affirmed this

idea by saying, “If I let them do a lot of things on their own they get less engaged. They need

more direction. They need more of me clarifying what’s going on.”

The researcher realized the importance of clear and concise directions while watching the

classroom observation videos. In the morning observations for P1 and P3, there was time at the

end of class for students to work on assignments independently. Both P1 and P3 listed two or

three different assignments the students should have worked on for the remaining class time. In

both classes, students worked well for about 5-10 minutes, but off-task behavior quickly

escalated. In their afternoon classes for that same lesson, both P1 and P3 changed their delivery

of instructions. P1 told them to focus on working on an online quiz, while P3 told students to

work on practice problems in the Delta Math program on their Chromebooks. This minor

adjustment in giving students one thing to work on rather than two or three led to a significant

increase in engagement compared with the morning classes. P1 contributed to this discussion

during the focus group expressing, “Sometimes, I like to give options. And I think maybe that’s

a mistake. If I’m going to give them options, I need to write them up on the board, ‘Do this

first, do this second, do this third.’” All participants agreed that repeating clear and concise

directions appeared to increase student engagement, especially in afternoon classes.

Lesson pacing. There was an obvious difference in attention spans and engagement

between morning and afternoon classes after watching the recorded lessons. Two of the three

60

participants adjusted the pace of their lesson between morning and afternoon to accommodate

those differences. Student attention spans significantly reduced in the afternoon compared with

morning classes. When discussing this observation in the focus group, P2 attested, “Yeah. Well,

I think as it gets closer to 2:40. If the clock gets close to 2:40, they’re done. They’re done. So

you do probably have to make it a little faster to get it finished before their attention span runs

out.” When asked about whether they had ever observed differences in student behavior

between morning and afternoon classes, P3 affirmed:

First is sloths. Fourth is rabbits. . . . Behavior wise, I think fourth period, they’re just

harder to keep in their seats, harder to keep, not that they need to stay in their seats, but

obviously I’m teaching. They need to stop talking and they need to not be throwing stuff

and things like that. So fourth period is harder to tame…But I feel like, thinking back to

my entire teaching career, I feel like every fourth period is always, kids are done for the

day and it’s the last period of the day. So they’re done. They’re energetic to be free and

go out, especially on sunny days, they’re excited to get out of here.

All participants agreed that they typically move at a faster pace in afternoon classes. P1 said this

could also be because “We’ve also done the same lesson multiple times.” Therefore, whether the

faster pace in the afternoon was due to multiple rehearsals of the lesson or recognition of shorter

attention spans in the afternoon, the data showed a faster lesson pace in afternoon classes helped

maintain student engagement.

Student attention. After examining the classroom observations and discussing those

observations with the participants in the focus group, the data showed that student attention

might change throughout the school day. After watching the recorded lessons, it was evident

that in morning classes, the participants had their students’ attention for about 10 to 15 minutes.

61

By the fourth block, the observed overall attention span decreased to five to seven minutes

before they started participating in off-task or disruptive behaviors. The chatter and talking were

much quicker to escalate in the afternoon also. Students seemed to lose focus in half the amount

of time in the afternoon when compared with morning classes. When asked about differences in

student engagement between morning and afternoon, P2 said, “[Fourth block is] just full of

energy, like an electron. Just go all over the place but then not using that attention to focus on

me. Just having lots of energy, but not using that to actually focus.” In addition, when asked

how student behavior relates to engagement, P2 attested about fourth block, “They seem to be

more engaged because that energy does hone in on me every so often. So they do answer more

questions and they do pay attention, but it’s kind of the attention span of a toddler.”

During the focus group, participants discussed how long they could typically keep

student attention. All participants agreed that they could hold students’ attention for a maximum

of 25-30 minutes before they became distracted and off-task. While attention spans seemed to be

shorter in the afternoon, morning classes needed different strategies to keep their attention. P2

had some insight into attention spans in morning classes. P2 explained:

So, I try to be more excited first period so I can wake them up and then I give them a

break and I tell them to move around and things like that. Whereas fourth period, we

don’t really need a break besides to tame the chaos.

Based on the data, teachers were more energetic and upbeat and gave more breaks for movement

to keep students awake and maintain their attention in the morning, but in the afternoon,

participants had to tame students down to keep their attention.

Student behavior. The data showed that student behavior influenced engagement and

achievement at any time of the day. Due to decreased attention spans and being more energetic

62

and active in afternoon classes, student behavior was often more problematic in the afternoon

classes. When asked about any observed differences in behavior between morning and afternoon

classes, P3 expressed, “Morning, more often in their chairs, not wanting to get up and go to the

bathroom. Fourth block . . . definitely more antsy, wanting to get more active.”

While coding the data from the classroom observations, the researcher observed 14

disruptive behaviors, but only four of those behaviors came from students in morning classes.

The majority of disruptive behavior occurred in the afternoon classes. The researcher also

noticed during the classroom observations that more challenging or disruptive behavior came

from male students compared to females. P2 supported this by saying, “So fourth period is

harder to tame, but also I have some typical boys my fourth period this semester.” When

discussing the behavior of ‘typical boys’ during the focus group, all participants agreed that it

was more often male students who were off-task, fidgety, or trying to distract other students.

When discussing how technology relates to student engagement, P2 disclosed, “There are some

boys that like to get on cool math or other games…I don’t feel like girls will do it as much, but

boys will definitely get on there and play a computer game at any opportunity.” All participants

agreed this observation regarding male student behavior could take place at any time of the day,

but male students displayed more off-task or disruptive behaviors in afternoon classes compared

with the morning.

Increase in achievement. Data from class observations, interviews, and the focus group

indicated that when students showed engagement, there was an increase in achievement from the

perspective of the teacher. When asked about observed differences in student achievement

between morning and afternoon classes, P3 acknowledged:

63

I mean obviously there’s probably a correlation between those kids [4th block] being

buzzy and crazy and not paying attention, to lower achievement. First period is tired,

which translates into them not focusing as well. And then fourth period’s energetic, so

they also don’t focus as well. So I feel like it probably balances out to being the same

amount of achievement.

When asked the same question regarding achievement, P2 disclosed about afternoon classes:

Often those are the classes that have lower grades compared to the morning classes

because there are more missing assignments. . . . Because they’re just, they’re tired and

they lose focus. There have been differences in test scores. Not so much this semester,

but last semester I had a huge gap from my test scores and grades from my fourth period

to my first period.

When asked the same question regarding differences in achievement between morning

and afternoon classes, P1 stated, “As far as achievement goes, I don’t think I’ve noticed that

much of a difference. I’d have to look at my scores.” When asked about the relationship

between student engagement and achievement, P3 said, “Yes, there is definitely a positive

correlation, strong positive correlation between student engagement and student achievement.

Which, I think student engagement looks a little different between first and fourth.” In response

to the same question concerning the relationship between student engagement and achievement,

P2 stated, “Well you can’t have one without the other, in my opinion. They have to be involved.

They have to know what’s going on in class. It’s paramount. They have to be engaged to pass

and to get the assignment.” During the focus group, the participants discussed strategies that

could increase student engagement, which in turn would boost achievement.

64

The researcher and participants discussed how formative assessment strategies were

effective in promoting engagement and measuring student achievement. P3 said, “If I didn’t do

formative assessments, I would miss those gaps that most of those kids missed somehow in

eighth grade. So for me, it gets them back on track.” All participants agreed that formative

assessments allowed teachers to measure achievement daily and useful tools for promoting

student engagement. While there may not be a consistent agreement among the participants that

achievement differs between morning and afternoon, all participants agreed that student

engagement definitely leads to an increase in achievement, and they agreed there was typically

higher engagement in the morning compared with the afternoon.

Incentives or rewards. Based on data gathered from the participants, another way to

increase student engagement at any time of the day was the use of incentives or rewards during

class. Incentives observed fostered productive and positive behavior from students, which

decreased distractions and promoted higher engagement. In the observation of their afternoon

class, P3 used the incentive of allowing students to take out their phones and listen to music if

they worked diligently for ten minutes. P3 noticed students were beginning off-task, disruptive

behavior, so they used the phones as an incentive for students to focus on their work. The off-

task, disruptive behaviors almost immediately stopped, and students began working on the

assignment. After ten minutes, students could retrieve their phones and listen to music. They

continued to work diligently for the remainder of class time. Participants also agreed that

incentives could promote higher achievement from students. P2 articulated:

I give out candy for answering questions right. And sometimes I’ll hide little notes on a

study guide and be like, ‘If you actually saw this, give me a fist bump tomorrow.’ So

then the next day I’m like, ‘Does anybody owe me anything?’ And then of course the

65

kids who didn’t study don’t. And then the kids who did study, give me a fist bump and

then it’s a little secret inside joke that we have.”

When asked about instructional strategies that promoted engagement and achievement, P2 said,

“Things where they can get up and move or move to stations or groups, I think helps with ninth-

graders in particular.”

P2 sometimes used movement as an incentive or motivator for students. P3 also

mentioned in the focus group they used gamification in the form of ‘board races’ to motivate

students, and the winning teams received food rewards of their choice. P2 explained, “They love

the board races. The first three teams that win, I’ll just go buy them what they want, like Takis

or whatever. So they’re running to the board and it’s chaos, but it’s fun.”

All participants agreed that pairing incentives or rewards with formative assessment

strategies promoted higher engagement because the students were having fun earning the

rewards, and when they were having fun, there was more engagement. Participants also agreed

that formative assessments were useful tools for promoting engagement and achievement at any

time of day, which made them effective strategies to overcome the observed differences in

student engagement between morning and afternoon classes.

Student participation. From the gathered data, student participation in the lesson

fostered higher engagement and achievement at any time of the day. However, the amount of

participation varied from morning to afternoon. P1 stated, “Second block feels more fresh with

their engagement. They are trying. They may be writing things down more, the pens aren’t

getting heavy to them.” When asked about fourth block regarding student participation, P3 said,

“They do answer more questions, and they do pay attention, but it’s kind of an attention span of a

toddler.” When P2 was asked about how their question trail activity that involved moving

66

around the room affected the student’s engagement, they said, “I can see them actually moving

around and participating. Again, they like it so they’re engaged in it. There’s nobody that’s just

sitting down. . . . They all actually get up and participate.”

When asked about instructional strategies used to foster high engagement and

achievement, P3 said, “I let the students just give me numbers and scenarios or just make up

problems on the spot, and they seem more apt to want to do questions that they made up on their

own.” According to P2, students were more likely to participate if they were actively involved in

creating something for the lesson. This possibly created a personal investment for them, which

made it more likely they would participate and increase engagement and achievement. All

participants agreed there was often more student participation in afternoon classes compared

with the morning because they were more awake and energetic. However, due to their decreased

attention span in the afternoon, the participation may not be as effective in promoting

engagement and achievement in afternoon classes.

Trustworthiness

Several methods used throughout the study ensured trustworthiness. The use of three

data sources in the forms of classroom observations, participant interviews, and a focus group

provided triangulation of data to enhance trustworthiness. Transcriptions of all interviews and

the focus group allowed member checking for participants to review and ensure accurate

recording of responses and responses came from their personal perspective. An impartial peer

reviewer examined the questions and transcriptions to ensure the methods of collection were

valid. The peer reviewer also helped ensure proper interpretation of the results from the data

collection and analysis. All participants had access to the transcripts of their interview and the

focus group upon request for member checking to ensure accurate recording and reporting of

67

responses. The researcher maintained an audit trail including all student permission forms for

recording lessons, participant consent forms, video recordings, researcher notes from

observations, transcripts of participant interviews, and a video recording and transcript of the

focus group.

Summary

Through classroom observations, interviews, and a focus group, data gathered helped the

researcher understand and provide insight into answering the research question. The data

revealed several key instructional strategies most effective in promoting positive student

engagement, achievement, and behavior when accommodating for differences in these attributes

between morning and afternoon classes. First, the data indicated frequent circulation by the

teacher throughout the lesson increased student engagement, especially in afternoon classes.

Second, the data showed that separating lessons into multiple, small segments with clear, concise

directions led to a higher engagement at any time of the day, especially in afternoon classes.

Third, when educators adjusted lesson pacing to accommodate differences in student attention

spans from morning to afternoon, it fostered higher engagement and achievement. Finally, the

data showed the use of multiple formative assessment strategies throughout a lesson increased

student participation, cultivating higher engagement and achievement at all times of the day, but

especially in morning classes when participation was typically lower.

68

CHAPTER FIVE: Conclusions, Implications, and Recommendations

This study aimed to identify instructional strategies most effective in promoting positive

engagement, achievement, and behavior from high school students despite time of the day. The

researcher selected three highly qualified, practicing high school educators in high-stakes tested

subject areas of college-preparatory (CP) level Biology I, Algebra 1, and English 1 as

participants. The researcher initially completed two classroom observations, followed by

individual interviews with each participant. The researcher observed each participant once in a

morning class and again for their afternoon class on the same day to observe any differences in

student engagement, participation, or behavior. The researcher also observed any differences in

teacher instruction between morning and afternoon classes. The final step in data collection was

a focus group discussion after completion of all classroom observations and participant

interviews. The purpose of this study was to identify instructional strategies most effective in

overcoming the discrepancy in student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning

and afternoon classes.

Statement of Problem

Existing research on the topic of how the time of the day affects performance focused on

whether there was a difference in human performance at different times of the day (Carrier &

Monk, 2000; Fabbri, Natale, & Adan, 2008; Onder, Horzum, & Besoluk, 2012; Preckel et al.,

2013; Randler, 2011; Randler, Bechtold, & Vogel, 2016; Randler, Rahafer, Arbabi, and

Bretschneider, 2014; Roenneberg et al., 2007; Vollmer, Potsch, and Randler, 2013; Zavada,

Gordijn, Beersma, Daen, & Roenneberg, 2005). The research established differences in

preferred times of day and performance (Biss and Hasher, 2012; Gelbmann et al., 2012;

Roenneberg et al., 2007; Kim, Dueker, Hasher, & Goldstein, 2002; Vinne et al., 2015). This

69

qualitative study focused on observed differences in student engagement, achievement, and

behavior between morning and afternoon classes and the identification of best practice strategies

for promoting these attributes to keep students engaged at all times of the day and promote

equitable performance from all students regardless of the time of the day they are in class.

Research Question

Observations of students and perspectives of highly qualified, state-tested subject area

teachers allowed the researcher to gain insight and understanding in connection to the following

research question:

1. What instructional strategies are most effective for high school teachers in promoting

positive student engagement, achievement, and behavior when accommodating for

differences in student engagement, achievement, and behavior between morning and

afternoon classes?

Theoretical Approach

The theoretical foundation for this study was constructivism. According to Olusegun

(2015), constructivism suggested that people construct their knowledge and understanding

through experiences and reflecting on those experiences. To accomplish this, people must ask

questions, explore, and assess what they already know. In the classroom setting, constructivism

involves encouraging students to use active learning techniques such as experiments and real-

world problem solving. The basic characteristics of constructivist learning environments

included the sharing of knowledge between students and teachers, shared authority between

students and teachers, heterogeneous small learning groups in the classroom, and the teacher

acting as a facilitator or guide for learning. Some of the goals associated with constructivist

learning environments included students determining how they will learn, student-centered

70

learning, classroom collaboration, and student reflection on their learning (Olusegun, 2015).

Constructivist theory best suited this investigation because it promoted the active involvement of

students in their learning, which promotes higher engagement and overall academic performance

(Saeed & Zyngier, 2012).

In addition to constructivism, best practice strategies to promote student engagement and

achievement were a foundation for this study. The research problem concerns how students

exhibited different levels of engagement, achievement, and behavior at different times of the day.

Best practice strategies for promoting achievement and engagement guided the researcher in the

formation of the teacher interview questions and the focus group discussion questions. The

researcher needed to familiarize herself with the research regarding the most current best practice

strategies for promoting student motivation and engagement in today’s generation of students.

This information was crucial for the foundation of the study, development of the interviews and

focus group questions, and provided the researcher with necessary background knowledge while

coding the data from classroom observations. The constructivist model encourages students to

be active participants in their learning while the teacher acts more as a facilitator who prompts,

mediates, and coaches students as they develop and assess their understanding of the material.

The research methodology applied to data analysis for this study was phenomenology.

This study investigated the phenomenon of students exhibiting differences in engagement and

achievement between morning and afternoon classes and the instructional differentiation

employed by teachers to overcome this phenomenon. The collected data took into account

anecdotal evidence from highly qualified educators and observational evidence by the researcher

to identify common themes among the data to construct a better understanding of the

phenomenon. Identification of common themes among the data allowed the researcher to draw

71

conclusions that could improve instructional practices for classroom educators in similar high

school settings.

Conclusions and Summary of Findings

This qualitative study explored differences in student engagement between morning and

afternoon classes and teacher perceptions about instructional strategies that worked best to

accommodate those observed differences to increase student engagement and achievement

despite time of the day. The researcher used classroom observations, personal interviews, and a

focus group as a basis for this study. Many factors may contribute to student engagement and

achievement. The data for this study revealed several themes such as academic feedback,

classroom management, student accountability, use of multiple instructional strategies, short

lesson segments, clear and concise directions, lesson pacing, student attention, student behavior,

incentives and rewards, and student participation. These themes became evident during the data

coding process. The researcher started categorizing the data using open coding. After

completion of open coding for all data sources, the researcher used axial coding to generate

emerging themes. Based on these emerging themes, the researcher identified four main

conclusions from the data using selective coding.

Frequent circulation during lessons. The first conclusion drawn from the data was that

frequent and consistent circulation by the teacher throughout the lesson increased student

engagement, especially in afternoon classes. Active monitoring of students fostered higher

engagement by holding students accountable for their actions during class, which made them

more likely to participate in the lesson. The frequent circulation also provided students with

consistent academic feedback throughout the lesson using questioning, prompting, checks for

understanding, and other formative assessment strategies. In addition, when the teacher was

72

more involved with students individually while circulating, they were more likely to form

positive relationships with students, which can increase their motivation to perform well for that

teacher. Finally, frequent circulation throughout a lesson greatly improved classroom

management by reducing disruptive behavior, keeping students on task, and prompting students

to stay awake and focused on the lesson. Teacher circulation was especially crucial in afternoon

classes. The data showed that student attention decreased substantially from morning to

afternoon, and they displayed more off-task or disruptive behaviors in the afternoon. Circulation

and active monitoring significantly reduced these unwanted behaviors and kept students focused

on their lesson tasks. Research by Cotton (1988) claimed the careful monitoring of student

progress was one of the major factors distinguishing effective teachers from ineffective ones.

The study found that monitoring student progress was a strong predictor of student achievement.

Chunking lesson into segments with clear, concise directions. A second conclusion

drawn from the data suggested separating a lesson into multiple, small segments providing clear

and concise directions can lead to increased engagement despite time of the day. The data

showed that students could focus on a task for a maximum of 25-30 minutes before they started

to lose engagement and display off-task behaviors. This timeframe was the same for morning

and afternoon based on data gathered in this study. The high school involved in the study was on

a block schedule, meaning students were in each class for 90 minutes. This length of time means

teachers should break up their lessons into 3-4 smaller segments to prevent student fatigue and

loss of engagement.

The data also revealed that clear and concise directions are required for each lesson

segment to accomplish full focus and engagement from students. When given a list of multiple

assignments at once without repeating clear directions, many students lost interest after 5-10

73

minutes and began off-task disruptive behaviors. However, when given clear, concise directions

for one task at a time, students clearly understood the expectations and maintained a higher focus

and engagement on the assignment. Teachers should employ a variety of instructional strategies

to keep students’ focus and attention. While the data showed the importance of using 3-4 lesson

segments daily, those 3-4 strategies should be frequently changed because students become

bored and lose focus if exposed to the same 3-4 learning strategies every day. The participants

involved in this study used strategies such as class discussion, videos, whole group instruction,

group/partner work, problem solving, and gamification strategies.

Adjust lesson pacing. The data showed that when participants adjusted their lesson

pacing to accommodate differences in attention spans from morning to afternoon, there was more

student engagement. The data from classroom observations indicated that students in the

morning had longer attention spans and could focus well on a task for 10-15 minutes before they

became distracted and engaged in off-task behavior. In afternoon classes, the attention span

decreased to approximately 5-7 minutes for most students. This was especially true with male

students. Previous research indicated there might be a slight difference in chronotype between

males and females (Duffy et al., 2010 & Roenneberg et al., 2007), possibly due to higher

estrogen levels in adolescent females.

The data from this study indicated a noticeable difference in attention span and disruptive

behavior between males and females in ninth grade. There were 14 disruptive behaviors

observed during the classroom observations, and only one of those was from a female student.

In addition, four of the 14 disruptive behavior incidents occurred in morning classes, while 10

occurred in afternoon classes. After discussing these observations with the participants during

the focus group, they agreed that male students typically displayed more disruptive behaviors

74

than females, and male students typically have a lower attention span. They also agreed that this

trend was worse in afternoon classes compared to the morning.

The participants unknowingly compensated for the change in attention span by moving at

a faster pace in the afternoon classes. Moving through the lesson at a quicker pace compensated

for the decreased attention spans, reduced off-task and disruptive behavior, and thus maintained

higher student engagement. All three participants had extra time left at the end of their afternoon

class and implemented a lesson summary closure activity to fill in the additional few minutes.

One participant suggested a faster pace in afternoon classes could also be due to lesson repetition

by the teacher throughout the day. The participants agreed if they were teaching the same lesson

two to three times each day that they were more likely to move faster by the final lesson of the

day. While lesson pacing should be adjusted based on student attention and engagement,

teachers should be conscious to prevent moving faster due to repetition and making lesson

shortcuts in afternoon classes.

Use of multiple formative assessment strategies. Finally, the data showed that the use

of multiple formative assessments throughout a lesson increased student engagement at any time

of the day. Formative assessment strategies used by the study participants included general

questioning, gamification with Kahoot, Plickers, Quizizz, board races, question trails, and

questions and responses using small whiteboards. The interviews and focus group revealed

formative assessments increased student participation and engagement by infusing incentives or

rewards such as food, extra points, or a fun activity involving movement. Formative assessments

also increased student participation because students were more likely to participate in activities

where they received immediate feedback or an incentive/reward. When students actively

participated, they were actively engaged, which should result in higher achievement. Formative

75

assessments also helped teachers gauge the mastery of the material and their lesson pacing.

Formative assessment strategies are also typically quick and could be done to transition to

another segment of the lesson. Using quick and fun assessment strategies retained the focus and

attention of students at any time of the day, but they were especially effective in afternoon

classes.

Implications

The results of this qualitative study indicated how high school educators could modify

their instructional practices to accommodate for differences in student engagement, achievement,

and behavior at different times of the day. High school educators can improve student

engagement and achievement due to the time of the day differences among adolescents in several

ways. They can use frequent circulation to monitor students throughout the lesson, fragment

their lessons into 3-4 smaller segments for block schedule, adjust lesson pacing to meet student

attention span needs—slower pace in morning classes and faster pace in afternoon classes, and

using multiple formative assessment strategies with incentives throughout the lesson to gauge

achievement and promote participation.

This study suggested that the use of frequent monitoring and a diversity of short

instructional strategies can significantly increase student engagement and achievement by

decreasing off-task, disruptive behaviors, promoting student participation, and increasing student

accountability. Educators and all stakeholders in education can use these findings to improve

instructional practices and teacher evaluations. Using these simple strategies could significantly

improve classroom management for educators and student participation, engagement, and

achievement among high school students despite misalignment of the typical school schedule

with their chronotype.

76

Limitations

The rural classification of the school used in the study was out of the researcher’s control.

The demographics of students and teachers may not be the same in another school setting;

therefore, results and conclusions are only applicable to other rural high schools. Additionally,

any personal student issues that affected student engagement and academic performance during

the time of the study may or may not have influenced the results. As the researcher had no

control over how teachers or students behaved during video-recorded class observations, another

limitation is participants could have put on a false display for the camera rather than an authentic

representation of a daily lesson.

Delimitations

One delimitation of this research study was the use of one rural high school setting. The

researcher worked as a teacher in the high school involved in the study. Another delimitation for

the study was not to include third block classes because the school involved in the study had four

lunch periods during the third block, which meant classes were broken up at different times

during this period for lunch. These breaks would have added other variables to the study. The

researcher selected teachers and courses involving only ninth-grade students to control the

variable of student age. The results may or may not apply to students in other grade levels. A

final delimitation was the selection of participants that taught CP level courses. The high school

involved in the study offered honors-level courses also, but CP level courses had the widest

range of student abilities within one class, which provided for a broader representation of the

general population.

77

Recommendations for Further Research

During the focus group, there was a discussion regarding recommendations for further

research to investigate how time of the day affects learning among high school students. One

participant suggested investigating how different class sizes affect student engagement. It would

be interesting to see if any differences exist in student engagement at different times of the day

when comparing smaller class sizes (less than 25) with classes of more than 25 students.

Because the results of this study indicated best instructional practices to promote high

engagement at different times of day, utilization of these strategies would be necessary for both

class size samples to see if those same strategies work despite the class size.

Another recommendation would be to apply the same study in an elementary school

setting. Previous research suggested there was a significant shift in chronotype from childhood

to adolescence (Gelbmann et al., 2012), which could have a significant impact on student

engagement at different times of the day. It would be interesting to see if there are differences in

engagement among elementary students throughout the school day or if there are different

instructional strategies that work best to engage those students and compare the results with the

results of this study.

A third recommendation for further research is to investigate how different course

schedules may influence student engagement. The school involved in this study uses a block

schedule where students go to four classes per day, 90 minutes each. A traditional school

schedule typically involves 6-8 class periods, 45-60 minutes each. It would be interesting to

repeat this study in a high school with similar demographics on a traditional schedule to see if

shorter classes eliminate the decrease in attention span and engagement by the end of the day.

78

A fourth recommendation would be to investigate the differences in attention,

participation, behavior, and engagement between male and female high school students. The

previous research indicated there might be a slight difference in chronotype between adolescent

males and females due to increases in estrogen in females at puberty (Duffy et al., 2010). Based

on the classroom observations from this study, male students overwhelmingly showed more

signs of off-task, disruptive behavior, which are indicators of disengagement. The best practices

identified in this study worked well to keep all students engaged, but there may be other

strategies to keep the attention and engagement of male students compared with females.

A final recommendation would be to conduct a quantitative study measuring achievement

differences between morning and afternoon classes. While this study found differences in student

behavior, attention, and engagement between morning and afternoon classes, all participants

agreed these factors influence achievement. Only one participant noticed a discrepancy in

achievement between morning and afternoon classes based on summative assessments in class

and state-standardized tests. The other two participants did not deny a difference, but had never

noticed it in the past. A quantitative study measuring achievement differences on end-of-course

state standardized exams between morning and afternoon classes could strengthen and support

the findings from this study.

Summary

The results of this study were relevant for all high school educators to consider.

Identifying best practices to overcome differences in student engagement, achievement, and

behavior between morning and afternoon classes has been a question the researcher strived to

answer since her first year of teaching. Achieving and maintaining student engagement is one of

the most challenging tasks for an educator. The best practices identified in this study to promote

79

student engagement at different times of day were frequent circulation and monitoring,

fragmenting lessons into multiple, short segments, adjusting lesson pacing to accommodate for

differences in student attention spans between morning and afternoon, and using multiple

formative assessments with incentives for students. Implementation of these strategies will help

high school educators overcome discrepancies in student attention, participation, engagement,

achievement, and behavior between morning and afternoon classes.

80

References

Akinbami, L., Llu, X., Pastor, P., & Reuben, C. (2011). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

among children aged 5-17 years in the United States, 1998-2009. Retrieved December

27, 2019, from National Center for Health Statistics Data Brief: http://www.cdc.gov

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with school:

Critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology in the

Schools, 45(5), 369-386. doi:10.1002/pits

Au, W. (2007). High-stakes testing and curricular control: A qualitative metasynthesis.

Educational Researcher, 36(5), 258-267. Retrieved from

http://www.jstor.org/stable/30137912

Berger, D. D. S. (2000). The effects of time of day of task presentation and individual differences

in self-reported optimal performance periods on the memory of adolescents enrolled in

an early starting time high school (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest

Dissertations & Theses Global. (Order No. 9969677)

Besoluk, S. (2011). Morningness-eveningness preferences and university entrance examination

scores of high school students. Personality & Individual Differences, 50(2), 248-252.

doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.038

Biss, R., & Hasher, L. (2012). Happy as a lark: Morning-type younger and older adults are

higher in positive affect. Emotion, 12(3), 437. doi:10.1037/a0027071

Bulger, S., & Watson, D. (2006). Broadening the definition of at-risk students. The Community

College Enterprise, 23-36. Retrieved from https://www.schoolcraft.edu/pdfs/cce/12.2.23-

32.pdf

81

Callan, R. J. (1999). Effects on matching and mismatching students' time-of-day preferences.

Journal of Educational Research, 92(5), 295-299. doi:10.1080/00220679909597609

Carrier, J. & Monk, T. H. (2000). Circadian rhythms of performance: New trends.

Chronobiology International, 17(6), 719-732. doi:10.1081/CBI-100102108

Carskadon, M. (2011). Sleep in adolescents: the perfect storm. Pediatric Clinics of North

America, 58(3), 637. doi:10.1016/j.pcl.2011.03.003

Charach, A. & Fernandez, R. (2013). Enhancing ADHD medication adherence: Challenges and

opportunities. Current Psychiatry Reports, 15(7), 371-379. doi:10.1007/s11920-013-

0371-6

Circadian rhythm. (n.d.). Psychology Today. Retrieved from https://www.psychologytoday.com

/us/basics/circadian-rhythm

Cotton, K. J. (1988). Monitoring student learning in the classroom: School improvement research

series close-up. Northwest Regional Educational Lab. Retrieved from

https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED298085

Díaz-Morales, J., & Escribano, C. (2013). Circadian preference and thinking styles: Implications

for school achievement. Chronobiology International, 30(10), 1231- 1239.

Drabick, D. A., Gadow K. D. (2012). Deconstructing oppositional defiant disorder: Clinic-based

evidence for an anger/irritability phenotype. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry. 51, 384–392. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.010

Duffy, J., Cain, S., Chang, A., Phillips, A., Münch, M., Gronfier, C., ... & Czeisler, C. (2011).

Sex difference in the near-24-hour intrinsic period of the human circadian timing system.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(3), 15602-15608.

doi:10.1073/pnas.1010666108

82

Estes, A., Rivera, V., Bryan, M., Cali, P., & Dawson, G. (2010). Discrepancies between

academic achievement and intellectual ability in higher functioning school-aged children

with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41,

1044-1052. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1127-3

Fabbri, M., Natale, V., & Adan, A. (2008). Effect of time of day on arithmetic fact retrieval in

a number-matching task. Acta Psychologica, 127, 485–490.

doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.08.011

Gelbmann, G., Kuhn-Natriashvili, S., Pazhedath, T., Ardeljan, M., Wöber, C., & Wöber- Bingöl,

C. (2012). Morningness: Protective factor for sleep-related and emotional problems in

childhood and adolescence?. Chronobiology international, 29(7), 898-910.

doi:10.3109/07420528.2012.686946

Goldstein, D., Hahn, C. S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Time of day,

intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in morning versus evening type

adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and Individual Differences Journal,

42(3), 431-440. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.008

Hedges, S. H., Kirby, A. V., Sreckovic, M. A., Kucharczyk, S., Hume, K., & Pace, S. (2014).

Falling through the cracks: Challenges for high school students with Autism Spectrum

Disorder. The High School Journal, 98(1), 64-82. doi:10.1353/hsj.2014.0014

Hines, C. B. (2004). Time of day effects on human performance. Catholic Education: A Journal

of Inquiry and Practice, 7(3), 390-413. doi:10.15365/joce.0703072013

Holloway, J. (1999). Giving our students the time of day. Personalized Learning, Educational

Leadership, 57(1), 87-89. Retrieved from http://www.ascd.org/publications/educational-

leadership/sept99/vol57/num01/-Giving-Our-Students-the-Time-of-Day.aspx

83

Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-

eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4(2),

97-110. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1027738

Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1977). Individual differences in human circadian rhythms.

Biological Psychology, 5, 179-190. Retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net

/profile/Olov_Ostberg/publication/298214503_H_O_1977_Biol_Psy_M-Es/links

/56e7172508ae438aab87f6e2/H-O-1977-Biol-Psy-M-Es.pdf

Intons-Peterson, M. J., Rocchi, P., West, T, McLellan, K., & Hackney, A. (1998). Aging, optimal

testing time, and negative priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 362-376. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.362

Kim, S., Dueker, G., Hasher, L., & Goldstein, D. (2002). Children's time of day preference: Age,

gender and ethnic differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1083-1090.

doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00214-8

Klein, J. (2001). Attention, scholastic achievement and timing of lessons. Scandinavian Journal

of Educational Research, 45(3), 301-309. doi:10.1080/00313830120074224

Loe, I. M. & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with

ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(6), 643-654. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsl054

May, C. P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: The costs and a benefit. Psychonomic

Bulletin & Review, 6(1), 142-147. doi:10.3758/BF03210822

McNeil, L. M. (2000). Contradictions of school reform:Educational costs of standardized

testing. New York: Routledge.

84

Oginska, H. & Oginska-Bruschal, K. (2014). Chronotype and personality factors of

predisposition to seasonal affective disorder. Chronobiology International, Early Online,

1-9. doi:10.3109/07420528.2013.874355

Olusegun, S. (2015). Constructivism learning theory: A paradigm for teaching and learning.

IOSR Journal of Research & Method in Education, 5(6), 66-70. doi:10.9790/7388-

05616670

Onder, I., Besoluk, S., Iskender, M., Masal, E., & Demirhan, E. (2014). Circadian preferences,

sleep quality and sleep patterns, personality, academic motivation and academic

achievement of university students. Learning and Individual Differences, 32, 184-192.

doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2014.02.003

Onder, I., Horzum, M. B., & Besoluk, S. (2012). Chronotype, learning approach, type/time of

instruction and academic achievement of the university students. In L. Golovkin, A.

Maliszkewicz (Eds.), Circadian rhythms : Biology, cognition and disorders (pp. 161-

186). New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Retrieved from

https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&AuthType=ip,sso&db=nlebk&AN

=550427&site=eds-live

Preckel, F., Lipnevich, A. A., Boehme, K., Brandner, L., Georgi, K., Konen, T…Roberts, R. D.

(2013). Morningness-eveningness and educational outcomes: The lark has an advantage

over the owl at high school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 114-134.

doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02059.x

Punja, S., Zorzela, L., Hartling, L., Urichuk, L., & Vohra, S. (2013). Long-acting versus short-

acting methylphenidate for pediatric ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis of

comparative efficacy. BMJ Open, 2013(3). doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-002312

85

Randler, C. (2011). Association between morningness-eveningness and mental and physical

health in adolescents. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 16(1), 29-38.

doi:10.1080/13548506.2010.521564

Randler, C., Bechtold, K., & Vogel, M. (2016). Chronotype and time of day do not influence

mathematical achievement in standardized tests, but impact on affect: Results from a

field experiment. International Online Journal of Educational Sciences, 8(5), 54-61.

doi:10.15345/iojes.2016.05.006

Randler, C., Rahafar, A., Arbabi, T., & Bretschneider, R. (2014). Affective state of school pupils

during their first lesson of the day: Effect of morningness-eveningness. Mind, Brain, and

Education, 8(4), 214-219. doi:10.1111/mbe.12060

Refinetti, R. (2012). Daily and circadian rhythmicity. Circadian Rhythm Laboratory: Circadian

Rhythms. Retrieved from http://www.circadian.org/biorhyt.html

Roenneberg, T., Kuehnle, T., Juda, M., Kantermann, T., Allebrandt, K., Gordijn, M., & Merrow,

M. (2007). Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 11(6),

429-438. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.005

Roenneberg, T., Wirz-Justice, A., & Merrow, M. (2003). Life between clocks: Daily temporal

patterns of human chronotypes. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 18(1), 80-90.

doi:10.1177/0748730402239679

Saeed, S., & Zyngier, D. (2012). How motivation influences student engagement: A qualitative

case study. Journal of Education and Learning, 1(2), 252-267. doi:10.5539/jel.v1n2p252

Serra-Pinheiro, M. A., Mattos, P., Regalla, M. A., Souza, I., Paixão, C. (2008). Inattention,

hyperactivity, oppositional-defiant symptoms and school failure. Arq Neuropsiquiatria.

66(4), 828-31. doi:10.1590/S0004-282X2008000600010

86

Sroufe, L. A. (2012, January 28). Ritalin gone wrong. The New York Times. Retrieved from

http://www.nytimes.com

Steenbergen-Hu, S., Makel, M. C., & Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (2016). What one hundred years of

research says about the effects of ability grouping and acceleration on k-12 students’

academic achievement: Findings of two second-order meta-analyses. Review of

Educational Research, 86(4), 849-899. doi:10.3102/0034654316675417

Tennessee Department of Education. (n.d.). TN Ready. Retrieved from https://www.tn.gov

/education/assessment/tnready.html

Toshalis, E., & Nakkula, M. J. (2012). Motivation, engagement, and student voice. Jobs for the

Future. Retrieved from https://www.howyouthlearn.org/pdf/Motivation%20Engagement

%20Student%20Voice_0.pdf

Urry, L. A., Cain, M., Wasserman, S., Minorsky, P., & Reece, J. B. (2018). Campbell biology:

AP edition. Pearson Education, Inc.

Vinne, V., Zerbini, G., Siersema, A., Pieper, A., Merrow, M., Hut, R. A…Kantermann, T.

(2015). Timing of examinations affects school performance differently in early and late

chronotypes. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 30(1), 53-60.

doi:10.1177/0748730414564786

Vollmer, C., Potsch, F., & Randler, C. (2013). Morningness is associated with better gradings

and higher attention in class. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 167-173.

doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.09.001

Webster-Stratton, C. (1993). Strategies for helping early school-aged children with oppositional

defiant and conduct disorders: The importance of home-school partnerships. School

Psychology Review, 22(3), 437-457. doi:10.1080/02796015.1993.12085665

87

Wile, A. J., & Shouppe, G. A. (2011). Does time-of-day of instruction impact class

achievement?. Perspectives In Learning, 12(1), 21-25. Retrieved from

https://csuepress.columbusstate.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1048&context=pil

Witkowski, S., Trujillo, L., Sherman, S., Carter, P., Matthews, M., & Schnyer, D. (2014). An

examination of the association between chronic sleep restriction and electrocortical

arousal in college students. Clinical Neurophysiology. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2014.06.026

Zavada, A., Gordijn, M. C. M., Beersma, D. G. M., Daan, S., & Roenneberg, T. (2005).

Comparison of the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire with the Horne-Östberg’s

Morningness-Eveningness Score. Chronobiology International, 22(2), 267-278.

doi:10.1081/CBI-200053536

Zepke, N., & Leach, L. (2010). Improving student engagement: Ten proposals for action. Active

Learning in Higher Education, 11(3), 167-177. doi:10.1177/1469787410379680

88

Appendix A:

Participant Informed Consent Form

89

Participant Informed Consent Form

Title of Study: Evaluating Time of Day Influence on Achievement and Engagement for High

School Students: Investigating Effective Instructional Strategies

Principal Investigator: Rachel Brouillette

Carson-Newman University

Email: [email protected]

You are being asked to take part in a research study. Before you decide to participate in this

study, it is important that you understand why the research is being done and what it will

involve. Please read the following information carefully. Please ask the researcher if there is

anything that is not clear or if you need more information.

Information and Purpose: The study for which you are being asked to participate is a part of

dissertation research focused on investigating differences in student engagement and

achievement at different times of day in a classroom setting. The purpose of this study is to gain

a better understanding of this phenomenon to determine best instructional practices for

promoting student engagement and achievement at any time of day.

Your Participation in Study Procedures: Your participation in this study will consist of two

classroom observations, an individual interview, and a focus group. Your participation is

strictly voluntary. There is no penalty for discontinuing participation.

Participants will complete:

1. Two classroom observations on the same day (1st or 2nd block and 4th block)

2. Individual interview

3. Focus group

The study will begin in January 2020 and will be completed by April 1, 2020. Audio and video

recording will be used throughout the research process. These recordings will be kept

confidential. Each participant will be given a pseudonym for the duration of the research. All

recorded material will be kept secure and private. You will have the opportunity to review your

responses in the researcher’s notes upon request at any time during the duration of the research.

Benefits and Risks: There will be no direct benefit to you for your participation in this study.

However, the benefit will be gaining insight regarding student performance at different times of

day. This may assist in modifying instruction with best practices for promoting student

engagement, motivation, and achievement at any time of day. There are no known foreseeable

risks associated with participating in the study.

Confidentiality: Your responses during observations and interviews will be anonymous. Every

effort will be made by the researcher to preserve your confidentiality including the following:

• Assigning code names/pseudonyms for participants on all research notes and

documents.

90

• Keeping notes, interview transcriptions, and any other identifying participant

information in a locked file cabinet in the personal possession of the researcher.

Participant data will be kept confidential except in cases where the researcher is legally obligated

to report specific incidents. The researcher will not share your individual responses with anyone

other than the research supervisor.

If you have any questions or concerns, please contact the researcher at [email protected], or

her supervisor, Dr. Steve Davidson at [email protected].

Subject’s Understanding

• I agree to participate in this study that I understand will be submitted in partial

fulfillment of the requirements for the EdD degree in Curriculum and Instruction at

Carson-Newman University.

• I understand that my participation is voluntary.

• I understand that all data collected will be limited to this use or other research-related

usage as authorized by the Carson-Newman University.

• I understand that I will not be identified by name in the final product.

• I am aware that all records will be kept confidential in the secure possession of the

researcher.

• I acknowledge that the contact information of the researcher and her advisor have

been made available to me along with a duplicate copy of this consent form.

• I understand the data I provide will not be used to evaluate my performance in my

classes.

• I understand that I may withdraw from the study at any time with no adverse

repercussions.

By signing below, I acknowledge that I have read and understand the above information. I am

aware that I can discontinue my participation in the study at any time.

Signature__________________________________________________________

Date_______________

91

Appendix B:

Video Release Form for Students

92

Dear Parent/Guardian,

I am a teacher at Science Hill High School and currently pursuing my Ed.D in Curriculum and

Instruction at Carson Newman University. I am working on my dissertation research as a

requirement to receive my doctoral degree.

The purpose of my research is to gain insight regarding differences in student engagement and

achievement at different times of day. The goal of the study is to discover or create instructional

strategies to best promote student engagement at all times of day to maximize student

achievement.

To collect data, I will need to conduct observations of students in class at different times of day

to observe differences in alertness, engagement, and overall participation in lessons. As a

classroom teacher myself, I cannot leave my class to conduct the observations. Instead, I will

video record the lessons and watch later to collect data observations.

The focus of the video will not be on individual students in the class. I am looking for general

signs of alertness, engagement and participation from the class. No student names or any other

personal information will appear on any materials in my dissertation. The video recordings will

be kept confidential and will not be viewed by anyone other than myself. The form on the back

of this letter will be used to document your permission for your child to be video recorded for the

purpose of my dissertation research.

Participation in the study is voluntary, and refusal to participate will involve no penalty. If you

have any questions, you may contact the researcher at [email protected] or her

dissertation supervisor, Dr. Steve Davidson, at [email protected].

Sincerely,

Rachel Brouillette

93

Student Video Release Form To be completed by the parent/legal guardians of minor students involved in this project, and the students.

Student Name______________________________

I am the parent/legal guardian of the child named above. I have read and understand the project

description given in the letter provided with this form, and agree to the following:

_____ I DO give permission to include my child in video recordings as part of observations to

investigate student engagement and participation for the purpose of the researcher’s dissertation.

I understand that my child’s name and any other personally identifiable information about my

child will not appear on any of the submitted materials. There are no known foreseeable risks for

your child to participate in the video observations.

_____ I DO NOT give permission to include my child in video recordings as part of observations

to investigate student engagement and participation for the purpose of the researcher’s

dissertation.

Signature of Parent/Guardian _____________________________ Date: ______________

I am the student named above. I have read and understand the project description given in the

letter provided with this form, and agree to the following:

_____ I DO give permission to be included in video recordings as part of observations to

investigate student engagement and participation for the purpose of the researcher’s dissertation.

I understand that my name and any other personally identifiable information about me will not

appear on any of the submitted materials. There are no known foreseeable risks for participating

in the video observations.

_____ I DO NOT give permission to be included in video recordings as part of observations to

investigate student engagement and participation for the purpose of the researcher’s dissertation.

Signature of Student: ________________________________ Date: _______________

94

Appendix C:

Participant Interview Questions

95

These first two questions should be answered as they relate to student engagement:

1. How do you define student engagement?

2. Describe any differences in student engagement you have observed between morning and

afternoon classes.

These next two questions should be answered as they relate to student achievement:

3. How do you define student achievement?

4. Describe any differences in student achievement you have observed between morning

and afternoon classes.

5. What are your thoughts about the relationship between student engagement and academic

achievement?

These next three questions should be answered as they relate to student behavior:

6. Describe any differences in student behavior you have observed between morning and

afternoon classes.

7. How does student behavior relate to student engagement?

8. How might student behavior affect achievement in the classroom?

These next two questions should be answered as they relate to instruction:

9. How could you modify instruction between morning and afternoon classes to

accommodate any differences in engagement or achievement?

10. Describe some instructional strategies you have found effective in promoting optimal

student achievement.

a. How do these instructional strategies affect student engagement?

Final Question:

11. Are there any additional comments you would like to add about differences in student

engagement, achievement, or behavior at different times of day?