Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of...

9
Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries María Azucena Vicente-Molina * , Ana Fernández-Sáinz, Julen Izagirre-Olaizola University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), Avenida Lehendakari Agirre, 83, E48015 Bilbao, Spain article info Article history: Received 20 July 2012 Received in revised form 27 April 2013 Accepted 2 May 2013 Available online 20 May 2013 JEL classications: Q57 R58 Keywords: Environmental knowledge University students Pro-environmental behaviour Cross-country comparison abstract This paper analyses the inuence of environmental knowledge on pro-environmental behaviour among university students from countries with different levels of economic development (USA, Spain, Mexico and Brazil). The explanatory variables include formal and informal education sources, gender, motiva- tions, attitudes and perceived effectiveness of pro-environmental behaviour. Differences are found be- tween students from emerging and developed countries which suggest that external factors (culture, environmental structures and services in each country) might play a relevant role in university studentsbehaviour towards the environment. A multinomial ordered logit model is applied to estimate the in- uence of the covariates on the environmental performance probability. The results also suggest that motivation and perceived effectiveness are not only signicant variables in both groups but also the most important ones in explaining pro-environmental behaviour. While knowledge (objective and subjective) inuences pro-environmental behaviour, attitude and informal education are not relevant variables. Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Education is one of the most important variables in explaining high levels of environmental concern and behaviour (Zilahy and Huisingh, 2009; Zsóka et al., 2012). Researchers suggest that more highly educated individuals are more concerned about environmental quality and are more motivated to engage in envi- ronmentally responsible behaviour since they are better aware of the potential damage (Lozano, 2006; Olli et al., 2001). More highly educated individuals seem to possess a higher level of environ- mental knowledge, which is translated into pro-environmental behaviour (Schlegelmilch et al., 1996). Although education and environmental knowledge seem to be signicantly and directly related, it is not clear what sort of rela- tionship exists between them or how it affects pro-environmental behaviour (Zsóka et al., 2012). General knowledge and even specic skills related to environmental issues are often acquired through the education system (García-Valiñas et al., 2010). Therefore, un- derstanding how education impacts on environmental knowledge and thus on the development of pro-environmental behaviour is an important issue for policy makers, marketers, green businesses, educators and other parties interested in the acceptance and enhancement of pro-environmental behaviour. Younger generations will be affected by environmental prob- lems arising from present actions, so they need to be provided with accurate environmental knowledge and skills to develop sustain- able solutions (Adomssent et al., 2007; Bradley et al., 1999; Oguz et al., 2010). Thus, higher education has a pivotal role in impelling pro-environmental behaviour and solutions, since it aims to raise responsible, competent individuals with knowledge, skills and values that will contribute to an environmentally sustainable, improving world (Adomssent, 2013; Corcoran and Wals, 2004; Lozano et al., 2013). In recent years, many universities have become engaged in sustainable development, although the trend has not yet fully permeated to all disciplines, academics and university leaders (Lozano et al., 2013). Considering that universities actually train people to perform important social roles effectively (Frank and Meyer, 2007), this paper focuses on university students since they * Corresponding author. Tel.: þ34 946013867. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M.A. Vicente-Molina), [email protected] (A. Fernández-Sáinz), [email protected] (J. Izagirre- Olaizola). Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Cleaner Production journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jclepro 0959-6526/$ e see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2013.05.015 Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138

Transcript of Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of...

Page 1: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138

Contents lists available

Journal of Cleaner Production

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate/ jc lepro

Environmental knowledge and other variables affectingpro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university studentsfrom emerging and advanced countries

María Azucena Vicente-Molina*, Ana Fernández-Sáinz, Julen Izagirre-OlaizolaUniversity of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), Avenida Lehendakari Agirre, 83, E48015 Bilbao, Spain

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 20 July 2012Received in revised form27 April 2013Accepted 2 May 2013Available online 20 May 2013

JEL classifications:Q57R58

Keywords:Environmental knowledgeUniversity studentsPro-environmental behaviourCross-country comparison

* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ34 946013867.E-mail addresses: [email protected]

[email protected] (A. Fernández-Sáinz), julen.iOlaizola).

0959-6526/$ e see front matter � 2013 Elsevier Ltd.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2013.05.015

a b s t r a c t

This paper analyses the influence of environmental knowledge on pro-environmental behaviour amonguniversity students from countries with different levels of economic development (USA, Spain, Mexicoand Brazil). The explanatory variables include formal and informal education sources, gender, motiva-tions, attitudes and perceived effectiveness of pro-environmental behaviour. Differences are found be-tween students from emerging and developed countries which suggest that external factors (culture,environmental structures and services in each country) might play a relevant role in university students’behaviour towards the environment. A multinomial ordered logit model is applied to estimate the in-fluence of the covariates on the environmental performance probability. The results also suggest thatmotivation and perceived effectiveness are not only significant variables in both groups but also the mostimportant ones in explaining pro-environmental behaviour. While knowledge (objective and subjective)influences pro-environmental behaviour, attitude and informal education are not relevant variables.

� 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Education is one of the most important variables in explaininghigh levels of environmental concern and behaviour (Zilahy andHuisingh, 2009; Zsóka et al., 2012). Researchers suggest thatmore highly educated individuals are more concerned aboutenvironmental quality and are more motivated to engage in envi-ronmentally responsible behaviour since they are better aware ofthe potential damage (Lozano, 2006; Olli et al., 2001). More highlyeducated individuals seem to possess a higher level of environ-mental knowledge, which is translated into pro-environmentalbehaviour (Schlegelmilch et al., 1996).

Although education and environmental knowledge seem to besignificantly and directly related, it is not clear what sort of rela-tionship exists between them or how it affects pro-environmentalbehaviour (Zsóka et al., 2012). General knowledge and even specificskills related to environmental issues are often acquired through

(M.A. Vicente-Molina),[email protected] (J. Izagirre-

All rights reserved.

the education system (García-Valiñas et al., 2010). Therefore, un-derstanding how education impacts on environmental knowledgeand thus on the development of pro-environmental behaviour is animportant issue for policy makers, marketers, green businesses,educators and other parties interested in the acceptance andenhancement of pro-environmental behaviour.

Younger generations will be affected by environmental prob-lems arising from present actions, so they need to be provided withaccurate environmental knowledge and skills to develop sustain-able solutions (Adomssent et al., 2007; Bradley et al., 1999; Oguzet al., 2010). Thus, higher education has a pivotal role in impellingpro-environmental behaviour and solutions, since it aims to raiseresponsible, competent individuals with knowledge, skills andvalues that will contribute to an environmentally sustainable,improving world (Adomssent, 2013; Corcoran and Wals, 2004;Lozano et al., 2013).

In recent years, many universities have become engaged insustainable development, although the trend has not yet fullypermeated to all disciplines, academics and university leaders(Lozano et al., 2013). Considering that universities actually trainpeople to perform important social roles effectively (Frank andMeyer, 2007), this paper focuses on university students since they

Page 2: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138 131

are the leaders, policy makers, scientists, consumers, researchers,and entrepreneurs of the future, and as such the future decision-makers1 in different areas (political, social, environmental, eco-nomic, etc.) (Lozano, 2006; Waas et al., 2010; Zilahy and Huisingh,2009). If future talent is able tomake decisions that are beneficial tothe environment, society is more likely to make progress along thepath towards sustainability. In this sense, the level of environ-mental knowledge and the role of environmental education inchanging and addressing lifestyles and attitudes could be crucial inaltering individuals’ behaviour and in turning society towardssustainability (Adomssent, 2013; Bradley et al., 1999; Szerényi et al.,2009). In that change, universities must understand and satisfy theneeds of present and future generations, establishing themselves asleaders of change towards sustainability (Lozano et al., 2013). It isalso essential to find out whether future leaders are ready to directtheir country towards environmentally sustainable development orwhether educational and political institutions need to introducechanges to promote higher levels of environmental knowledge andbehaviour at universities.

Although there aremany studies focused on school students andenvironmental topics (Asmuni et al., 2012; Bradley et al., 1999),there are very few that analyse university students as a group per se.Some of these studies focus on identifying the influence of a fewvariables on the behavioural intentions of business students(Cordano et al., 2010), the environmental beliefs of studentsmajoring in different areas (Ewert and Baker, 2001), the level ofgeneral environmental concern of students (Oguz et al., 2010) orsimply use university students to define a general consumer profile(Synodinos, 1990). However, we are unaware of any study focusedon the environmental knowledge of university students and itspossible links to a set of environmental behaviour variables (recy-cling of different types of waste, public transport use and greenpurchasing). Moreover, much of the research on environmentalbehaviour originates from the United States (Cordano et al., 2010;Diamantopoulos et al., 2003). Although there has recently beensome expansion across different countries, little is known aboutpro-environmental behaviour outside the United States (Cordanoet al., 2010) and even less about university students and possibledifferences between those in emerging and advanced countries(International Monetary Fund, 2012).

The aim of this paper is to identify the main factors that influ-ence the pro-environmental behaviour of university students fromtwo emerging countries, Mexico and Brazil, and two advancedcountries, the United States and Spain (International MonetaryFund, 2012) in order to propose a multinomial ordered logitmodel to define students’ pro-environmental behaviour. Thismodel will also enable measurements to be taken of how a relativeincrease/reduction in a significant variable might affect the pro-environmental behaviour of university students from these coun-tries. This main aim can be divided into three objectives:

First, given that people with more education are more likely todevelop environmental performance, this paper explores factorsrelated to education level (knowledge of the environment) thatmay influence pro-environmental behaviour. In this regard, special

1 It is assumed that education is the key to knowledge, especially in the currentknowledge society, and knowledge open the door to elite positions. “Virtually allelite occupations globally are certified by the university, and nearly all the world’sstratification systems are legitimated by university-based knowledge” (Frank andMeyer, 2007). Of course, university students are not the only people who can ac-cess to top positions and make decisions for a sustainable world. Nevertheless, theyare more likely to become decision makers and leaders of sustainability, since atuniversity they acquire the technical and specialised knowledge necessary to makeimportant decisions and to carry out legal, social, technological, etc. innovations fora more sustainable world.

attention is paid to different environmental knowledge measures(objective and subjective). The type of studies/degree (science,engineering or social sciences) is also analysed since previous re-sults suggest that there may be differences in pro-environmentalbehaviour due to this factor (Talay et al., 2004). This variable andthe number of subjects on environmental topics are consideredtogether as an indicator of formal education. Other informal sour-ces of knowledge (television, newspapers, family, friends, speci-alised books and journals, etc.), the reliability of that knowledgesource and its influence on pro-environmental behaviour are alsotaken into account in this work. These variables are included in anattempt to determine whether they are predictors of pro-environmental behaviour for this group.

The second objective is to provide evidence about differenttypes of socio-demographic and psychological variables, whichaccording to the relevant literature seem to influence pro-environmental behaviour.

The third objective is to find what similarities and differencesexist between the pro-environmental behaviour of students fromemerging and developed countries.

The rest of the paper is structured as follows: Section 2 startswith a literature review, presenting a summary on research find-ings regarding variables such as environmental knowledge, moti-vations, attitudes, perceived effectiveness and gender, amongothers. Section 3 presents the methodology for obtaining data andexplanatory variables, explains the theoretical model and itseconometric formulation. Section 4 presents the multinomial or-dered logit model and discusses the results produced by the model.The main conclusions for supporting recommendations for uni-versity institutions and educational policy makers are set out inSection 5. Finally, the major limitations of the work are explained inthis section.

2. Background and context

There has been a great deal of research into predictors of pro-environmental behaviour. However, despite the plethora ofstudies on pro-environmental behaviour there is still disagreementregarding how pro-environmental behaviour can be predicted fromattitudes and other variables (Bamberg and Moser, 2007; Kollmussand Agyeman, 2002; Mobley et al., 2010).

This section presents the main results of a literature review onvariables among individuals (knowledge, attitudes, motivationsand perceived effectiveness of pro-environmental behaviour).Gender is also studied because it is considered as potentiallyaffecting both environmental knowledge and behaviour. Othervariables such as type of studies/degree, informal education sour-ces (television, newspapers, family, etc.), price sensitivity and theenvironmental structures and services of each country are alsoreviewed.

2.1. Environmental knowledge

Environmental knowledge can be defined as one’s ability toidentify a number of symbols, concepts and behaviour patternsrelated to environmental protection (Laroche et al., 2001). In theearliest linear regression models, knowledge was defined as asource from which environmental attitudes were formed andbehaviour manifested (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002). It has beendemonstrated that those models were wrong, and it is necessary toconsider variables explaining pro-environmental behaviour forfuture research (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002). Although theo-retical knowledge seems to play a significant role in pro-environmental behaviour, the empirical evidence is not so clear(Kaiser and Fuhrer, 2003; Laroche et al., 2001; Zsóka et al., 2012).

Page 3: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138132

Some studies find no significant relationship between environ-mental knowledge and pro-environmental behaviour (Bartiaux,2008; Laroche et al., 2001; Maloney and Ward, 1973). Otherstudies reveal that a deeper knowledge of environmental issuesand how to solve them increases the likelihood of individual takingactions to protect the environment (Kaiser and Fuhrer, 2003;Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Mobley et al., 2010). People whohave greater knowledge of environmental problems are moreprone to behave in a pro-environmental way, ceteris paribus (Oguzet al., 2010).

A shortage of knowledge or the holding of contradictory infor-mation might limit pro-environmental behaviour. The study con-ducted by Kennedy et al. (2009) in Canada found that more than60% of respondents felt that their pro-environmental behaviourwas often constrained by a perceived lack of knowledge. Otherscholars have suggested that a lack of appropriate knowledge or anexcess of self-perceived knowledge might impel individuals tomake environmentally wrong decisions. Thus, if such individualsare more aware of environment problems and their causes, theywill become more motivated to act towards the environment inmore responsible ways (Barber et al., 2009). It has recently beenassumed that knowledge is a necessary but not sufficient conditionfor pro-environmental behaviour by an individual (Kaiser andFuhrer, 2003; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002) since cultural factorscould act as barriers (Kennedy et al., 2009; Lozano, 2006).

Two main approaches have been used to analyse the environ-mental knowledge of individuals: objective and subjective knowl-edge (Barber et al., 2009).

Objective knowledge (actual knowledge) refers to how much aperson actually knows about a type of product, issue or object.Subjective knowledge (also called perceived knowledge) showshow much a person thinks that he/she knows (Dodd et al., 2005).Most of the scales for measuring (environmental) knowledge havebeen developed solely in accordance with subjective (self-rated)knowledge (Amyx et al., 1994; Schlegelmilch et al., 1996) orobjective (actual) knowledge (Bartiaux, 2008; Bradley et al., 1999;DiEnno and Hilton, 2005; Laroche et al., 2001; Maloney and Ward,1973). This might be the source of the contradictory results found(Laroche et al., 2001; Schlegelmilch et al., 1996).

Martin and Simintiras (1995) measure both objective and sub-jective knowledge of environmental issues. Their study reveals thatthe ability of consumers to answer questions on environmentalissues correctly does not correlate with subjective environmentalknowledge and purchase intention. Additionally, Ellen (1994) findsno significant relationship between either objective or subjectiveknowledge and recycling-based shopping decisions. Hence, it isessential to identify the types of knowledge that encourage envi-ronmental behaviour effectively if the most efficient informationalstrategies are to be promoted in education (Frick et al., 2004).

2.2. Attitudes

Milfont and Duckitt (2010) define environmental attitude as apsychological tendency expressed by evaluating the natural envi-ronment with some degree of favour or disfavour. However otherauthors refer to environmental attitudes as environmental concern(Dunlap and Jones, 2002).

The empirical findings regarding the relationship between at-titudes and environmental behaviour are contradictory. Somestudies have reported a positive relationship between environ-mental attitude and pro-environmental behaviour (Kim andChoi, 2003; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Schlegelmilch et al.,1996; Straughan and Roberts, 1999; Tilikidou, 2007), while othersfind a negative relationship (Cottrell, 2003). Furthermore, otherstudies reveal a weak relationship between attitudes and pro-

environmental behaviour (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Olliet al., 2001). The empirical evidence suggests that attitude aloneis a poor predictor of intentional pro-environmental behaviour(Vermeir and Verbeke, 2006). Some relevant studies conclude thateach behaviour pattern has its own cluster of predictors, pointingout that general attitudes are bad predictors for specific environ-mental behaviour patterns (Balderjahn, 1988; Hines et al., 1986e1987). Maineiri et al. (1997) explain this in terms of four reasons:(1) weak correlations between environmental behaviour patterns(not all patterns are interchangeable); (2) different levels of spec-ificity in the measurement of attitude and behaviour; (3) effects ofother variables; and (4) lack of measure of reliability and validity.

2.3. Motivations

Motivation can be defined as a strong internal stimulus aroundwhich behaviour is organised (Wilkie, 1990). Motivation is there-fore usually understood as a reason for behaviour (Moisander,2007). It is shaped by two components: intensity and direction.Both determine the behaviour that is chosen and why. Stern et al.(1993) claim that the stronger the egoistic orientation of the indi-vidual, the stronger the motivation for pro-environmental behav-iour is. However, this is contradicted by other studies, whichsuggest that pro-environmental behaviour is positively related tounselfish or altruistic values (Hopper and Nielsen, 1991; Thøgersen,2011).

In this regard, a key motivation for consumers is the importancegiven to price. As a result, price-sensitive people can be preventedfrom buying greenproducts (Fotopoulos and Krystallis, 2002; Soyezet al., 2011). The Canadian study by Kennedy et al. (2009) revealsthat more than 48% of respondents feel that lack of money is animportant constraint for green purchasing. Therefore, those in-dividuals that show a high sensitivity to price are expected to beless supportive of pro-environmental purchase behaviour, withprice acting as a barrier.

2.4. Perceived consumer effectiveness (PCE)

Some individuals may believe that their own efforts willsignificantly mitigate environmental problems, whereas othersmay think that their efforts will not make any difference.

PCE was first measured as an element of personality variables topredict environmental concern (Kinnear et al., 1974). ThenStraughan and Roberts (1999) measured it as one of the attitudinalvariables in predicting behaviour and it was found to be a betterpredictor than environmental concern. Thus, attitude and PCE arecurrently modelled as two distinct constructs in environmentalstudies (Ellen et al., 1991).

In general, the findings of empirical research suggest that PCE ispositively related to pro-environmental attitudes (Kim and Choi,2003; Laskova, 2007). Some researchers argue that if an individ-ual believes that an environmental problem can be solved by aspecific activity, then that belief should strongly influence the in-dividual’s willingness to engage in that specific activity but not inanother (Ellen et al., 1991). Therefore, PCE might be useful forpredicting general pro-environmental behaviour.

2.5. Gender

Previous research has examined gender differences in a varietyof environmental and behavioural variables. In the 90’s moststudies found that women participated more in pro-environmentalbehaviour (Davidson and Freudenburg, 1996; Zelezny et al., 2000).Zelezny et al. (2000) study gender differences across 14 countriesand find significant gender differences in environmental attitudes

Page 4: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138 133

and behaviour patterns within countries, with women beingconsistently more pro-environmental than men. However,Davidson and Freudenburg (1996) claim that gender differences inenvironmentalism are not universal. Most authors conclude thatmen have more knowledge about environmental issues thanwomen (Diamantopoulos et al., 2003). These differences may bedue to the different socialisation patterns of boys and girls (Schahnand Holzer, 1990).

2.6. Academic discipline and number of environment-relatedsubjects studied

Sometimes the type of discipline or the environment-relatedsubjects studied might affect an individual’s knowledge of theenvironment and affect the relationship between attitudes, beliefsand behaviour (Guagnano et al., 1995). A study of university stu-dents from 14 different faculties in Ankara (Turkey) concluded thatmost of those who were studying environment-related subjectswere from the social sciences (Talay et al., 2004). Therefore, itmakes sense to analyse the possible influence of social sciencedisciplines on individual pro-environmental behaviour. The num-ber of environment-related subjects is also analysed, since littleattention has been paid to this issue in the relevant literature(Ewert and Baker, 2001).

2.7. Informal education

People can learn more about environmental issues through themedia (watching television or reading newspapers), the Internet orsocial interaction (family, friends).2 The literature on the matterwidely recognises the importance of non-formal education chan-nels in generating environmental responsibility (Chan, 1998). Themedia can thus influence environmental performance (Thamwipatet al., 2012), while the effect of socialisation factors depends on thetype of environmental behaviour that is measured (Chan, 1998).Moreover social norms, which are generated within individualsthrough informal education and the media, seem to influence pro-environmental behaviour through attitudes and behavioural in-tentions (Bamberg and Moser, 2007), so informal environmentaleducation seems to be essential for individuals to acquire envi-ronmentally favourable attitudes (Ballantyne et al., 1998).

2.8. Country-specific factors: access to environmental structuresand cultural differences

The quality and availability of community environmental ser-vices play a significant role in determining individuals’ participa-tion in pro-environmental behaviour (Kennedy et al., 2009). Thus,in areas where there are accessible structures, pro-environmentalbehaviour will arise much more easily than in areas withoutstructural services (Derksen and Gartrell, 1993). For example,people may be motivated to buy green products but if thoseproducts are not offered for sale in an accessible location they willnot actually buy them.Moreover, the lack of easy access to recyclingbins might prevent people from recycling and the absence of publictransport may require them to use more polluting transportation.Diamantopoulos et al. (2003) argue that country-specific factorssuch as availability of green products, environmental legislation or

2 It must be stressed that in this paper informal education is considered as ed-ucation that does not lead directly to a degree. However, the increasingly importantrole that informal sources of knowledge play within formal education cannot beignored. For further discussion about different knowledge types, see Adomssent(2013).

cultural influences might explain differences in the characteristicsof a particular country’s pro-environmental consumers. Accordingto Cordano et al. (2010) pro-environmental behaviour seems to beinfluenced by environmental beliefs, which differ from one cultureto another. Environmental problems are not perceived in the sameway in all countries. In advanced economies the perceived impor-tance of environmental issues has increased in the past few de-cades. In emerging economies the situation is different. Countriessuch as Brazil and Mexico are still undergoing rapid industrialisa-tion and environmental standards (and people’s attitudes) are notable to cope with the pressure that is generated by rapid growth(Wang et al., 2011).

As mentioned above, country specific factors are included in thisstudy by comparing emerging countries to developed ones.

3. Methodology: data collection and econometric modelchoice

Data were collected using a structured questionnaire. Themeasuring procedures established in the relevant literature(Tilikidou, 2007) were followed in developing the scales. FromNovember 2009 to May 2011 the questionnaire was given out atfour publicly-run universities: the University of the Basque Country(Spain), the University of Reno-Nevada (USA), UNAM (Mexico) andSalvador do Bahia University (Brazil). A sample of 2226 studentswas obtained.

The questionnaire was divided into blocks. The first partmeasured objective/subjective knowledge. Objective knowledgewas tested via ten questions on current environmental issues andbehaviour patterns with three possible answers (true/false/noanswer). Subjective environmental knowledgewasmeasured usingtwo single self-report items (before and after answering theobjective knowledge test). The second part of the questionnairecomprised several items intended to measure attitudes, motiva-tions and perceived effectiveness in regard to a variety of topicsrelated to the environment. Respondents were asked to specify on afive-point Likert scale whether they agreed or disagreed with eachone. They were then asked to indicate their two main sources ofinformal environmental information (television, radio, newspa-pers, specialised journals and books, family, membership of groupsand others) and their level of credibility.

In another section, respondents were asked to answer howoftenthey recycled, used public transport for environmental protectionand purchased green products. Variables such as environmentalcourses attended, type of discipline/degree, price sensitivity, etc.,were also included in other parts of the questionnaire. The finalblock included socio-demographic variables such as gender, age,income and employment status, although only gender is analysedhere.

Ordered logit models are a kind of discrete choice models whichhave been used in various economic applications including choiceof vehicle type (Choo and Mokhtarian, 2004), consumer brand(Paap and Franses, 2000) and recycling preferences (Nixon et al.,2009).

These logit models are used to measure the effect of eachexplanatory variable on the probability of specific behaviour choice.Additionally, they can indicate the sensitivity with which thedependent variable responds to changes in the explanatoryvariables.

In these models a set of individuals (i ¼ 1.N) are faced with arange of mutually exclusive alternatives, i.e. they have to make achoice (j) from a given set of options (A). In the ordered logit modelthe ordinal response variable, Yi, is the discrete realisation of anunderlying, unobservable (latent) continuous random variable, Y*

i .The categories are envisaged as contiguous intervals on the

Page 5: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138134

continuous scale. The unobservable Y*i is assumed to satisfy a linear

regression model, i.e. Y*i ¼ b0xi þ εi, where xi is a covariate vector, b

a vector of regression coefficients and εi the error term. Theobservable categorical variable, Yi, is assumed to arise from Y*

i asfollows:

Yi ¼

8><>:

0; if Y*i � 0

1; if 0 < Y*i � m1

2; if m1 < Y*i

where the m1 is an unknown cut-off point (category boundary) inthe distribution of Y*

i . In this paper environmental performance, Yi,is an ordinal variable with three categories: low level (0), mediumlevel (1) and high level (2) behaviour. In principle, the individualscan respond to the questionnaire with their own Y*

i if asked to doso. Given three possible answers, they can choose the option thatmost closely represents their own feelings on the question.Therefore, the different probabilities of the categories are:

PðYi ¼ 0Þ ¼ F��b0xi

�;

PðYi ¼ 1Þ ¼ F�m1 � b0xi

�� F��b0xi

PðYi ¼ 2Þ ¼ 1� F�m1 � b0xi

where F is the cumulative distribution function of the logisticdistribution.

Interpretation of the coefficients in ordered logit models is morecomplicated than in the ordinary regression setting (Daykin andMoffatt, 2002), since there is no natural conditional mean func-tion in the model. The outcome variable, Yi, is merely a label for theordered, non-quantitative outcomes.

As usual, the marginal effects of the regressors, xi, on theprobabilities are not equal to the coefficients. For the probabilisticmodel used, the marginal effects of changes in the regressors are:

vPðYi ¼ 0Þvxim

¼ �f�b0xi

�bm

vPðYi ¼ 1Þvxim

¼ �f�m1 � b0xi

�� f��b0xi

��bm

vPðYi ¼ 2Þvxim

¼ f�m1 � b0xi

�bm

where f is the density function of the logistic distribution.

Table 1Statistics of the explanatory variables: Mean and standard deviation in brackets.

Variable Description All

N Number of observations 2226Gender Dummy, 1 if male 0.4995 (0Objective knowledge In the test, sum of correct answers 4.1801 (1Subjective knowledge pre-test Pre-test subjective knowledge 5.9615 (1Subjective knowledge post-test Post-test subjective knowledge 6.1790 (2Type of degree/formal education Dummy, 1 if science and/or

engineering0.3800 (0

Number of subjects Number of subjects 0.6488 (1Perception of lack of information Factor by factor analysis 3.475 (1Motivation (altruism) Factor by factor analysis 3.1733 (0Attitudes Factor by factor analysis 2.2669 (0Perceived consumer effectiveness

(PCE)Factor by factor analysis 4.005 (0

Price sensitivity Factor by factor analysis 3.5604 (1

Consequently, in the marginal effects neither the sign nor themagnitude of the coefficient is informative about the result above,so direct interpretation of the coefficients is fundamentallyambiguous (Greene and Hensher, 2010). The implication of thesefindings is that the effect of a change in one of the variables in themodel depends on all model parameters, on data and on theprobability (cell) which is of interest.

This paper analyses the highest probability of pro-environmental behaviour.

In this type of model, the only certainties in the signs of thepartial effects are the following:

� If there is a considered variable with a positive coefficient, in-creases in that variable will raise the probability in the highestcell (pro-environmental behaviour) and decrease the proba-bility in the lowest cell (non-environmental behaviour), andvice versa.

� The sum of all the changes will be zero, since the new proba-bilities must still add up to one.

4. Results and discussions

Table 1 shows the mean and standard deviation of the variablesconsidered for the four countries that make up the total sample,and briefly explains how they are measured.

An exploratory factor analysis was conducted with the itemsrelated to perception of the need for more information, motiva-tions, attitudes, consumer perceived effectiveness and pricesensitivity to evaluate the ability of the items to measure eachconstruct. Validity and reliability tests were conducted on eachfactor with satisfactory results, Cronbach alphas varied from 0.4 to0.7 (Cronbach and Shavelson, 2004; Schmitt, 1996).

The results of the exploratory factor analysis reveal that Bra-zilian students show higher levels of altruistic motivation, Spanishstudents have the most favourable attitudes towards the environ-ment and students from these two countries have the highest levelsof PCE. Finally, American college students allocate most importanceto price in their consumer activities.

The results show similar numbers of men and women in thewhole sample (with more women in the case of Spain).

In general, the results reveal that there is little knowledgeof environmental topics, as suggested in previous research(Schlegelmilch et al., 1996; Kollmus and Agyeman, 2002). Brazilianstudents obtain the highest score (5.1 out of 10) in objectiveknowledge, while Mexicans get the lowest (3.65). In all cases,pretest and posttest subjective knowledge is higher than objectiveknowledge. Brazil also stands out for the number of subjects relatedto environmental issues studied by university students.

Spain Brazil Mexico USA

640 347 642 597.500) 0.4203 (0.494) 0.5216 (0.500) 0.5561 (0.497) 0.5109 (0.500).693) 4.2515 (1.740) 5.1008 (1.380) 3.6511 (1.546) 4.1373 (1.726).779) 5.6151 (1.673) 6.8912 (1.272) 6.2121 (1.727) 5.5273 (1.946).157) 5.898 (2.100) 6.7446 (1.829) 6.1403 (2.008) 6.1692 (2.476).485) 0.4625 (0.499) 0.6080 (0.489) 0.2212 (0.415) 0.3299 (0.471)

.358) 0.3744 (0.908) 1.629 (2.276) 0.2898 (0.781) 0.7441 (1.224)

.093) 3.1604 (1.183) 4.1220 (1.087) 3.8122 (1.070) 3.0705 (0.643)

.754) 2.9826 (0.687) 3.5512 (0.760) 3.1494 (0.733) 3.1833 (0.761)

.812) 2.5149 (0.732) 1.9903 (0.837) 2.2983 (0.818) 2.1274 (0.794)

.719) 3.7791 (0.686) 4.1981 (0.678) 4.2456 (0.679) 3.8809 (0.712)

.147) 3.4742 (1.136) 3.1072 (1.197) 3.4319 (1.100) 4.0519 (1.006)

Page 6: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138 135

A predominance of social science students can also be observed,except in the case of Brazil.

Table 2 shows the distribution of the dependent variable (IND),the components that comprise (I1, I2, I3) and the indicators in thedifferent sub-samples. The dependent variable e environmentalperformance e is constructed using three components: recycling,public transport use and green purchasing. All individuals areplaced in one of 3 classes: low (0), medium (1) or high (2) level foreach component. Depending on these values, individuals areconsidered to have a low (0), medium (1) or high (2) level of pro-environmental behaviour.

Students in the Spanish subsample recycle most, while Mexi-cans are the most likely to use public transport as a way to protectthe environment. USA students top the list in green purchasingbehaviour. In general, Spanish university students have the mostadvanced environmental performance, followed by Mexican stu-dents, with US and Brazilians showing lower levels. This suggeststhat structural and cultural differences across countries might beinfluencing pro-environmental behaviour. It is clear that the lack ofeasy access to recycling bins might prevent people from recyclingand the absence of public transport may require them to use morepolluting transportation. The behaviour patterns analysed (recy-cling, public transport use and green purchasing) seem to beinfluenced by other factors (cultural and/or infrastructural) whichare different in each considered country. This suggests that struc-tural and cultural differences across countries might be influencingpro-environmental behaviour, which are different in each country(Diamantopoulos et al., 2003; Cordano et al., 2010). Environmentalproblems are not perceived in the same way in all countries.

4.1. Pro-environmental behaviour ordered logit model

Table 3 shows the results of the model estimates (showingpositive or negative direction and the effect of each explanatoryvariable on the dependent variable) and the marginal effects. Thefour countries are grouped in pairs in line with the IMF classifica-tion (2012) and the objectives of this study, taking into accountadvanced economies (USA and Spain) and emerging countries(Brazil and Mexico).

Although informal education was one of the variables to beanalysed in the logit model, it seemed to provide little relevantinformation and is therefore finally not included.

The results show that most of the variables included in themodel have a significant effect on the dependent variable, and theeffects are generally consistent with findings reported in previousresearch. Nevertheless, there are some significant differences be-tween the two subsamples:

Table 2Distribution and composition of dependent variables.

Variable Description All Spain Brazil Mexico USA

Dependent variable:IND

Low 0.1181 0.0469 0.2017 0.0841 0.1826Medium 0.6986 0.6859 0.6513 0.7352 0.7002High 0.1833 0.2672 0.1470 0.1807 0.1173Mean 1.0651 1.2203 0.9452 1.0966 0.9347

I1: Recycling Low 0.2421 0.0594 0.4986 0.3178 0.2077Medium 0.4003 0.1688 0.3775 0.5218 0.5310High 0.3576 0.7719 0.1239 0.1604 0.2613Mean 1.1154 1.7125 0.6253 0.8427 1.0536

I2: Public transportuse

Low 0.2727 0.1234 0.2738 0.1044 0.6131Medium 0.2659 0.3219 0.1787 0.1978 0.3300High 0.4614 0.5547 0.5476 0.6978 0.0570Mean 1.1887 1.4312 1.2738 1.5935 0.4439

I3: Green purchasing Low 0.3935 0.5891 0.3919 0.4050 0.1725Medium 0.2947 0.2188 0.3084 0.3660 0.2915High 0.3118 0.1922 0.2997 0.2290 0.5360Mean 0.9182 0.6031 0.9078 0.8239 1.3635

1) In the case of theUnited States and Spain, a relative increase inobjective knowledge (correct answers in the test) increases theprobability3 of high environmental performance. The sameapplies to pretest subjective knowledge, perceived lack of in-formation, motivation and PCE. On the other hand, pricesensitivity is a major barrier to pro-environmental behaviour.

Surprisingly, an increase in the number of study subjects con-cerned with environmental issues slightly reduces the probabilityof high environmental performance. Science and engineering stu-dents are more likely to behave pro-environmentally than socialscience students. The results also reveal that being male decreasesthe likelihood of high environmental behaviour.

2) In the Brazil andMexico subsample, the direction of the effectsof pretest subjective knowledge, motivation and PCE are alsopositive. However, perceived lack of information, type of de-gree and number of subjects studied are no longer relevant,while the effect of objective knowledge, attitudes, gender andsensitivity to price become negative. This last finding might bedue to the fact that the subsample results from joining twoemerging countries, and thus covers very different objectiveknowledge levels. By contrast, post-test subjective knowledgehas a significant positive effect, so an increase in the responsesthat an individual believes that he/she has got right increasesthe likelihood of high environmental behaviour.

On the other hand, the model estimate reveals that PCE andmotivation show the highest partial effects (0.05432/0.03541 and0.09818/0.07948 respectively) for the most pro-environmentalbehaviour in both subsamples. This suggests that the most impor-tant effects in all the countries considered are motivation and PCE.

5. Conclusions, recommendations and limitations

While it would be desirable to be able to demonstrate a clear,coherent causal link between education and pro-environmentalbehaviour, such a link, as previous literature suggest (Zsóka et al.,2012), is difficult to be established reliably.

Formal education and knowledge of environmental issuesclearly influence pro-environmental behaviour, but they do so in acomplex way. Objective and subjective knowledge influence theenvironmental performance of students from both advanced andemerging countries, while the type of degree impacts only in thecase of advanced countries.

Pretest subjective knowledge is the most relevant of all theknowledge-related factors analysed, i.e. what college studentsthink they know is more relevant than what they actually know.This implies that students might be making wrong decisions abouttheir environmental performance. The prevalence of this variablemight partially explain the contradictory results found in objectiveknowledge and environment-related subjects studied. In this sense,it may be desirable for students to become familiar with currentenvironmental issues, rather than developing technical environ-mental knowledge.

The results also reveal that psychological and gender variablesare important in explaining pro-environmental behaviour. Thus,motivation and the PCE are the factors that best explain the prob-ability of high environmental performance in all countries. Pro-environmental behaviour is basically linked to altruistic motiva-tions. Overall, it can be said that individuals who perceive lower

3 In this case the probability of medium and low environmental performancedecreases since the sum is constant.

Page 7: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

Table 3Ordered logit model for pro-environmental behaviour.

Estimations Marginal effects

USAeSpain BrazileMexico USAeSpain BrazileMexico

Coefficients eindex function

Coefficients eindex function

Effect(pr ¼ 2)a

Elasticity(pr ¼ 2)

Effect(pr ¼ 2)

Elasticity(pr ¼ 2)

Constant �0.94699**b �0.93899**Objective knowledge (test result) 0.06219** �0.06210** 0.01520 0.40528 -0.01450 �0.40489Pretest subjective knowledge 0.09331*** 0.05660** 0.02280 0.79013 0.01322 0.56789Postest subjective knowledge �0.00649 0.04056* �0.00159 -0.05936 0.00947 0.40033Gender (male) �0.15886** �0.15224* �0.03864 �0.23992 �0.03591 �0.23877Type of Studies (science and engineering) 0.19581** �0.04987 0.04865 0.30206 �0.01157 �0.07691Number of subjects �0.07478** 0.03096 �0.01827 �0.06680 0.00723 0.03613Perceived lack of information 0.08256* 0.06578 0.02017 0.39130 0.01536 0.40113Motivation (altruistic) 0.40179*** 0.34028*** 0.09818 1.87812 0.07948 1.73030Attitude 0.02804 0.04532 0.00685 0.09860 0.01059 0.15304Perceived effectiveness 0.22230*** 0.15161** 0.05432 1.28998 0.03541 0.99494Price sensitivity �0.19825*** �0.07310* -0.04845 �1.13576 -0.01707 �0.37754

Notes:a pr ¼ 2 is the probability of the highest pro-environmental behaviour.b *** e Significant at P ¼ 0.01, ** e Significant at P ¼ 0.05, * e Significant at P ¼ 0.10.

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138136

costs (selfish point of view) and higher profits (the perception thatpositive results can be achieved for society) are more likely todevelop high environmental performance.

In addition, the results confirm that gender influences envi-ronmental behaviour. Women are more likely to carry out envi-ronmentally friendly activities in both advanced and emergingcountries.

Attitude is not a significant variable in explaining pro-environmental behaviour. This is not surprising, since empiricalevidence states that attitude is not a good predictor of pro-environmental behaviour (Vermeir and Verbeke, 2006). Never-theless, the contradictory results found in the relationship betweenattitudes and pro-environmental behaviour suggest that deeperresearch is needed. In fact, the way in which attitude (“environ-mental concern”) and motivation (“internal stimulus”) have beendefined in earlier research and indeed here, could be partlyresponsible for those contradictory results, since these factors seemnot to be clearly defined. In this respect, Kollmuss and Agyeman(2002) argue “that this difficulty in delimiting the different fac-tors is due to the fact that most are broadly and vaguely defined,interrelated, and often do not have clear boundaries”.

The estimated ordered logit models for both emerging andadvanced countries show differences but also similarities. Thefactors with the biggest weight in terms of environmental perfor-mance are psychological variables such as motivation, PCE and theimportance of price as a motivational factor for engaging in greenpurchasing. Knowledge-related factors are also relevant, andgender must also be taken into account in explaining pro-environmental behaviour.

The main conclusion in regard to differences between emergingand developed countries is that this economic-development-related classification cannot be applied simplistically to environ-mental performance. Moreover, pro-environmental behaviour isanalysed as the sum of three components and the results revealthat different patterns occur in each context. Thus, Spanish stu-dents stand out for their recycling behaviour, Mexican and Spanishstudents show a greater tendency to use public transport and USAstudents show far higher buying behaviour than any of the othersbut are the least likely to use public transport. These patterns areprobably due to the different cultural habits and structures thatexist in each country.

Recommendations for companies must be linked to improvingthe perception of the cost-benefit link for pro-environmental

behaviour. Firms intending to develop green marketing activitiesshould strive to get their potential consumers to perceive specificbenefits arising from this kind of behaviour. Additionally, includingor increasing the supply of green products in conventional retailing(hypermarkets and supermarkets) to facilitate environmentally-friendly purchases might be an interesting option in those coun-tries where such purchases are lower, e.g. Spain and Brazil.

Data show that students with greener behaviour have altruisticmotivations in going green. Nevertheless, as Rex and Baumann(2007) argue, to achieve greener production and consumptionpatterns it is necessary to address a wider range of consumers, soarguments from conventional marketing must not be forgotten.

In regard to educational institutions, the main challenge is toincrease both objective and subjective environmental knowledgelevels. As noted above, the subsequent relationship betweenknowledge and behaviour is complex, so the integration of sus-tainable development into higher education must consider andaddress that complexity. Nevertheless, while increasing objectiveknowledge might be a way for higher education institutions toimprove pro-environmental behaviour in advanced countries, thismeasure does not seem to be worth applying in emerging coun-tries. Additionally, educational institutions and governmentsshould attempt to reduce the gap between objective and subjectiveknowledge of environmental issues so that their future leadersbecome aware of their actual cognitive capacity to tackle environ-mental issues effectively.

Finally, researchers should consider in their investigations thatdifferent patterns of environmental behaviour might be explainedby structural and cultural differences across countries. Therefore,the scales constructed previously to define the influence of envi-ronmental factors in pro-environmental behaviour are likely tohave to be adapted to the research objectives, and also to the spe-cific cultural and geographical context of the research.

There are several limitations that should be considered beforethese results are generalised. First, the subsamples were taken at asingle university in each of the four countries studied. This impliesthat they might not reflect different situations that may exist ineach country, so the results cannot be generalised to these coun-tries as a whole.

Second, although the sample comes from four different coun-tries, generalisation to other cultures may be problematic andmorefuture research needs to be done so that the conclusions can beapplied to other countries with different socio-economic

Page 8: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138 137

characteristics. However, this analysis may be sufficient for thepurpose of looking for differences between university students incountries with different levels of economic and social development.

Moreover, the dependent variable e environmental perfor-mance e is measured through three different components: recy-cling, public transport use and green purchasing. Several studiesreported in this paper indicate that the effect of explanatory vari-ables might vary depending on the issue analysed. Thus, it might beinteresting to supplement this research by including other aspectsof pro-environmental behaviour.

In the model drawn up, the behaviour patterns analysed (recy-cling, public transport use and green purchasing) seem to beinfluenced by cultural and infrastructural factors, which aredifferent in each country. Nevertheless, our results are, in general,consistent with previous research that highlights the significance ofknowledge and formal education, motivations, PCE and gender inpro-environmental behaviour.

In the model, the assumption that students will deliver theirproenvironmental awareness and performance in their future job-posts is underlying. Though it is highly wishful, this is a ratherdubious position considering the limitations (mostly of a financialnature) that executives must consider in their decision makingprocess. But it should be kept in mind that university students arethe consumers, researchers and entrepreneurs of the future, and iffuture talent is able to make decisions that are beneficial to theenvironment, society is more likely to make progress along thepath towards sustainability. In this sense, the level of environ-mental knowledge and the role of environmental education inchanging and addressing lifestyles and attitudes could be crucial inaltering individuals’ behaviour and in turning society towardssustainability.

Moreover, it should be taken into account that the dynamicnature of the inter-relationships among explanatory variables mayproduce a spiral withmutual impacts resulting from changes to anyof the variables. Since this is a cross-sectional study, this fact hasnot been considered. Time evolution dimension may be a futureresearch line using longitudinal data, as has been recommended byVicente-Molina et al. (2012).

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge financial support from CátedraUNESCO (Basque Government, grant 08/25), the FESIDE Foundation(2009, 2010 and 2011) and the Econometrics Research Group(Basque Government grant IT-IT642-13).

References

Adomssent, M., 2013. Exploring universities’ transformative potential forsustainability-bound learning in changing landscapes of knowledge commu-nication. J. Clean. Prod. 49, 11e24.

Adomssent, M., Godemann, J., Michelsen, G., 2007. Transferability of approaches tosustainable development at universities as a challenge. Int. J. Sust. High. Educ. 8(4), 385e402.

Amyx, D.A., DeJong, P.F., Lin, X., Chakraborty, G., Wiener, J.L., 1994. Influences ofpurchase intentions for ecologically safe products: an exploratory study. In:Park, C.W., Smith, D.C. (Eds.), Marketing Theory and Applications, Proceedingsof the 1994 American Marketing Association Winter Educators Conference.AMA, Chicago, pp. 341e347.

Asmuni, S., Khalili, J.M., Zain, Z.M., 2012. Sustainable consumption practices ofstudents in an urban setting: a case in Selangor. Procedia e Soc. Behav. Sci. 36,716e722.

Balderjahn, I., 1988. Personality variables and environmental attitudes as predictorsof ecologically responsible consumption patterns. J. Bus. Res. 17 (1), 51e56.

Ballantyne, R., Connell, S., Fien, J., 1998. Students as catalysts of environmentalchange: a framework for researching intergenerational influence throughenvironmental education. Environ. Educ. Res. 4, 285e298.

Bamberg, S., Moser, G., 2007. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford and Tomera: anew meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmentalbehaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 14e25.

Barber, N., Taylor, D.C., Strick, S., 2009. Environmental knowledge and attitudes:influencing the purchase decisions of wine consumers. In: Int. CHRIEConference-Refereed Track. Event 16 [2009]. http://scholarworks.umass.edu/refereed/Sessions/Wednesday/16.

Bartiaux, F., 2008. Does environmental information overcome practice compart-mentalisation and change consumers’ behaviours? J. Clean. Prod. 16 (11), 1170e1180.

Bradley, J.C., Waliczek, T.M., Zajicek, J.M., 1999. Relationship between environmentalknowledge and environmental attitude of high school students. J. Environ.Educ. 30, 17e21.

Chan, K., 1998. Mass communication and proenvironmental behaviour: wasterecycling in Hong Kong. J. Environ. Manag. 52, 317e325.

Choo, S., Mokhtarian, L., 2004. What type of vehicle do people drive? the role ofattitude and lifestyle in influencing vehicle type choice. Transp. Res. Part. A:Policy Pract. 38 (3), 201e222.

Corcoran, P.B., Wals, A.E.J., 2004. Higher Education and the Challenge of Sustain-ability: Problematics, Promise and Practice. Kluwer Academic Publishers,Netherlands.

Cordano, M., Welcomer, S., Scherer, R., Pradenas, L., Parada, V., 2010. Understandingcultural differences in the antecedents of pro-environmental behaviour: acomparative analysis of business students in the United States and Chile.J. Environ. Educ. 41 (4), 224e238.

Cottrell, S.P., 2003. Influence of sociodemographic and environmental attitudes ofgeneral responsible environmental behaviour among recreational boaters. En-viron. Behav. 35, 347e375.

Cronbach, L.J., Shavelson, R.J., 2004. My current thoughts on coefficient alpha andsuccessor procedures. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 64, 391e418.

Davidson, D., Freudenburg, W., 1996. Gender and environmental risk concerns: areview and analysis of available research. Environ. Behav. 28, 302e339.

Daykin, A., Moffatt, P., 2002. Analyzing ordered responses: a review of the orderedprobit model. Underst. Stat. 1 (3), 157e166.

Derksen, L., Gartrell, J., 1993. The social context of recycling. Am. Sociol. Rev. 58 (3),434e442.

Diamantopoulos, A., Schlegelmilch, B.B., Sinkovics, R.R., Bohlen, G.M., 2003. Cansocio-demographics still play a role in profiling green consumers? A review ofthe evidence and an empirical investigation. J. Bus. Res. 56, 465e480.

DiEnno, C.M., Hilton, S.C., 2005. High school students’ knowledge, attitudes, andlevels of enjoyment of an environmental education unit on nonnative plants.J. Environ. Educ. 37 (1), 13e25.

Dodd, T.H., Laverie, D.A., Wilcox, J.F., Duhan, D.F., 2005. Differential effects ofexperience, subjective knowledge, and objective knowledge on sources of in-formation used in consumer wine purchasing. J. Hosp. Tour. Res. 29 (1), 3e19.

Dunlap, R.E., Jones, R.E., 2002. Environmental concern: conceptual and measure-ment issues. In: Dunlap, R.E., Michelson, W. (Eds.), Handbook of EnvironmentalSociology. Greenwood Press, Westport, CT, pp. 482e524.

Ellen, P.S., 1994. Do we know what we need to know? Objective and subjectiveknowledge effects on pro-ecological behaviors. J. Bus. Res. 10 (1), 43e52.

Ellen, P.M., Wiener, J.L., Cobb-Walgren, C., 1991. The role of perceived consumereffectiveness in motivating environmentally conscious behaviours. J. PublicPolicy Mark 10, 102e117.

Ewert, A., Baker, D., 2001. Standing for where you sit: and exploratory analysis ofthe relationship between academic major and environment beliefs. Environ.Behav. 33, 687e707.

Fotopoulos, C., Krystallis, A., 2002. Organic product avoidance. Reasons for rejectionand potential buyers’ identification in a countrywide survey. Br. Food J. 104 (3,4, 5), 233e260.

Frank, D.J., Meyer, J.W., 2007. University expansion and the knowledge society.Theor. Soc. 36, 287e311.

Frick, J., Kaiser, F.G., Wilson, M., 2004. Environmental knowledge and conservationbehaviour: exploring prevalence and structure in a representative sample. Pers.Individ. Differ. 37, 1597e1613.

García-Valiñas, M.A., Muñiz-Pérez, M.A., Cordero-Ferrera, J.M., 2010. The role ofschools in providing environmental knowledge in science. In: Mancebón-Torrubia, M.J., Xímenez-de-Embún, D.P., Gómez-Sancho, J.M., Giménez-Esteban, G. (Eds.), 2010. Investigaciones de Economía de la Educación, vol. 5 (5).Asoc. de Econ. de la Educ, pp. 87e100.

Greene, W.H., Hensher, D.A., 2010. Modelling Ordered Choices. Cambridge Univer-sity Press, Cambridge.

Guagnano, G., Stern, P., Dietz, T., 1995. Influences on attitude-behaviour relation-ships: a natural experiment with curbside recycling programs. Environ. Behav.27, 699e718.

Hines, J.M.,Hungerford,H.R., Tomera,A.N.,1986e1987.Analysisandsynthesisof researchon responsible environmental behaviour: a meta-analysis. J. Environ. Educ.18, 1e8.

Hopper, J.R., Nielsen, J.M., 1991. Recycling as altruistic behavior. Normative andbehavioral strategies to expand participation in a community recycling pro-gram. Environ. Behav. 23 (2), 195e220.

International Monetary Fund, 2012. World Economic Outlook April 2012. Series:World Economic and Financial Surveys. IMF Publication Services, Washington.

Kaiser, F.G., Fuhrer, U., 2003. Ecological behaviour’s dependency on different formsof knowledge. Appl. Psychol. Int. Rev. 52 (4), 598e613.

Kennedy, E.H., Beckley, T.M., McFarlane, B.L., Nadeau, S., 2009. Why we don’t walkthe talk: understanding the environmental values/behaviour gap in Canada.Hum. Ecol. Rev. 16 (2), 151e160.

Kim, Y., Choi, S., 2003. Antecedents of pro-environmental behaviours: an exami-nation of cultural values, self-efficacy, and environmental attitudes. In: Paper

Page 9: Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries

M.A. Vicente-Molina et al. / Journal of Cleaner Production 61 (2013) 130e138138

Presented at the Annual Meeting of the International Communication Associ-ation. online from. http://citation.allacademic.com/meta/p_mla_apa_research_citation/1/1/1/5/2/p111527_index.html.

Kinnear, T.C., Taylor, J.R., Ahmed, S.A., 1974. Ecologically concerned consumers: whoare they? J. Mark 11, 20e24.

Kollmuss, A., Agyeman, J., 2002. Mind the gap: why do people act environmentallyand what are the barriers to pro-environmental behaviour? Environ. Educ. Res.8 (3), 239e260.

Laroche, M., Bergeron, J., Barbaro-Forleo, G., 2001. Targeting consumers who arewilling to pay more for environmentally friendly products. J. Consum. Mark 18(6), 503e520.

Laskova, A., 2007. Perceived consumer effectiveness and environmental concerns.In: Proceedings of the 13th Asia Pacific Management Conference, Melbourne,Australia, pp. 206e209.

Lozano, R., 2006. Incorporation and institutionalisation of SD into universities:breaking through barriers to change. J. Clean. Prod. 14 (9e11), 787e796.

Lozano, R., Lukman, R., Lozano, F.J., Huisingh, D., Lambrechts, W., 2013. Declarationsfor sustainability in higher education: becoming better leaders, throughaddressing the university system. J. Clean. Prod. 48, 10e19.

Mainieri, T., Barnett, E., Valdero, T., Unipan, J., Oskamp, S., 1997. Green buying: theinfluence of environmental concern on consumer behaviour. J. Soc. Psychol. 137(2), 189e204.

Maloney, M.P., Ward, M.P., 1973. Ecology: let’s hear from the people. An objectivescale for the measurement of ecological attitudes and knowledge. Am. Psychol.28, 583e586.

Martin, B., Simintiras, A.C., 1995. The impact of green product lines on the envi-ronment: does what they know affect how they feel? Mark. Intell. Plan. 13 (4),16e23.

Milfont, T.L., Duckitt, J., 2010. The environmental attitudes inventory: a valid andreliable measure to assess the structure of environmental attitudes. J. Environ.Psychol. 30 (1), 80e94.

Mobley, C., Vagias, W.M., DeWard, S.L., 2010. Exploring additional determinants ofenvironmentally responsible behaviour: the influence of environmental liter-ature and environmental attitudes. Environ. Behav. 42 (4), 420e447.

Moisander, J., 2007. Motivational complexity of green consumerism. Int. J. Consum.Stud. 31 (4), 404e409.

Nixon, H., Saphores, J.D.M., Ogunseitan, O.A., Shapiro, A.A., 2009. Understandingpreferences for recycling electronic waste in California: the influence of envi-ronmental attitudes and beliefs on willingness to pay. Environ. Behav. 41, 101e124.

Oguz, D., Çakci, I., Kavas, S., 2010. Environmental awareness of university studentsin Ankara, Turkey. Afr. J. Agric. Res. 5, 2629e2636.

Olli, E., Grendstad, G., Wollebaek, D., 2001. Correlates of environmental behaviours:bringing back social context. Environ. Behav. 33 (2), 181e208.

Paap, R., Franses, P.H., 2000. A dynamic multinomial probit model for brand choicewith different long-run and short-run effects of marketing mix variables.J. Appl. Econom. 15, 717e744.

Rex, E., Baumann, H., 2007. Beyond ecolabels: what green marketing can learn fromconventional marketing. J. Clean. Prod. 15 (6), 567e576.

Schahn, J., Holzer, E., 1990. Studies of individual environmental concern: the role ofknowledge, gender, and background variables. Environ. Behav. 22 (6), 767e786.

Schlegelmilch, B.B., Bohlen, G.M., Diamantopoulos, A., 1996. The link between greenpurchasing decisions and measures of environmental consciousness. Eur. J.Mark 30, 35e55.

Schmitt, N., 1996. Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychol. Assess. 8 (4), 350e353.

Soyez, K., Francis, J., Smirnova, M., 2011. How individual, product and situationaldeterminants affect the intention to buy and organic food buying behavior: across-national comparison in five nations. Der Mark 51 (1), 27e35.

Stern, P.S., Dietz, T., Karlof, L., 1993. Values orientation, gender, and environmentalconcern. Environ. Behav. 25 (3), 322e348.

Straughan, R.D., Roberts, J.A., 1999. Environmental segmentation alternatives: a lookat green consumer behaviour in the new millennium. J. Consum. Mark 16 (6),558e575.

Synodinos, N.E., 1990. Environmental attitudes and knowledge: a comparison ofmarketing and business students with other groups. J. Bus. Res. 20 (2), 161e170.

Szerényi, Z.M., Zsóka, Á., Széchy, A., 8e10 June 2009. Environmental Education andPro-environmental Consumer Behaviour-Results of a University Survey. JointActions on Climate Change, Denmark.

Talay, I., Gündüz, S., Akpinar, N., 2004. On the status of environmental educationand awareness of undergraduate students at Ankara University, Turkey. Inter. J.Environ. Pollut. 21 (3), 293e308.

Thamwipat, K., Maneewan, S., Pumiaroen, T., 2012. Behavior of media exposure andparticipation in environmental activities of king Mongkut’s University Technol-ogy Thonburi dormitory students. World Acad. Sci. Eng. Technol. 61, 247e251.

Thøgersen, J., 2011. Green shopping for selfish reasons or the common good? Am.Behav. Sci. 55 (8), 1052e1076.

Tilikidou, I., 2007. The effects of knowledge and attitudes upon Greeks’ pro-environmental purchasing behaviour. Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Manag.14, 121e134.

Vermeir, I., Verbeke, W., 2006. Sustainable food consumption: exploring the con-sumer “attitude e behavioral intention” gap. J. Agric. Environ. Ethics 19 (2),169e194.

Vicente-Molina, M.A., Tamayo-Orbegozo, U., Izagirre-Olaizola, J., 2012. Revisión dela metodología empleada y resultados alcanzados en la investigación sobreactuación medioambiental y rendimiento económico. J. Quant. Method. Econ.Bus. Adm. 14 (5), 5e35.

Waas, T., Verbruggen, A., Wright, T., 2010. University research for sustainable devel-opment: definition and characteristics explored. J. Clean. Prod. 18 (7), 629e636.

Wang, Y., Liu, J., Hansson, L., Zhang, K., Wang, R., 2011. Implementing stricterenvironmental regulation to enhance eco-efficiency and sustainability: a casestudy of Shandong Province’s pulp and paper industry. J. Clean. Prod. 19 (4),303e310.

Wilkie, W.L., 1990. Consumer Behaviour, second ed. John Wiley and Sons, New York.Zelezny, L.C., Chua, P.P., Aldrich, C., 2000. Elaborating on gender differences in

environmentalism. J. Soc. Issue. 56 (3), 443e457.Zilahy, G., Huisingh, D., 2009. The roles of academia in regional sustainability ini-

tiatives. J. Clean. Prod. 17 (2), 1057e1066.Zsóka, A., Szerényi, Z., Széchy, A., Kocsis, T., 2012. Greening due to environmental

education? Environmental knowledge, attitudes, consumer behaviour andeveryday pro-environmental activities of Hungarian high school and universitystudents. J. Clean. Prod.. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2012.11.030.