Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800...

15
Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source-detector distance on the fast optical signal Gabriele Gratton, * Carrie R. Brumback, Brian A. Gordon, Melanie A. Pearson, Kathy A. Low, and Monica Fabiani Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL 61801, USA Received 27 October 2005; revised 9 March 2006; accepted 16 May 2006 Available online 26 July 2006 Fast optical signals can be used to study the time course of neuronal activity in localized cortical areas. The first report of such signals [Gratton, G., Corballis, P. M., Cho, E., Fabiani, M., Hood, D., 1995a. Shades of gray matter: Noninvasive optical images of human brain responses during visual stimulation. Psychophysiol, 32, 505 – 509.] was based on photon delay measures. Subsequently, other laboratories have also measured fast optical signals, but a debate still exists about how these signals are generated and optimally recorded. Here we report data from a visual stimulation paradigm in which different parameters (continuous: DC intensity; modulated: AC intensity and photon delay), wavelengths (shorter and longer than the hemoglobin isosbestic point), and source-detector distances (shorter and longer than 22.5 mm) were used to record fast signals. Results indicate that a localized fast signal (peak latency = 80 ms) can be detected with both delay and AC intensity measures in visual cortex, but not with unmodulated DC measures. This is likely due to the fact that differential measures (delay and AC intensity) are less sensitive to superficial noise sources, which heavily influence DC intensity. The fast effect had similar sign at wavelengths shorter and longer than the hemoglobin isosbestic point, consistent with light scattering but not rapid deoxygenation accounts of this phenom- enon. Finally, the fast signal was only measured at source-detector distances greater than 22.5 mm, consistent with the intracranial origin of the signal, and providing indications about the minimum distance for recording. These data address some of the open questions in the field and provide indications about the optimal recording methods for fast optical signals. D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Fast optical signals; Event-related optical signal (EROS); Scattering; Optical brain imaging Introduction Fast optical signals provide a tool for imaging human brain function with sub-cm spatial resolution and ms-level temporal resolution. Ten years ago, we reported the first non-invasive measurement of fast changes in the optical properties of the human brain in response to visual stimulation (Gratton et al., 1995a). Due to limitations in the recording apparatus, this initial study was carried out on a very small subject sample (N = 3) and was based on a relatively slow sampling rate (20 Hz). The data were obtained with a frequency – domain optical spectrometer. The phase delay measures in response to the stimulus showed a few picoseconds increase in the mean time taken by photons to move between sources and detectors located on the surface of the head, compared to average baseline measures obtained immediately before the stimulus. We labeled these optical changes the ‘‘event-related optical signal’’ or EROS, as they are obtained by observing the average optical brain response time locked to a stimulus or other event. This initial observation was followed by a series of studies replicating these effects in the visual domain with larger sample sizes (Gratton, 1997; Gratton et al., 1998, 2000, 2001; Gratton and Fabiani, 2003). Additional studies showed that fast optical signals can be obtained with other modalities and from other cortical areas, including auditory (Rinne et al., 1999; Tse et al., 2006; Tse and Penney, in press; Fabiani et al., 2006), somatosensory (Maclin et al., 2004), motor (Gratton et al., 1995b; DeSoto et al., 2001), and frontal cortices (Low et al., 2006). All these studies provided results that were consistent with the original finding, showing increases in delay in active cortical areas. They also indicated that the optical changes are simultaneous with electrical changes measured with evoked potentials (EPs; Gratton et al., 1997, 2001; Maclin et al., 2004) or event-related brain potentials (ERPs; DeSoto et al., 2001; Fabiani et al., 2006; Low et al., 2006). Finally, comparisons of the localizations of optical responses with changes in the blood oxygenation level- dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) signal also indicated substantial correspondence between the two sets of responses (Gratton et al., 1997, 2000). 1053-8119/$ - see front matter D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.030 * Corresponding author. Beckman Institute, University of Illinois, 405 N. Mathews Ave., Urbana, IL 61801, USA. E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Gratton). Available online on ScienceDirect (www.sciencedirect.com). www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576 – 1590

Transcript of Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800...

Page 1: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg

NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576 – 1590

Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source-detector

distance on the fast optical signal

Gabriele Gratton,* Carrie R. Brumback, Brian A. Gordon, Melanie A. Pearson,

Kathy A. Low, and Monica Fabiani

Department of Psychology and Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL 61801, USA

Received 27 October 2005; revised 9 March 2006; accepted 16 May 2006

Available online 26 July 2006

Fast optical signals can be used to study the time course of neuronal

activity in localized cortical areas. The first report of such signals

[Gratton, G., Corballis, P. M., Cho, E., Fabiani, M., Hood, D., 1995a.

Shades of gray matter: Noninvasive optical images of human brain

responses during visual stimulation. Psychophysiol, 32, 505–509.] was

based on photon delay measures. Subsequently, other laboratories have

also measured fast optical signals, but a debate still exists about how

these signals are generated and optimally recorded. Here we report data

from a visual stimulation paradigm in which different parameters

(continuous: DC intensity; modulated: AC intensity and photon delay),

wavelengths (shorter and longer than the hemoglobin isosbestic point),

and source-detector distances (shorter and longer than 22.5 mm) were

used to record fast signals. Results indicate that a localized fast signal

(peak latency = 80 ms) can be detected with both delay and AC intensity

measures in visual cortex, but not with unmodulated DCmeasures. This

is likely due to the fact that differential measures (delay and AC

intensity) are less sensitive to superficial noise sources, which heavily

influence DC intensity. The fast effect had similar sign at wavelengths

shorter and longer than the hemoglobin isosbestic point, consistent with

light scattering but not rapid deoxygenation accounts of this phenom-

enon. Finally, the fast signal was only measured at source-detector

distances greater than 22.5 mm, consistent with the intracranial origin

of the signal, and providing indications about the minimum distance for

recording. These data address some of the open questions in the field

and provide indications about the optimal recording methods for fast

optical signals.

D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Fast optical signals; Event-related optical signal (EROS);

Scattering; Optical brain imaging

1053-8119/$ - see front matter D 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.030

* Corresponding author. Beckman Institute, University of Illinois, 405 N.

Mathews Ave., Urbana, IL 61801, USA.

E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Gratton).

Available online on ScienceDirect (www.sciencedirect.com).

Introduction

Fast optical signals provide a tool for imaging human brain

function with sub-cm spatial resolution and ms-level temporal

resolution. Ten years ago, we reported the first non-invasive

measurement of fast changes in the optical properties of the human

brain in response to visual stimulation (Gratton et al., 1995a). Due to

limitations in the recording apparatus, this initial study was carried

out on a very small subject sample (N = 3) and was based on a

relatively slow sampling rate (20 Hz). The data were obtained with a

frequency–domain optical spectrometer. The phase delay measures

in response to the stimulus showed a few picoseconds increase in the

mean time taken by photons to move between sources and detectors

located on the surface of the head, compared to average baseline

measures obtained immediately before the stimulus. We labeled

these optical changes the ‘‘event-related optical signal’’ or EROS, as

they are obtained by observing the average optical brain response

time locked to a stimulus or other event.

This initial observation was followed by a series of studies

replicating these effects in the visual domain with larger sample

sizes (Gratton, 1997; Gratton et al., 1998, 2000, 2001; Gratton and

Fabiani, 2003). Additional studies showed that fast optical signals

can be obtained with other modalities and from other cortical areas,

including auditory (Rinne et al., 1999; Tse et al., 2006; Tse and

Penney, in press; Fabiani et al., 2006), somatosensory (Maclin et

al., 2004), motor (Gratton et al., 1995b; DeSoto et al., 2001), and

frontal cortices (Low et al., 2006).

All these studies provided results that were consistent with the

original finding, showing increases in delay in active cortical

areas. They also indicated that the optical changes are simultaneous

with electrical changes measured with evoked potentials (EPs;

Gratton et al., 1997, 2001; Maclin et al., 2004) or event-related

brain potentials (ERPs; DeSoto et al., 2001; Fabiani et al., 2006;

Low et al., 2006). Finally, comparisons of the localizations of

optical responses with changes in the blood oxygenation level-

dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)

signal also indicated substantial correspondence between the two

sets of responses (Gratton et al., 1997, 2000).

Page 2: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1577

Other laboratories (Wolf et al., 2000, 2002, 2003a,b; Tse and

Penney, in press) have recently reported similar fast optical effects

(rapid increases in phase delay in association with cortical

activation) in the visual, motor, and auditory modalities, all

obtained with frequency–domain methods.1 Two other laborato-

ries have also reported the occurrence of fast optical signals but

have used different recording methods in which reductions in the

intensity of light reaching the detectors was the main finding rather

than increases in photon delay (Franceschini and Boas, 2004;

Steinbrink et al., 2000). Intensity changes were also observed by

Wolf et al. (2003a,b), concurrently with the phase delay effects.

In parallel with the recordings obtained in humans, Rector et al.

(1997; see also Rector et al., 2005) demonstrated that fast optical

changes, presumably due to changes in light scattering, can also be

observed in freely behaving animals. This finding is consistent

with earlier reports obtained in isolated neurons (e.g., Cohen et al.,

1971) and hippocampal slices (Frostig et al., 1990).

These data demonstrate the feasibility of detecting fast brain

responses, presumably directly related to neuronal activity, using

optical methods. Such data can potentially be very useful in

cognitive neuroscience research because they provide a good

combination of spatial and temporal information and thus may

facilitate our understanding of cortical dynamics. However,

questions remain about which measurements – changes in

intensity or delay – are optimal for recording fast optical signals.

A direct comparison based on the extant literature is difficult

because only a few studies have reported both intensity and delay

effects recorded from the same subjects in the same study (Wolf et

al., 2003a,b; Maclin et al., 2003).2 Further, the results of these

studies have revealed some inconsistencies, with some showing

more evident results with intensity measures and others with delay

measures. Intensity measures are simpler and less expensive to

obtain. However, continuous unmodulated (DC) intensity measures

may be sensitive to noise from many different sources, including

superficial events (such as capillary pulse) and environmental light

sources extraneous to the measurements. In contrast, measures that

are based on variations over time in the amount (AC intensity) as

well as the velocity (delay) of the photon diffusion process may be

exquisitely sensitive to differential effects occurring deep into the

tissue and provide better control for external sources of noise.

The current paper is aimed at providing a more systematic

examination of the parameters for the non-invasive recording of

fast optical signals. The methodology used, based on frequency–

domain optical methods (Gratton et al., 1990), allows for the direct

comparison of intensity and delay changes during visual stimula-

tion. We used a larger subjects sample than those used in our

previous studies (N = 19), and recorded from a dense array of

channels (160), all located over occipital areas, thus providing a

high spatial sampling. In addition to comparing intensity and delay

1 One lab did report inability to obtain reliable phase delay changes with

visual stimulation, and presented Monte Carlo simulation data purportedly

indicating that the phase delay changes should be too small to be detected

using current technologies (Syre et al., 2003). Notably, the spatial sampling

used in that study was much inferior and less systematic than that used in

the work from our laboratory, and this report has in fact been followed by a

number of positive findings when the measurements have been taken with

methods more closely matching those used in our lab.2 Frequency domain equipment is needed to obtain such data, as

continuous wave systems cannot record photon delay measures but are

limited to light intensity.

data, we also examined the effects of two wavelengths (690 and

830 nm) and the effects of source-detector distances in the

recording montage.

The use of multiple wavelengths provides some information

about the biophysical phenomena underlying the fast optical

signal. Specifically, two hypotheses about the generation of fast

optical effects can be contrasted: the scattering hypothesis and the

rapid deoxygenation hypothesis. Fast optical effects are commonly

considered to be due to rapid scattering changes co-occurring with

neuronal activity (presumably due either to changes in membrane

transparency or to movement of water from intra- to extra-cellular

compartments during neuronal depolarization). Whereas consider-

able evidence for this interpretation is available in in vitro tissue

preparations (e.g., Frostig et al., 1990), no direct demonstration of

the scattering hypothesis has been presented in vivo, and in

particular from the intact human head. A possible alternative

explanation is that the fast optical effects recorded at the scalp in

humans may be due to rapid deoxygenation of the hemoglobin

contained in brain tissue such as those reported with high field

functional magnetic resonance imaging (Hu et al., 1997), and with

exposed cortex optical imaging in animals (Vanzetta and Grinvald,

1999). According to this hypothesis, the occurrence of cortical

activity should induce a rapid increase in oxygen consumption in a

particular cortical area (see also Weber et al., 2004). When this

occurs, hemoglobin becomes more deoxygenated. These two

hypotheses can be contrasted by determining whether fast optical

imaging effects have the same or different sign (e.g., increases in

photon delay) at wavelengths shorter and longer than the

hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the

absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over). If

rapid deoxygenation is the cause of the fast optical signal, it

should invert sign when measured using wavelengths on opposite

sides of the hemoglobin isosbestic point, as an increase in the

concentration of deoxy-hemoglobin should be associated with a

concurrent decrease in the concentration of oxy-hemoglobin. If

instead the fast optical signal is due to changes in scattering,

similar results should be observed for both wavelengths (although

presumably effects should be slightly larger in absolute terms at

shorter wavelengths, as scattering decreases with the fourth power

of the light wavelength). Thus, a comparison of the fast optical

effects at 690 and 830 nm should distinguish between these two

possibilities.

A third hypothesis is that the fast optical signal is related to

rapid changes in blood flow or blood volume. However, empirical

evidence for such changes is currently scanty: Although reports

exist of changes of this type within 1 s from stimulation (e.g.,

Lindauer et al., 1993; Jones et al., 2001), their time course (onset

latency longer than 0.5 s) is slower than that of the fast optical

signals as reported in the literature. For this reason, this hypothesis

will not be further considered here.

This study also provides empirical data assessing the optimal

range of source-detector distances to be used for recording the fast

signal to maximize the exploration of brain volumes rather than of

more superficial structures (such as muscles or skin).3 The relative

3 Evidence that the fast signals are generated inside the brain has been

presented in several studies (e.g., Gratton et al., 1995a,b, 1997, 2000),

which have shown close correspondence between the locations at which

such signals are observed and the cortical areas activated by a particular

stimulus (as determined by fMRI).

Page 3: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901578

depth of the area probed by the optical sensors can be investigated

by varying the source-detector distance (e.g., Gratton et al., 2000;

Choi et al., 2004). At very short source-detector distances (less

than 20–25 mm), the volume investigated by non-invasive optical

methods should be very superficial (at most 1 cm deep), including

only very small portions of brain tissue. However, at longer source-

detector distances this volume should encompass a greater portion

of brain tissue. Thus, as fast optical signals are generated inside the

brain, we should expect these signals to increase in size with

source-detector distance. Conversely, noise signals generated in the

skin, superficial muscles, or the environment should remain

constant or decrease in amplitude with source-detector distance.

However, with very long source-detector distances (exceeding 40–

50 mm), relatively little light is expected to reach the detector and

the signals can become noisy and unstable. Thus, it is important to

establish the optimal range of source-detector distances for the

recording of optical signals and to exclude distances that would

contribute data from structures other than the brain and/or add to

the noise. Because the optical montage used in the current study

used a variety of source-detector distances for the same surface

locations, it is possible to estimate the size of the fast optical effect

as a function of source-detector distance and thus assess the

minimum usable distance for fast optical recording.

As in our previous optical imaging work on visual cortex

(Gratton et al., 1995a,b; Gratton and Fabiani, 2003), we used

a pattern-reversing stimulus. This type of stimulus was chosen

because it is isoluminant and minimizes stimulus-related

changes in environmental light. This study also used a number

of recent methodological innovations designed to improve

spatial resolution and increase the power and rigor of the

statistical analyses, which are described in more detail in the

Methods section. This study was also part of a larger project

investigating neurovascular coupling, whose results are beyond

the scope of the current paper and will be reported elsewhere.

However, for comparison and validation purposes we also

report here visual evoked potentials (VEPs) that were recorded

concurrently with the optical data. The experimental design

and the use of multiple wavelengths also allowed us to

compute slow/hemodynamic optical effects, which are also

reported here in a summary fashion for comparison with the

fast signal.

Methods

Participants

Nineteen young adults (9 females, age 20–28, mean age 22.3)

participated in a multiple-session study, including an MR session

(during which structural MRs were collected) and an optical

session (during which both optical imaging and ERP data were

collected). All subjects reported themselves in good health and free

from medications affecting the central nervous system. All subjects

had normal or corrected to normal vision.

The data were collected within the context of a larger project

investigating neurovascular coupling in younger and older adults.

For the purpose of the current paper, only a subset of the data are

presented. All participants signed written consent forms and were

compensated for their time. All aspects of the experiment were

approved by the local IRB and conformed to the Helsinki

declaration for the protection of human subjects.

Task and stimuli

During the optical recording, participants sat in front of a

computer monitor centered at eye-level at a distance of approxi-

mately 80 cm. The optical recording was divided into two sessions,

with data from a different set of 80 overlaid optical channels

(defined by a particular source-detector pair) collected from each.

A set of 80 channels is referred to as a ‘‘montage’’. A session

consisted of 60 blocks, each lasting 60 s. Every block began with a

20-s period during which a fixation cross was displayed. After the

initial fixation, a black and white checkerboard appeared and the

cells switched color (i.e., reversed), with a frequency that varied in

different blocks. The checkerboard subtended a total visual angle

of 15- horizontally and 17- vertically and had a spatial frequency

of 0.4 cycles/degree.

The reversal frequencies used were 1.0417 Hz (1-Hz condi-

tion), 2.0833 Hz (2-Hz condition), 4.1667 Hz (4-Hz condition),

6.25 Hz (6-Hz condition), and 8.3333 Hz (8-Hz condition). The

reason for these stimulation frequencies was that they are all (apart

from 8.33 Hz) factors of the sampling rate (62.5 Hz), thus an

integer number of sampling points occurred between stimuli. The

reversal frequency conditions were presented with the following

order (once for each montage): 1-Hz, 2-Hz, 4-Hz, 6-Hz, 8-Hz, 1-

Hz, 2-Hz, 4-Hz, 2-Hz, 1-Hz, 8-Hz, 6-Hz, 4-Hz, 2-Hz, 1-Hz. An

unequal number of blocks for the different frequencies was used to

reduce the difference between the number of reversals (trials)

across conditions by increasing the number of blocks at the lowest

frequencies.

Each block started with 20 s of fixation, followed by a

stimulation period lasting 19.2 s, and was followed by a 20.8-s

blank period. For the purposes of the current paper, only data from

the reversal period are considered, with each reversal considered as a

stimulus. The total amount of light generated by the computer screen

was identical during all phases of the stimulation, and did not vary

when a reversal occurred, as the color reversal of each square leads to

an isoluminant condition. The only environmental light variation

was determined by the refreshing rate of the computer screen (70

Hz), which, however, was faster than retinal persistence and not time

locked with the stimulation conditions across stimuli, blocks, or

subjects. The participants’ task was to fixate at the center of the

checkerboard. Brief rest periods were given every 5 blocks.

Optical recording

Optical data were recorded using an Imagent device (ISS Inc.,

Champaign, IL), based on 64 laser diode sources (32 emitting light

at 690 nm and 32 emitting light at 830 nm) and 8 photomultiplier

tubes (PMTs) as detectors. Only a subset of the source laser diodes

were used at one time, using the montage described in Fig. 1. The

light sources reached the head by using 0.4-mm optic fibers,

whereas 3-mm fiber optic bundles were used for connecting the

head to the detectors (photomultiplier tubes, PMTs). Source and

detector fibers were held in place on the participant’s head using

modified motorcycle helmets. Hair was combed away from under

the detector fibers before they were put in place; the source fibers,

being much smaller, could be placed through the hair. The use of a

rigid helmet minimized the effects of head and body movements on

the recordings. Source fibers were paired, so that each location was

illuminated by a fiber connected to a 690-nm laser and another

connected to an 830-nm laser (which were never turned on at the

same time).

Page 4: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the helmet (back view) and montage

used for recording optical data in the current study. Sources are indicated by

the small circles, and detectors are indicated by the large circles. Blue

indicates the sources and detectors used in one recording session and red

indicates the source and detectors used in the other recording session (the

order of the two sessions was counterbalanced across subjects). Each

channel (or source-detector pair) is indicated by a line, in blue or red,

depending on the session. Black and open circles indicate locations on the

helmet that were not used in this study.

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1579

The light sources were time multiplexed in cycles of 16 ms

divided into ten 1.6-ms periods (1.6 ms ‘‘on’’ periods, and 14.4 ms

‘‘off’’ periods for each source), so that at any given time each

detector could only receive light from one source closer than 6-cm

distance. Our measurements indicate that the amount of light

transmitted between a source and a detector at distances exceeding

6 cm is negligible (Gratton and Fabiani, 2003).

The light sources were modulated in intensity at 110 MHz. The

average intensity during the ‘‘on’’ periods was less than 10 mW per

source; considering the off periods, each source emitted an average

of less than 1 mW light power, well below FDA standards. The

detector amplifiers were modulated at a frequency of 110.00625

MHz. This created a cross-correlation (also called heterodyning or

beating) frequency of 6.25 kHz. This frequency was used for the

recording of frequency–domain data. The optical data from each

detector were sampled continuously at a frequency of 50 kHz.

Eight points were collected for each oscillation of the cross-

correlation frequency. As each source was on for 1.6 ms, there

were 10 oscillations per source during each multiplex period. To

eliminate the possibility of cross-talk between sources, we

discarded the first two oscillations (.32 ms) of recording from

each channel. The remaining eight oscillations (for a total of 64

digitized points) were used to compute a fast Fourier transform

(FFT) yielding estimates of DC intensity (zero-frequency or

average effects), AC intensity (amplitude of oscillations at the

6.25-kHz cross-correlation frequency), and phase delay4 for each

4 In a heterodyning system, a change in phase at the beating frequency

reflects an equivalent phase change between the input frequencies. As the

beating frequency is always lower than the original frequencies, this

represents an amplification of the time difference between the original

frequencies.

source/detector combination (80 per montage, 160 in total) with an

effective sampling period of 16 ms (62.5 Hz).

Electrophysiological recordings and analysis

VEPs were recorded continuously during the stimulation period

at 200 Hz from eight scalp electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz, T5, T6, P3, P4,

and right mastoid) referred to the left mastoid using a 1- to 100-Hz

bandpass, with a 60-Hz notch filter. The data were transformed off-

line into an average mastoid reference system. Vertical and

horizontal electro-oculograms (EOG) were recorded bipolarly from

electrodes placed above and below the right eye and to the left and

right of the outer canthus of each eye, respectively. EOG artifacts

were corrected off-line using a procedure described by Gratton et

al. (1983). Data were segmented into epochs time locked to each

stimulus, starting with a 25-ms pre-stimulus baseline. Trials with

shifts exceeding 200 AV were discarded. Separate averages were

computed for each subject, electrode, and stimulation condition.

Note that the optical and electrophysiological data were recorded

simultaneously.

Structural MRI

All subjects included in this study underwent a high-resolution

structural MRI scan in a Siemens 3-T Magnetom Allegra MR

Headscanner located at the Biomagnetic Imaging Center (BIC) of

the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Using an

MPRAGE sequence, a 144-slice scan with a 1.2-mm slice

thickness was obtained either in the sagittal or axial plane. The

pulse parameters used in MR recording included a repetition time

of 800 ms, an echo time of 4.38 ms, and a flip angle of 8-. Thefield of view was 240 � 240 � 172.8 mm with matrix dimensions

of 192 � 256 � 144 and voxel size of 1.25 � 0.938 � 1.2 mm.

Fig. 2. Grand average time course of the visual evoked potentials (VEPs).

The data are presented as changes in the variability (standard deviation)

across electrodes with respect to a pre-stimulus baseline. The two vertical

shaded bars represent the intervals of interest for this study. The different

colored lines represent the different frequencies of stimulation (checker-

board reversal).

Page 5: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Table 1

Steps in the analysis of fast optical data

Pre-processing

For each channel, condition, and participant

Phase wrapping correction and transformation in picoseconds

Correction of slow drifts

Pulse correction

DC and AC normalization

Computation of modulation

Filtering (optional)

Estimation of phase SD (to discard noisy channels)

Segmentation into stimulus (or response)-locked epochs and averaging

of like trial types

Filtering (optional)

Baseline subtraction

Mapping and statistical analysis

Set up statistical contrasts to compare conditions and/or groups

For each participant:

Select a 2.5-mm step grid within desired view (e.g., front-back)

using Talairach coordinates

For each location of the grid and measure (phase, AC, DC, modulation)

Determine the channels whose volumes encompass location

For each time point and statistical contrast:

Compute value associated with each contrast

For DC intensity, AC intensity, and modulation, compute logarithm

of value

Average together all selected channels

For each time point and statistical contrast, use SPM approach to:

Spatially filter maps

Compute t score maps across participants

Convert t score maps into Z score maps

Estimate a criterion Z value using a ‘‘blob analysis’’ approach

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901580

Co-registration of optical and MR anatomical data

This procedure comprised the following steps. First, the

location of each source and detector was digitized in 3D with

respect to three fiducial points (located on the nasion and left and

right pre-auricular points) on each individual subject using a

Polhemus ‘‘3Space’’\ 3D digitizer. Second, the structural MR

obtained for each participant (see above), allowed us to co-register

the source and detector positions on the MR anatomical image

where the fiducial points were marked using Beekley Spots\. The

average accuracy of the co-registration procedure was estimated to

be about 7 mm. Third, for each participant the MR and optical

source and detector points were subjected to Talairach transfor-

mation to place them in a common space. Fourth, to further reduce

the influence of anatomical variability across participants, the

individual anatomical images were centered on the midpoint

between the left and right calcarine fissures as identified in the

structural MR images. This allowed for a more accurate between-

subjects co-registration of visual cortex. Fifth, two-dimensional

Table 2

Estimated signal-to-noise (S/N) at various stages in the analysis

No. of trials No. of channels Frequencies

Data collection 1 1 31

Averaging 240 1 31

Spatial reconstruction 240 6 31

Frequency filtering 240 6 10

Grand average 240 6 10

reconstructions of optical effects at various latencies from

stimulation were obtained using a 2.5-mm grid.

Analysis of optical data

The steps of the analysis of fast optical data are presented

schematically in Table 1. To provide a reference point, we

presented the estimated signal-to-noise achieved at each major

stage of the analysis in Table 2. The signal size estimates were

obtained from the grand average effects reported in this study,

whereas the noise estimates were obtained from the standard

deviations of the single trial data. Note that such estimates are

heavily dependent on the conditions used in a particular

experiment and therefore may be hard to generalize across studies.

The optical data from each participant and location were first

corrected for phase wrapping around the 360- mark and trans-

formed into picoseconds. Then, slow drifts were corrected using a

polynomial regression method (effectively eliminating frequencies

below 0.01 Hz). A pulse correction algorithm developed by

Gratton and Corballis (1995) was then applied. DC intensity and

AC intensity were normalized by dividing them by the average

value across each block. For each channel (i.e., source-detector

combination), an estimate of the standard deviation (SD) of the

phase was obtained and used to discard channels with excessive

variability (SD > 160 ps), usually due to insufficient amount of

light reaching the detectors to provide valid estimates. This

procedure led to discarding approximately 35% of the channels,

mostly because the montage comprised a large range of source-

detector distances (including very long ones). Table 3 reports, for

each source-detector distance, the total number of channels from

which data were recorded, as well as the number of channels with

acceptable noise levels.

The data were then segmented into 240 ms epochs, time locked

to each pattern reversal (i.e., stimulus), beginning 16 ms before

each reversal. The first five stimulations from each block were

discarded to generate stable stimulation conditions. The remaining

epochs were used to compute average waveforms for each

measurement type (DC intensity, AC intensity, and delay).

Modulation was also estimated using the ratio of the AC value

over the DC value. For each channel and subject, averages were

computed over a total of 240 trials in the 1-Hz condition, 560 trials

in the 2-Hz condition, 900 trials in the 4-Hz condition, 920 trials in

the 6-Hz condition, and 1240 trials in the 8-Hz condition, The data

were then filtered to eliminate frequencies above 10 Hz (to increase

signal-to-noise ratio, see Maclin et al., 2003), and a baseline

(estimated as the average of a 32-ms peri-stimulus period) was

subtracted from the data. This baseline was selected to reduce the

impact of the previous trial while maintaining sufficient baseline

duration.

For each participant, a 2.5-mm grid was established over the

surface of the occipital scalp (centered on the mid-calcarine point,

(Hz) No. of subjects Signal (ps) Noise (ps) S/N

1 1.0 133.6 0.008

1 1.0 8.6 0.116

1 1.0 3.5 0.285

1 1.0 1.1 0.882

19 1.0 0.3 3.846

Page 6: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Table 3

Average number of channels accepted as a function of source-detector

distance

Distance (mm) Average number of

channels recorded

Channels with

acceptable noise

12.5–17.5 14.1 14.1

17.5–22.5 44.1 42.3

22.5–27.5 33.8 27.6

27.5–32.5 24.3 15.7

32.5–37.5 15.8 5.8

37.5–42.5 10.4 1.1

42.5–50 9.3 0.2

>50 8.3 0.4

Total 160.0 107.2

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1581

as described above). For each location of the grid, the channels

whose recording volumes encompassed the location were then

determined (independently for each participant). For delay data, the

different channels were then averaged together. For DC intensity,

AC intensity, and modulation, the logarithms of the values were

averaged, achieving the mathematical equivalent of a ‘‘pi detector’’

(Wolf et al., 2000). For phase data, the absolute value (rather than

the logarithm) was used for the following reasons. First, phase

delay may be negative. As a consequence, the product of two

negative values would generate a positive number, which would be

incorrect in this case (the sign of the effect would depend on the

number of channels crossing a particular point). Second, whereas

effects at different locations along the photon path combine in a

multiplicative fashion for intensity measures, they combine in an

additive fashion for phase delay measures. Note that the volumes

corresponding to each channel (i.e., source-detector pair) were

‘‘discretised,’’ that is, any given pixel was classified as being either

in or out of the path rather than being assigned a probability.

Most points in the central grid region were within the path of

several (up to 20) channels that were weighted equally. This

allowed us to select channels with specific source-detector

distances—an important prerequisite for the analysis of source-

detector distance effects. In fact, for most of the analyses (apart

from that of the source-detector distance effect), only channels with

source-detector distances between 22.5 mm and 50 mm were used.

The rationale for this choice is that channels with source-detector

distances shorter than 22.5 mm are unlikely to be sensitive to

cortical activity (at least in occipital areas), and channels with

source-detector distances exceeding 50 mm are typically very

noisy.

For each measurement type, the previous methods yielded a

grid (map) of activity for each time point (15 time points; sampling

rate: 16 ms), stimulation condition, and participant. These maps

were spatially filtered using an 8-mm Gaussian filter (2 cm kernel),

and used for statistical analysis. This analysis was conducted by

computing three types of t score maps across participants: one

related to the difference between the average maps and the pre-

stimulus baseline across stimulation conditions (average effect),

one related to the linear trend between different stimulation

conditions (with the 1-Hz stimulation condition expected to

produce the largest response, and the 8-Hz stimulation condition

expected to produce the smallest response), and one related to the

1-Hz condition compared to baseline. Each of these t score maps

was converted into Z score maps. Following the methodology

commonly used to correct for the problem of multiple comparisons

when analyzing brain imaging data, the significance of a response

was estimated by comparing the Z score value observed at the peak

point with a criterion value estimated using a ‘‘blob analysis’’

approach (see Friston et al., 1995).

This method assumes that the spatial sampling is relatively

homogenous over the area in which the criterion is computed.

However, the montage used only afforded a relatively constant

spatial sampling (i.e., channel density) in the central recording

region, whereas spatial sampling was much sparser at the periphery

of the recording region. Therefore, a region of interest (ROI)

encompassing the central recording region was used for this

estimation. The use of a restricted ROI also increased the power of

the analysis. Expressed in Talairach coordinates, the ROI ranged

from �21 to +21 along the x (right-left) axis and between �12 and

+12 along the z (bottom-top) axis. Note that the ROI was not

defined along the y (back-front) axis because only surface

projections were used.

In order to reduce the number of comparisons and reduce the

probability of alpha error, only the maps obtained at a latency of 80

ms from pattern-reversal stimulation were used for the determina-

tion of the statistical significance of an effect. This latency was

selected on the basis of previous studies (e.g., Gratton et al., 2001).

In all cases, directional hypotheses were made (with expected

increases for the delay parameter, and decreases for all other

parameters). Criteria corresponding to one-directional P values of

0.05 (corrected for multiple comparisons) were used to estimate the

significance of effects.

The methods used in the current study (using multiple wave-

lengths and a blocked stimulation design) also permitted the

computation of slow optical effects. Relative oxy- and deoxy-

hemoglobin concentration changes were estimated using standard

procedures (Boas et al., 2001) and averaged across blocks for each

subject, stimulation condition, and source-detector pair.

Results

Overall analyses

The purpose of this section is to report the overall effects

obtained in the study, in order to demonstrate that they were similar

to those reported in previous work. As the current study was part of

a larger project investigating the relationship between neuronal and

vascular measures, a more complete analysis of the relationship

between various measures will be presented elsewhere. Here we

only present summary measures for VEPs and slow/hemodynamic

optical effects for comparison and validation purposes.

A summary of the VEPs recorded in the current study is

presented in Fig. 2. Following Gratton et al. (2001), we report the

VEP in terms of variability (standard deviation) across electrodes

for each stimulation frequency condition. Although this measure

does not provide information about the electrode at which effects

are largest, it provides useful information about the latency at

which the electrodes separate from each other the most, indicating

the occurrence of localized brain activity. Note that two peaks are

observable in the variability waveforms (shaded gray in Fig. 2):

one with a latency of approximately 80 ms and one with a latency

of approximately 128 ms. They correspond roughly to the C1 and

N1 components of the VEP, thought to correspond to activity in

striate and extrastriate cortex, respectively. As in Gratton et al.

(2001), both these components are much larger for the 1-Hz

condition than for the other stimulation conditions, and, as

Page 7: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 3. Left column: Z score maps (computed across subjects) of the change in oxy-hemoglobin (HbO2, top) and deoxy-hemoglobin concentration (Hb, bottom)

between 10 and 20 s from stimulation, averaged across the 5 stimulation frequency conditions. Red and yellow indicate increases in hemoglobin concentration

with respect to baseline (z > 2.0), and blue indicates decreases. The region of interest is indicated by the green box. Right column: time course of oxy- and

deoxy-hemoglobin concentration changes at surface location x = 12, z = 0 (in Talairach coordinates).

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901582

expected, the amplitude of the response appears to be inversely

related to stimulation frequency (e.g., Van der Tweel and Verduyn

Lunel, 1965).

Fig. 4. Grand average coronal (back view) maps of the peak latency of the fast opti

maps computed across subjects at different latencies from stimulation. The colo

(positive for delay and negative for AC intensity). In this figure, only voxels in w

intensity (at any latency) are shown. TL = top left; TR = top right; BL = bottom

Fig. 3 reports statistical (Z scores computed across subjects)

surface maps (back views) of the oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin

effects during each stimulation period (measured during the

cal response (delay left; AC intensity: right). The data were based on Z score

r indicates the latency of the peak Z score value for that particular voxel

hich the Z score was greater than +2 for delay or smaller than �2 for AC

left; BR = bottom right.

Page 8: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1583

interval between 10 and 20 s after the beginning of stimulation).

Fig. 3 also reports the time course of the oxy- and deoxy-

hemoglobin changes (right panel). For all these figures, data are

collapsed across stimulation frequencies. These data indicate that

the visual stimulation caused an increase in oxy-hemoglobin and a

decrease in deoxy-hemoglobin, consistent with a typical blood

flow (BOLD) effect. This effect occurred over an extended period

of time (several seconds), also consistent with extant literature

(e.g., Villringer and Chance, 1997).

Fig. 4 reports summary grand average surface maps of the fast

optical response measured using the delay parameter (left) and

AC intensity parameter (right). In Fig. 4, only voxels with a

cross-subject Z exceeding the value of +2 (for delay) or lower

than �2 (for AC intensity) are shown. The color indicates the

latency of the peak response (the color scale indicating the actual

latency of the response in ms is reported below the maps). The

data are collapsed across stimulation frequencies and wave-

lengths. For the delay parameter, Fig. 4 indicates the occurrence

of a medial response with a latency of approximately 80 ms,

followed by a more lateral response with a latency of 120–160

ms. The latencies of these two responses correspond roughly with

those of the two peaks of the VEP reported in Fig. 2. Further, the

Fig. 5. Axial grand average Z score maps (computed across subjects, Talairach coo

from stimulation for each contrast condition (average, slope, and 1-Hz condition

indicates the region of interest; the white cross indicates the peak point. Red and ye

2.0), and shades of blue indicate decreases in the parameter. Peak Z = peak Z value

Crit. = criterion Z score for statistical significance, taking into consideration the pr

left; BR = back right.

total area activated (albeit at different latencies) corresponds

roughly with that showing a slow optical response in Fig. 3.

These data suggest that the fast optical signal provides temporal

information about the latency of activity in cortical areas.

However, for the AC parameter, a response exceeding the

criterion (with a latency of approximately 80 ms) could be

observed only over the right occipital area.

Fig. 5 reports simplified tomographic reconstructions (for the

principle used for estimating the depth of effects, see Gratton et al.,

2000) of the fast optical effect at a latency of 80 ms for delay and

AC intensity. The data are presented as axial Z score maps

(computed across subjects), corresponding to the z = 0 slice in

Talairach coordinates. Separate maps are presented for the average

across conditions, the inverse linear trend as a function of

stimulation frequency, and for the 1-Hz condition. Although

preliminary, these data are consistent with the summary latency

maps presented in Fig. 4, in that a bilateral, medial response is

observed for the delay parameter, whereas the AC intensity

parameter shows only a right hemisphere response. The linear

trend (slope) data indicate that the 1-Hz condition shows a larger

response than the other stimulation conditions (consistent with the

VEP data).

rdinate of the slice: z = 0) of the fast optical response at a latency of 80 ms

alone) for delay (top row) and AC intensity (bottom row). The green box

llow colors indicate increases in the parameter with respect to baseline (Z >

within the region of interest (positive for delay, negative for AC intensity);

oblem of multiple comparisons. FL = front left; FR = front right; BL = back

Page 9: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 6. Amplitude of the optical response (delay parameter) obtained for each subject at a latency of 80 ms for Talairach coordinate x = 12, y = surface, z = 0.

Right panel: response in picoseconds. Left panel: response as a function of the standard deviation of the raw data.

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901584

All of the data presented so far were obtained across subjects.

The individual subject effects for the delay parameter are

presented in Fig. 6. The graphs depict the mean amplitude of

the response for each subject at a latency of 80 ms and taken at

the peak location of the maximum effect for the group (Talairach

coordinates: x = 12, y = surface, z = 0). One subject did not have

valid channels at this location. In the right panel, the responses

are presented in picoseconds. In the left panel, they are presented

as a fraction of the variability (SD) measured on single trials for

each channel (before filtering). Note that approximately half of

the subjects showed a response exceeding 1 ps at this location,

and approximately two thirds of the subjects showed a response

exceeding 0.01 standard units.

Comparison of different measures of the fast signal

The delay parameter grand average Z score maps at a latency

of 80 ms from the onset of the reversal, averaged across

stimulation conditions (average), for the linear trend (slope) and

for the 1-Hz condition (1 Hz), are presented in Fig. 7A. The Z

score maps for AC intensity, modulation, and DC intensity are

shown in Figs. 7B, C, and D, respectively. Data obtained with

690 nm and 830 nm light were combined for this analysis. These

data indicate that a significant fast optical response can be

observed in occipital cortex at a latency of 80 ms from

stimulation (pattern reversal). As predicted, this response was

characterized by an increase in delay and a reduction in AC

intensity and modulation. The results for DC intensity were much

less clear, and it is not obvious that a significant response could

be observed with this method, once corrected for multiple-

comparisons. The results of trend analysis (slope) indicate that the

fast optical effects decrease with stimulus frequency, confirming

our previous findings (Gratton et al., 2001), and in agreement

with the electrophysiological data obtained in the current as well

as in previous studies (Van der Tweel and Verduyn Lunel, 1965;

Gratton et al., 2001).

For delay and AC intensity the grand average time course of the

response at the peak location for each type of contrast is presented

in Fig. 8. This figure illustrates the mean size of the response with

each measurement technique. It indicates that, in the 1-Hz

condition, the delay change is of the order of 2 ps, whereas the

AC and modulation effects are of the order of 1 part over a

thousand (the log10 of the effect, multiplied by 100, is reported in

Fig. 8—note that 10(0.04/100)�1.001). All effects are 2–3 times

larger in the 1-Hz condition alone than in the other contrasts, as

expected based on the known physiology of electrical steady-state

responses in visual cortex. The sizes of these effects are of the

same order of magnitude as those reported in previous studies (e.g.,

Maclin et al., 2003).

Overall, these data imply that fast optical responses can be

observed in a robust manner with different dependent variables

(such as phase delay, modulation and AC intensity measures),

although unmodulated DC intensity measures are less effective in

measuring these signals.

Comparison of different wavelengths

Fig. 9 shows grand average time course data separated by

source wavelength (690 and 830 nm) and measure (delay and

AC intensity) for the 1-Hz condition at the Talairach location x =

12, z = 0 (corresponding maps are shown in Fig. 10). The

results show that there is a substantial similarity of the 1-Hz fast

optical signals measured with 690 nm and 830 nm light. The

AC response appeared slightly larger at 690 nm, but also noisier

(as indicated by the larger error bars). For the AC intensity

measures at 830 nm, differences begin to emerge later in the

epoch, past the 80-ms peak of activity that is common to all

graphs and corresponds to the latency of the earliest electro-

physiological response. In fact, whereas the other three measures

return to baseline by the end of the epoch, the AC measure at

this wavelength remains relatively flat. This is likely to reflect

the influence of slow oxygenation changes (in this case,

increases in oxy-hemoglobin due to a ‘‘BOLD-type’’ response)

superimposed on the fast effect. This shift may occur because

the rise function of the BOLD response extends over a very

long period of time (see Fig. 3). These effects are much less

visible in the delay parameter, and for the AC measures taken at

690 nm.

In summary, these data are consistent with the hypothesis that

scattering is the dominant factor underlying fast effects and are

Page 10: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 7. Grand average coronal (back view) Z score maps of the fast optical response at a latency of 80 ms from stimulation for each contrast condition (left

column = average, middle column = slope, and right colum = 1-Hz condition) for delay (A), AC intensity (B), modulation (C), and DC intensity (D). Red and

yellow colors indicate increases in the parameter with respect to baseline (Z > 2.0), and shades of blue indicate decreases in the parameter. Other data reported

in the figure have the same meaning as in Fig. 5.

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1585

Page 11: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 8. Time course of the fast optical response at Talairach coordinates x = 12, y = posterior surface, z = 0, for the delay parameter (left panel, in picoseconds)

and AC intensity parameter (right panel, expressed as log10 (AC change) � 100). Error bars represent the standard error computed across subjects, separately

for each time point. The vertical bar at time zero indicates a pattern reversal in the checkerboard stimulus.

Fig. 9. Grand average time course of the fast optical response at the location of peak response for the 1-Hz condition, for delay (top row) and AC intensity

(bottom row), and for 690-nm light (left column) and 830-nm light (right column). The data are from surface location x = 12, z = 0 (in Talairach coordinates).

The delay is expressed in picoseconds, and the AC intensity as log10 (AC change) � 100. The error bars represent the standard error computed across subjects,

separately for each time point.

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901586

Page 12: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 10. Grand average Z score maps of the fast optical response at a latency of 80 ms from stimulation for the 1-Hz condition, separately for delay (top row)

and AC intensity (bottom row), and for 690-nm light (left column) and 830-nm light (right column). The region of interest is indicated by a green box, the peak

point by a white cross. Other data reported in the figure have the same meaning as in Fig. 5.

Table 4

Signal, noise (estimated across subjects), and signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) as

a function of source-detector distance for delay and AC intensity at

Talairach x = 12, z = 0 (AC effect = log10(AC) � 100; delay effect = ps,

distance in mm)

Distance (mm) Delay AC intensity

Signal Noise S/N Signal Noise S/N

12.5–17.5 0.553 0.439 1.262 0.013 0.009 1.414

17.5–22.5 1.027 0.486 2.113 0.004 0.004 0.860

22.5–27.5 0.751 0.304 2.472 0.018 0.013 1.408

27.5 32.5 2.095 0.608 3.445 0.026 0.017 1.500

32.5–37.5 1.212 1.399 0.866 0.021 0.023 0.926

37.5–42.5 3.664 2.865 1.279 0.244 0.109 2.242

22.5–50 2.057 0.551 3.737 0.033 0.013 2.587

The distances 22.5–50 mm are those used for all the analyses presented in

the study and are shown here for comparison purposes.

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1587

clearly in contrast with the rapid deoxygenation hypothesis.

Whereas the scattering hypothesis predicts same-sign effects at

wavelengths placed at either side of the hemoglobin isosbestic

point, the rapid deoxygenation hypothesis predicts effects of

opposite sign.

Effect of source-detector distance

To study the effect of source-detector distance, we separately

measured the amplitude of the fast response at the point of peak

for channels grouped by source-detector distance. To obtain

enough channels for reliable measures, we selected five 5-mm

source-detector distance bins: 12.5–17.5 mm, 17.5–22.5 mm,

22.5–27.5 mm, 27–32.5 mm, and 32.5–37.5 mm. Data from

both wavelengths (690 nm and 830 nm) were combined for this

analysis. Only the 1-Hz condition data were used for this analysis

because this condition exhibited the largest fast response and

therefore was expected to be more stable even at long source-

detector distances. To make the results from different distances

comparable, we used the same surface location (Talairach

coordinates: x = 12, z = 0) and selected channels crossing this

point but varying in source-detector distance. Note that, due to

the variability of the sensors placements with respect to the

individual subjects’ brains and to the different amount of noise

across subjects, not all subjects contributed to all bins. The

amplitude of the fast signals for each bin is presented in Table 4,

separately for phase delay and AC intensity. The same data are

presented in graphic form in Fig. 11.

These data indicate that the fast optical response, measured

either with delay or AC intensity measures, increases as a function

of source-detector distance. The response is practically non-

existent, or even of opposite sign (for the delay parameter) at

short source-detector distances (less than 22.5 mm), and quite large

at long source-detector distances (exceeding 22.5 mm). However,

at the longest source-detector distances, the data became very

noisy, as indicated by the large error bars in Fig. 11. When

separated in 5-mm bins, only the delay effects between 22.5 and

32.5 mm distance were significant. These results are consistent

Page 13: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

Fig. 11. Line plots representing the magnitude of the fast optical response (delay on the left, AC intensity on the right) at the peak location (latency = 80 ms;

Talairach coordinates x = 12; y = posterior surface of cortex; z = 0) as a function of source-detector distance. The error bars represent the standard error

computed across subjects. The distance values reflect the midpoint of the 5-mm bins.

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901588

with predictions based on anatomical information, as channels with

short source-detector distances are unlikely to reach the visual

cortex and to be affected by brain phenomena, whereas channels

with long source-detector distances are much more likely to reflect

intracranial activity. In summary, these data are consistent with the

brain origin of fast optical effects and indicate the minimum

distances that would be useful for recording fast signals, at least

from visual cortex.

Discussion

The results of the current study indicate that different dependent

variables (i.e., phase delay, modulation, and AC intensity) can all

be used to measure fast cortical phenomena, at least in visual

cortex. The results of the wavelength analyses are consistent with

the scattering hypothesis and are clearly inconsistent with the fast-

deoxygenation hypothesis for the origin of the fast signal, although

other hypotheses (such as rapid blood flow effects) were not

directly tested. The distance analyses are consistent with the brain

origin of the effects and suggest that care must be taken in

choosing (a priori) or selecting (post hoc) the range of distances

that can be used to best observe these effects.

The issue of the method of measurement of fast optical

response deserves further discussion. First, consistent with

previous findings, our current data indicate that phase delay,

modulation, and AC intensity all can be used to measure fast

optical responses, whereas similar clear-cut results are not obtained

with unmodulated DC intensity measures. This appears to contrast

with results from another laboratory that has instead reported fast

optical effects with continuous-light measures (e.g., Steinbrink et

al., 2000). However, we do not believe that these results are

necessarily in contradiction. In fact, when measures are taken with

unmodulated DC instruments, it is indispensable to eliminate any

possible contamination from extraneous environmental light

sources—which may occur in some cases and not others. This

requirement is not as strict for AC measures, as external sources of

noise can be distinguished from brain effects because the latter are

carried by the modulation frequency whereas the former are not.

Thus, we believe that the lack of a significant effect in our

unmodulated DC data may be due to the greater difficulty in

controlling the noise with these measures than with AC measures.

Other labs may have applied a stricter control of these noise

sources than we did (such as taking measurements in the dark,

etc.). Note that although Franceschini and Boas (2004) reported

their measures as intensity effects, they used a low-frequency

modulation method (a few hundred Hz). Therefore, their measures

should be considered more similar to the AC than the DC intensity

measures reported in the current paper.

Given the significantly lower cost of instruments for intensity

measurement than of those for the measurement of phase delay, the

current results suggest that it may be a good economic strategy to

focus on the former rather than the latter. However, such a

conclusion may not be entirely appropriate. In fact, some of the

results of the current study suggest that phase delay may actually

be the measure of choice when the interest is in fast optical signals.

First, the signal-to-noise ratio (as expressed by the Z score) is

slightly larger for the phase signals. Second, AC intensity measures

appear to be more contaminated by slow effects due to oxygenation

changes than are the phase measures. Although both measures may

be sensitive to fast and slow effects, it appears that phase measures

are relatively more sensitive to fast effects and intensity measures

are relatively more sensitive to slow effects. Further, whereas fast

signals based on phase delay have been obtained from most areas

of the brain (such as frontal, e.g., DeSoto et al., 2001; parietal, e.g.,

Maclin et al., 2004; temporal, e.g., Rinne et al., 1999; and occipital

cortex, e.g., Gratton et al., 1995a,b), it remains to be established

whether variations in the anatomy of activated areas will influence

the effectiveness of intensity measures in revealing fast signals. For

instance, Maclin et al. (2004) showed that a 16-ms response in the

fast optical signal elicited by median nerve stimulation could be

observed with delay but not with AC intensity measures, whereas a

longer latency response to the same stimulation could be measured

with both parameters. Although more research is needed to

definitely confirm this hypothesis, it is possible that different

types of instruments may be better tuned for the detection of fast

and slow effects. Of course, multi-wavelength frequency–domain

instrumentation does allow the recording and comparison of

different signals in parallel and therefore provides more informa-

tion than continuous wave instrumentation. This multi-measure

approach would prove useful if it were shown that the various

measures are not redundant, and are differently sensitive to the

signal and/or to the noise. In the current study, it appears that delay

and intensity parameters are differentially sensitive to contamina-

Page 14: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–1590 1589

tion from slow/hemodynamic optical effects. Thus, in a multivar-

iate sense, a combination of the two measures may be useful.

It may at first appear surprising that the modulation signal is

more robust than the DC signal. However, this occurs because the

modulated light (AC) represents only a fraction of the total light

(DC). Hence, a stimulus-related change in modulated light

influences proportionally more the AC signal, and therefore the

modulation, than the DC signal. For the same reason, noise

influencing only the unmodulated portion of the light will also

have comparably less effect on the modulation parameter. Thus, the

modulation has a better S/N ratio than the unmodulated DC.

The results of the source-detector distance analysis indicate that

fast optical effects are best recorded using source-detector

distances exceeding 22.5 mm. This is consistent with the brain

origin of these effects. However, they also indicate that at distances

of 40–50 mm or more, the effects become very noisy and very few

channels can be used, at least with the methodology used in the

current study (although this number depends on the criterion used

to accept a channel and on other parameters used in the recording).

This provides some indication of the optimum source-detector

distances to be used to record these effects. The data also suggest

that the sign of the phase delay effect may actually depend on the

source-detector distance: at very short source-detector distances the

effect may actually invert. A bipolar effect of this type for the

phase delay parameter is predicted by theory of the photon wave

propagation (see Fishkin and Gratton, 1993) and by Monte Carlo

simulations (see Gratton et al., 1995b). In simple terms, this

phenomenon is due to the fact that the phase delay is related to the

weighted average of the travel time of photons following different

trajectories. Because of the physical arrangement used in our head

measurements (in which sources and detectors are located on the

same side of a bounded surface), photons traveling along

superficial paths tend to follow shorter paths than photons traveling

along deeper paths. Thus, the same scattering (or absorbing) object

located at a given depth may intersect the path of relatively slow

photons for short source-detector distances and of relatively fast

photons for long source-detector distances. This should produce

opposite-sign effects on the weighted average of the photon time-

of-flight, which is essentially what is measured by the phase delay

parameter. As a consequence, theory predicts a bimodal effect of

phase delay as a function of source-detector distance. This is

exactly what appears to occur in our study. However, when the

effect occurs at sufficient depth with respect to the source-detector

distance, the effect may occur consistently in the same direction

(increases in phase for increases in transparency, or vice versa for

reduction in transparency). Hence, for future studies, we advocate

using distances of at least 22.5 mm.

In addition to addressing issues about the characteristics of the

fast optical signal, the current study was also based on a number of

other methodological innovations. We used a larger subject sample

and a finer spatial sampling than any other published optical

reports from the occipital area. The spatial resolution may be

particularly useful because the 80-ms fast optical response, unlike

the corresponding slow hemodynamic activations, appears to be

relatively localized to small cortical areas. Another critical feature

of the current study was the use of an advanced method for co-

registering data from multiple subjects. This method involved

collecting individual structural MR data, co-registering optical and

MR data, performing Talairach transformations, and using surface

cortical features for further anatomical alignment. This complex

co-registration method is relatively similar to that used in other

imaging modalities but has been very rarely used in optical

imaging studies (e.g., Maclin et al., 2004) and never with such a

large sample.

Finally, the current study provides data about the biophysical

mechanisms responsible for the fast optical signal, supporting

the hypothesis that scattering changes are contributing to its

generation. This hypothesis was first postulated on the basis of

observations on in vitro preparations, both on isolated nerves

(Cohen et al., 1971) and on hippocampal slices (Frostig et al.,

1990). However, no data directly supporting this hypothesis

have been reported in humans thus far. Here we show that a

same-sign effect is observed using wavelengths shorter and

longer than the hemoglobin isosbestic point – a result consistent

with the scattering hypothesis and directly contrasting with the

most obvious alternative theory – that fast optical effects are

due to rapid deoxygenation effects. However, using more

wavelengths will be useful to further illustrate other possible

components contributing to these signals (such as volume

effects, etc.) by providing more details about the spectra of fast

optical signals.

The wavelength analysis has also consequences for the design

of instruments for the measurement of fast optical signals. Whereas

the scattering hypothesis would suggest that a device designed to

measure fast optical signals should use shorter wavelength (690

nm) rather than longer wavelength (830 nm) light sources, this

conclusion is in contrast with another observation. In fact, the noise

level is also greater at 690 nm, as shown by the larger error bars in

Fig. 9, as well as by the Z score values reported in Fig. 10. This is

likely due to the lower transparency of tissue to light at short (i.e.,

690 nm) wavelengths because of the higher absorption by

hemoglobin (particularly deoxy-hemoglobin) at these wavelengths.

This leads to a smaller number of photons reaching the detector

with 690 nm than with 830 nm light and a consequent lower signal-

to-noise ratio. For this reason, it appears that a longer wavelength,

rather than a shorter one, may be preferable for the recording of

fast signals.

In summary, the results of the current study indicate that fast

optical effects due to scattering changes occurring in the brain can

be measured non-invasively with different types of dependent

variables, including phase delay, AC intensity, and modulation.

They also show how refinement of the optical recording and

analysis techniques can contribute significant advancements to the

concurrent measurement of the localization and time course of

neuronal signals.

Acknowledgments

The work presented in this paper was supported by NIBIB grant

# EB002011 to G. Gratton and by NIA grant #AG21887 to M.

Fabiani. We are grateful to Dr. Ed Maclin for comments on an

earlier version of the manuscript.

References

Boas, D.A., Gaudette, T., Strangman, G., Cheng, X., Marota, J.J.A.,

Mandeville, J.B., 2001. The accuracy of near infrared spectroscopy and

imaging during focal changes in cerebral hemodynamics. NeuroImage

13, 76–90.

Choi, J., Wolf, M., Toronov, V., Wolf, U., Polzonetti, C., Hueber, D.,

Safonova, L.P., Gupta, R., Michalos, A., Mantulin, W., Gratton,

Page 15: Effects of measurement method, wavelength, and source ... · hemoglobin isosbestic point (800 nm—the point at which the absorption spectra of oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin cross over).

G. Gratton et al. / NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1576–15901590

E., 2004. Noninvasive determination of the optical properties of

adult brain: near-infrared spectroscopy approach. J. Biomed. Opt. 9,

221–229.

Cohen, L.B., Hille, B., Keynes, R.D., Landowne, D., Rojas, E., 1971.

Analysis of the potential-dependent changes in optical retardation in the

squid giant axon. J. Physiol. 218, 205–237.

DeSoto, M.C., Fabiani, M., Geary, D.L., Gratton, G., 2001. When in doubt,

do it both ways: brain evidence of the simultaneous activation of

conflicting responses in a spatial Stroop task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13,

523–536.

Fabiani, M., Low, K.A., Wee, E., Sable, J.J., Gratton, G., 2006. Reduced

suppression or labile memory? Mechanisms of inefficient filtering of

irrelevant information in older adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 184 (4),

637–650.

Fishkin, J., Gratton, E., 1993. Propagation of photon-density waves in

strongly scattering media containing an absorbing semi-infinite plane

bounded by a straight edge. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 10, 127–140.

Franceschini, M.A., Boas, D.A., 2004. Noninvasive measurement of

neuronal activity with near-infrared optical imaging. NeuroImage 21,

372–386.

Friston, K.J., Holmes, A.P., Worsley, K.J., Poline, J.-P., Frith, C.R.,

Frackowiack, R.S.J., 1995. Statistical parametric maps in function

neuroimaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2,

189–210.

Frostig, R.D., Lieke, E.E., Ts’o, D.Y., Grinvald, A., 1990. Cortical

functional architecture and local coupling between neuronal activity

and the microcirculation revealed by in vivo high-resolution optical

imaging of intrinsic signals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 87, 6082–6086.

Gratton, G., 1997. Attention and probability effects in the human occipital

cortex: an optical imaging study. NeuroReport 8, 1749–1753.

Gratton, G., Corballis, P.M., 1995. Removing the heart from the brain:

compensation for the pulse artifact in the photon migration signal.

Psychophysiology 32, 292–299.

Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., 2003. The event related optical signal (EROS) in

visual cortex: replicability, consistency, localization and resolution.

Psychophysiology 40, 561–571.

Gratton, G., Coles, M.G.H., Donchin, E., 1983. A new method for off-line

removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55,

468–484.

Gratton, E., Mantulin, W.W., van de Ven, M.J., Fishkin, J.B., Maris, M.B.,

Chance, B., 1990. The possibility of a near-infrared optical imaging

system using frequency–domain methods. Proc. III Intl. Conf. for

Peace through Mind/Brain. pp. 183–189.

Gratton, G., Corballis, P.M., Cho, E., Fabiani, M., Hood, D., 1995a. Shades

of gray matter: noninvasive optical images of human brain responses

during visual stimulation. Psychophysiology 32, 505–509.

Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Friedman, D., Franceschini, M.A., Fantini, S.,

Gratton, E., 1995b. Rapid changes of optical parameters in the human

brain during a tapping task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 446–456.

Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Corballis, P.M., Hood, D.C., Goodman-Wood,

M.R., Hirsch, J., Kim, K., Friedman, D., Gratton, E., 1997. Fast and

localized event-related optical signals (EROS) in the human occipital

cortex: comparison with the visual evoked potential and fMRI.

NeuroImage 6, 168–180.

Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Goodman-Wood, M.R., DeSoto, M.C., 1998.

Memory-driven processing in human medial occipital cortex: an event-

related optical signal (EROS) study. Psychophysiology 38, 348–351.

Gratton, G., Sarno, A.J., Maclin, E., Corballis, P.M., Fabiani, M., 2000.

Toward non-invasive 3-D imaging of the time course of cortical

activity: investigation of the depth of the event-related optical signal

(EROS). NeuroImage 11, 491–504.

Gratton, G., Goodman-Wood, M.R., Fabiani, M., 2001. Comparison of

neuronal and hemodynamic measure of the brain response to visual

stimulation: an optical imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 13, 13–25.

Hu, X., Le, T.H., Ugurbil, K., 1997. Evaluation of the early response in

fMRI in individual subjects using short stimulus duration. Magn.

Reson. Med. 37, 877–884.

Jones, M., Berwick, J., Johnson, D., Mayhew, J., 2001. Concurrent optical

imaging spectroscopy and laser-Doppler flowmetry: the relationship

between blood flow, oxygenation, and volume in rodent barrel cortex.

NeuroImage 13, 1002–1015.

Lindauer, U., Villringer, A., Dirnagl, U., 1993. Characterization of CBF

response to somatosensory stimulation: model and influence of

anesthetics. J. Physiol. 93, 1223–1228.

Low, K.A., Leaver, E., Kramer, A.F., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., 2006. Fast

optical imaging of frontal cortex during active and passive oddball

tasks. Psychophysiol. 43, 127–136.

Maclin, E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., 2003. Optimum filtering for EROS

measurements. Psychophysiology 40, 542–547.

Maclin, E.L., Low, K.A., Sable, J.J., Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., 2004. The

event related optical signal (EROS) to electrical stimulation of the

median nerve. NeuroImage 21, 1798–1804.

Rector, D.M., Poe, G.R., Kristensen, M.P., Harper, R.M., 1997. Light

scattering changes follow evoked potentials from hippocampal

Schaeffer collateral stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 1707–1713.

Rector, D.M., Carter, K.M., Volegov, P.L., George, J.S., 2005. Spatio-

temporal mapping of rat whisker barrels with fast scattered light signals.

NeuroImage 26, 619–627.

Rinne, T., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Cowan, N., Maclin, E., Stinard, A.,

Sinkkonen, J., Alho, K., Naatanen, R., 1999. Scalp-recorded optical

signals make sound processing from the auditory cortex visible.

NeuroImage 10, 620–624.

Steinbrink, J., Kohl, M., Obrig, H., Curio, G., Syre, F., Thomas, F.,

Wabnitz, H., Rinneberg, H., Villringer, A., 2000. Somatosensory

evoked fast optical intensity changes detected non-invasively in the

adult human head. Neurosci. Lett. 291, 105–108.

Syre, F., Obrig, H., Steinbrink, J., Kohl, M., Wenzel, R., Villringer, A.,

2003. Are VEP correlated fast optical signals detectable in the human

adult by non-invasive near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS)? Adv. Exp.

Med. Biol. 530, 421–431.

Tse, C.-Y., Tien, K.-R., Penney, T.B., 2006. Event-related optical

imaging reveals the temporal dynamics of right temporal and frontal

cortex activation in pre-attentive change detection. NeuroImage 29,

314–320.

Tse, C.-Y., Penney, T.B., in press. Optical imaging of cortical activity

elicited by unattended temporal deviants. IEEE EMBM.

Van der Tweel, L.H., Verduyn Lunel, H.F.E., 1965. Human visual responses

to sinusoidally modulated light. Electroencephalogr. Clinical Neuro-

physiol. 18, 587–598.

Vanzetta, I., Grinvald, A., 1999. Increased cortical oxidative metabolism

due to sensory stimulation: implications for functional brain imaging.

Science 286, 1555–1558.

Villringer, A., Chance, B., 1997. Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and

imaging of human brain function. Trends Neurosci. 20, 435–442.

Weber, B., Burger, C., Wyss, M.T., von Schulthess, G.K., Scheffold, F.,

Buck, A., 2004. Optical imaging of the spatiotemporal dynamics of

cerebral blood flow and oxidative metabolism in the rat barrel cortex.

Eur. J. Neurosci. 20 (10), 2664–2670.

Wolf, U., Wolf, M., Toronov, V., Michalos, A., Paunescu, L.A., Gratton, E.,

2000. Detecting cerebral functional slow and fast signals by frequency–

domain near-infrared spectroscopy using two different sensors.. OSA

Biomed. Top Meeting, Tech. Digest. pp. 427–429.

Wolf, M., Wolf, U., Choi, J.H., Gupta, R., Safonova, L.P., Paunescu, L.A.,

Michalos, A., Gratton, E., 2002. Functional frequency–domain near-

infrared spectroscopy detects fast neuronal signal in the motor cortex.

NeuroImage 17, 1868–1875.

Wolf, M., Wolf, U., Choi, J.H., Gupta, R., Safonova, L.P., Paunescu, L.A.,

Michalos, A., Gratton, E., 2003a. Detection of the fast neuronal signal

on the motor cortex using functional frequency domain near infrared

spectroscopy. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 510, 193–197.

Wolf, M., Wolf, U., Choi, J.H., Toronov, V., Paunescu, L.A., Michalos, A.,

Gratton, E., 2003b. Fast cerebral functional signal in the 100 ms range

detected in the visual cortex by frequency–domain near-infrared

spectrophotometry. Psychophysiology 40, 521–528.