EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text...

21
EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology I Text: Sousa, D. A. (Ed.), (2010). Mind, brain, & education: Neuroscience implications for the classroom. Table of Contents: Chpt. One: How science met technology Chpt. Two: Neuroimaging tools and the evolution of educational neuroscience Chpt. Three: The current impact of neuroscience on teaching and learning Chpt. Four: The role of emotion and skilled intuition in learning Chpt. Five: The Speaking brain Chpt. Six: The Reading brain Chpt. Seven: Constructing a reading brain Chpt. Eight: The mathematical brain Chpt. Nine: The calculating brain Chpt. Ten: The computing brain Chpt. Eleven: The creative-artistic brain Chpt. Twelve: The future of educational neuroscience Course Topics Introduction - Educational neuroscience is a legitimate scientific area of study that overlaps psychology, neuroscience and pedagogy. [See diagram – p. 2] [ See Fig. 1.2 and Fig 1.3 Diagrams of the Brain ] Chpt 1 – How Science Met Pedagogy A. Technology used to study the living brain: i. CAT Scan – 1970s ii. MRI – 1980s iii. PET Scan – 1970s iv. fMRI – 1990s

Transcript of EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text...

Page 1: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology I

Text: Sousa, D. A. (Ed.), (2010). Mind, brain, & education: Neuroscience implications for the

classroom.

Table of Contents:

Chpt. One: How science met technology

Chpt. Two: Neuroimaging tools and the evolution of educational neuroscience

Chpt. Three: The current impact of neuroscience on teaching and learning

Chpt. Four: The role of emotion and skilled intuition in learning

Chpt. Five: The Speaking brain

Chpt. Six: The Reading brain

Chpt. Seven: Constructing a reading brain

Chpt. Eight: The mathematical brain

Chpt. Nine: The calculating brain

Chpt. Ten: The computing brain

Chpt. Eleven: The creative-artistic brain

Chpt. Twelve: The future of educational neuroscience

Course Topics

Introduction - Educational neuroscience is a legitimate scientific area of study that overlaps

psychology, neuroscience and pedagogy. [See diagram – p. 2]

[ See Fig. 1.2 and Fig 1.3 Diagrams of the Brain ]

Chpt 1 – How Science Met Pedagogy

A. Technology used to study the living brain:

i. CAT Scan – 1970s

ii. MRI – 1980s

iii. PET Scan – 1970s

iv. fMRI – 1990s

Page 2: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

B. Professional Development

i. Learning Styles – Dunn & Dunn 1970s

ii. Multiple Intelligences – Gardner 1980s

iii. Triarchic theory of Intelligence – Sternberg 1980s

iv. Emotional Intelligence – Goleman 1990s

v. Neurogenesis – Kempermann & Gage 2000s

vi. Neuroplasticity – Shaywitz 2000s, Simos et al 2000s

vii. Memory Levels and Learning – Sousa 2000s

Page 3: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Chpt 2 - Neuroimaging – Evolution of Educational Neuroscience

A. We now understand that executive functions of the brain ‘connect’ activities among

various areas that deal with specific activities.

B. We now realize that at an early age (6-10 months), during language development,

phoneme discrimination becomes more language-specific (English, Mandarin). This

impacts a child’s capacity to learn a second language. NOTE: Interesting finding that

human social interactions impact phoneme discrimination differently than audio-video

recordings do.

C. Although most of the neural networks are common to all people, their efficiency varies,

partly due to genetic variations. The expression of these genetic variations is influenced by

experience. There is evidence that aspects of the culture in which children are raised can

influence the way in which genes shape neural networks – ultimately influencing child

behaviour. Example: The major development of the executive function occurs between four

and seven years of age. Training on conflict management during that period produced

improved conflict resolution skills as compared to other training techniques. Similarly,

working memory training tasks and meditation produced improved students’ attention in

classrooms where they were provided. This means that this type of attention training could

be beneficial for students who have poorer initial efficiency. [i.e – some forms of attention

can be taught.]

D. Studies in early education show that, with practice the connectivity between brain areas is

strengthened, and tasks can be carried out more efficiently.

Activity: Choose one of these statements and examine its potential impact on the teaching-learning process

in your classroom.

Page 4: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Chpt 3 - Impact of Neuroscience on Teaching and Learning

A. Proper learning behaviour is no longer defined by students sitting quietly, doing exactly what

they are told without question or discussion, and reporting back memorized facts on tests.

The work of Vygotsky on the zone of proximal development (ZPD) and Krashen on reducing

the negative effects of stress on learning and the practice of individualizing instruction are

supported by our current understanding of how the brain operates during learning

experiences.

B. We can learn a lot about motivation, intrinsic rewards, and ZPD from popular computer

games.

C. Krashen’s ‘affective filter’ helps us understand the neurological impact of stress and emotion

on brain functions during the learning process.

D. We cannot learn anything that is not recognized by our brains. The role of the reticular

activating system (RAS) is the basis upon which all our lessons should be planned.

E. Dopamine is a learning-friendly neurotransmitter. It promotes motivation, enhances memory,

and provides focus as well as making us feel good. Dopamine production can be activated by

certain environmental influences and teaching strategies.

F. Dopamine ‘drop’ occurs when a student experience the negative emotions related to making

a mistake. Effective and frequent formative assessments can reduce the fear of making

mistakes.

G. Neuroplasticity and pattern-based memory provide us with the basis to choose effective

teaching strategies. Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons

that will enhance student learning.

H. Intelligence is not a fixed capacity – it can be increased by making our brains’ neural networks

stronger, more efficient, accessible, and durable. Teachers who collaborate in ‘learning

communities’ can help students become more intelligent.

Activity: Choose one of these statements and examine its potential impact on the teaching-learning

process in your classroom.

Page 5: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Chpt 4 - Role of Emotion and Skilled Intuition in Learning

A. The message from social and affective neuroscience is clear: no longer can we think of

learning as separate from or disrupted by emotion… building academic knowledge involves

integrating emotion and cognition in social context.

B. The learners’ emotional reaction to the outcome of their efforts consciously or nonconsciously

shapes their future behaviour. Therefore efficient learners build useful and relevant intuitions

that guide their thinking and decision making. These intuitions are not randomly generated,

they are shaped and organized by experience and are specific and relevant to the particular

context in which they were learned.

C. Relevant intuitions and emotional learning is enhanced when teachers foster emotional

connection to the material students are learning. One way is to offer students a choice of how

they will learn the material (writing/performing a play, doing a research report, designing a

model). Another is to assign open-ended problems that create space for emotional reactions.

D. Intuition can be understood as the incorporation of the nonconscious emotional signals into

knowledge acquisition. Building curricular opportunities for students to develop skilled

intuitions is therefore a meta-learning process.

E. We must actively manage the social and emotional climate of the classroom. Students will

allow themselves to experience failure only if they can do so within an atmosphere of trust

and respect.

F. Critical thinking requires students to use intuition and emotional signals to know how, when,

and why to use the new knowledge they have acquired.

Page 6: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Chpt 5 - The Speaking Brain

Much of what we often believe is true about how the brain enables human speech was discovered in

the mid 1800’s using negative reasoning (i.e. - If a part of the brain was injured and the patient lost

an ability/function then that part of the brain was the part responsible for that function).

Pierre Broca – 1865 Broca’s area enables us to speak.

Carl Wernicke – 1875 Wernicke’s area enables us to understand language.

Also on the left side of the brain we find the arcuate fasciculus, a large bundle of nerves that connects

Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas, and makes it possible for us to communicate using language. The direct

and synchronized connection between these two areas makes rapid back and forth conversation

possible. Broca’s area also seems to be involved in some semantic and working memory processes –

providing coordination and integration or neural information from other language processing areas of

the brain (see Fig. 1)

Figure 1. – Process Functions and Locations of the Brain

Brain Area

Process

Right middle and superior temporal Understanding semantics

Bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal Monitoring coherence

Left inferior frontal-left anterior temporal Integrating text

Bilateral medial frontal/posterior right temporal/parietal

Interpreting the perspective of the agent or actor

Left dominant, bilateral intraparietal sulcus Imaging spatial information

Prefrontal cortex, parietal lobes Working memory – holding language while other processes are performed

Medial temporal lobe, prefrontal regions, parietal lobe

Episodic memory – recalling an experience

Temporal and frontal lobes Word processing & grammatical processing

Lateral dominance of the brain’s left hemisphere for language processing has been supported by

modern structural and functional image studies. For most people the left side of the brain controls

language (96% of right handed and 76% of left handed people).

The knowledge of and competence for human language is acquired through

various means and modality types. Linguists regard speaking, signing, and

language comprehension as primary faculties of language, i.e., innate or inherent

Page 7: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

and biologically determined, whereas they regard reading and writing as

secondary abilities. Indeed, the native or first language (L1) is acquired during the

first years of life through such primary faculties while children are rapidly

expanding their linguistic knowledge (2). In contrast, reading and writing are

learned with much conscious effort and repetition, usually at school.

Speech in infants develops from babbling at around 6 to 8 months of age, to the

one-word stage at 10 to 12 months, and then to the two-word stage around 2

years. Note that sign systems are spontaneously acquired by both deaf and

hearing infants in a similar developmental course. Speech perception and even

grammatical knowledge develops much earlier, within the first months after birth.

(Sakai, 2005)1

At school age typically developing children were assumed to have a fully developed spoken language

system that could serve as the basis for learning to read and write. This being said, speech and

language processing are only part of the cognitive demands placed on the brain when children are in

school. If cognitive resources are being spent on other competing processes, children may not have

optimal capacity to understand and produce spoken language (e.g. – listening to the teacher and

making notes at the same time OR processing feelings of fear or sadness while being expected to

speak.)

Language and the Right Hemisphere

Although the left hemisphere is the dominant one for language, the right hemisphere is also involved.

It has long been seen as responsible for understanding and producing prosody, the intonational and

emotional aspects of spoken language. While prosodic elements contribute essential information to

verbal communication, they are not considered to be language in the same sense as phonological

(related to the sounds of speech), semantic (related to the meaning of words) and syntactical (related

to the grammatical arrangement of words) elements of language. The right hemisphere is also

involved in interpreting humor and metaphors, making inferences, understanding sarcasm or irony,

and comprehending discourse. The right hemisphere may also assist in understanding more

demanding semantic tasks such as when words are only distantly related, being used to imply a

meaning other than the literal ones, or have two very different meanings. This makes the right

hemisphere essential when we draw inferences from our experiences. While language is

predominantly left hemisphere function, both hemispheres are necessary for a fully functioning,

flexible spoken language system. Our ability to examine the brain using fMRI technology is also

challenging previously believed knowledge on how speech and language develop.

Speech and Language Development

1 Sakai, K. L (2005) Language Acquisition and Brain Development , downloaded on Dec 22,2014 from www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 310 4 NOVEMBER 2005

Page 8: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Behavioural information gathered in the past led us to believe that, by school age, children were able

to clearly produce most speech sounds, had mastered basic grammatical aspects of spoken language,

and acquired a sufficient lexicon to talk about various concrete and abstract experiences. Having

learned language, they were now ready to use language as a vehicle to expand their learning across

the curriculum.

We now know that behaviour measures are not exact measures of brain function, and may even lead

to incorrect conclusions about brain function. We know that not all children come to school with fully

developed speech and language, ready to use these skills for reading, writing, and oral expression.

Even older children vary in their capacity to understand and use higher order or more abstract

language. During school years children are exposed to experiences that change the brain’s structures

and functions. The brain develops in response to the environmental input it receives. Although

genetics plays an important role, there are differences in the rate at which the development of brain

processes occurs.

We also know that even if children are behaviourally performing in the same way as adults, they have

different neural patterns that may reflect the use of different cognitive strategies. Children’s brains

work harder than adult’s brains to accomplish the same behavioural result. This is especially true of

boys because it appears that boys do not convert sensory information to language as easily as girls

do. Not all performance is gender based though, language skills are determined by a child’s genetic

make-up and the amount of time and effort spent on practice and development of those skills. Not all

children are capable of the same level of verbal expression; some are verbally fluent while others

struggle to put their thoughts into words.

Differences in language skills are not related to innate intelligence or motivation; rather they are

related to individual differences in brain development. Being slower does not necessarily mean that a

child knows or understands less; it simply means that the child needs more time to express what he

or she knows.

Bilingualism

Mastering two languages has traditionally been seen as an age-related issue. Behavioural studies

showed that young children who were exposed to two languages before the age of seven developed

proficiency in both languages. Brain imaging studies now show that bilingual adults who were

exposed to two languages before age five actually process their languages in overlapping brain areas

– the same areas that monolingual children use. Bilinguals who learn their second language later

appear to use different strategies. Their brains function in a more bilateral manner with more

distributive activation in the frontal lobe, in areas thought to represent working memory and

inhibitory processes. This pattern of activation is thought to be consistent with greater cognitive

effort and less automatic processing. It seems that the most efficient use of neural resources occurs

when language learning happens early.

Neuroplasticity

Page 9: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Previous beliefs about the brain being fully formed at birth have been proven incorrect. Both the

structures and functions of brain cells have been proven to change during one’s life. This plasticity is

greatest in the earliest years of life. Even in adolescence language networks interact with other

cortical resources, such as memory, and thereby change the brain’s structures and functions. New

neurons are created in the hippocampus, a process that impacts the formation of memories. The

frontal lobe continues to develop thorough early adulthood, making it possible for adolescents to

develop metacognitive and metalinguistic skills. This enables adolescents to think more abstractly

and to communicate and think more flexibly and creatively. Adolescence is when sophisticated forms

of communication and language use can develop.

Research Findings to Consider

The acquisition and refinement of speech and language is ongoing until early adulthood.

School-age children do not have fully developed language systems.

Children are less efficient language processors than adults.

Gender differences in language processing have been observed.

Boys may have more difficulty with verbal expression, and how information is presented may

make a greater difference in their ability to learn.

The brain learns a second language most easily before school age.

During school years, children’s brains continue to mature and develop with both age and new

experiences with language.

Children may not be able to coordinate ‘listening to language’ and ‘writing language’.

Simply because a child can behaviourally perform a task does not mean that the brain is

efficiently performing that task.

Language skills can vary widely in groups of same-age children.

Spoken language is not the only means to determine whether a child understands a concept.

Language is not simply a tool which children apply to the learning process. It is a growing, changing

skill.

Page 10: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Chpt 6 - The Reading Brain

In today’s world reading is our most powerful portal to knowledge. Formal education reinforces this

first by focusing on children’s need to ‘learn to read’ and then upon their need to use ‘reading to

learn’. In many ways the learning to read/reading to learn link sets the stage for most measures of

children’s success for the remainder of their lives.

Unlike hearing, speaking, and basic motor skills, reading is a relatively recent cultural invention. Most

historians/archeologists date the origins of written records to about 4000 BCE, with the creation of

syllables around 2600 BCE and finally the first alphabets around 1800 BCE. It wasn’t until when

Socrates (469 - 399 BCE) begrudgingly accepted the written word as a necessary supplement to the

oral tradition of teaching that learning from texts became well established. Even then he argued

‘that a dependence on the technology of the alphabet will alter a person’s mind,

and not for the better. By substituting outer symbols for inner memories, writing

threatens to make us shallower thinkers, preventing us from achieving the

intellectual depth that leads to wisdom and true happiness.’

Nicholas Carr (2010)2

As we contemplate Socrates’ prediction, and Carr’s fear about how the internet exacerbates our

dependence upon ‘reading to learn’ we can take some solace in the fact that neuroscience offers

us better ways to understand the impact that the alphabet has had on altering our brains. The fact

that the human brain was not evolved to read may explain why reading is not a naturally acquired

skill and therefore must be taught explicitly and formally in schools. Once again we see that the

combination of behavioural and neurological studies provide us with knowledge that guide our

teaching.

Two Routes for Reading

We begin this task with an understanding that reading and writing are extensions of language,

which is hard wired in our brains. We know that language is mainly a left hemisphere function that

is guided by right hemisphere support. To this we add our understanding of how the brain uses

short term memory to convert written symbols into the sounds and meanings that enable

communication and learning. Reading begins with the visual input from the ‘page’ which triggers

the left posterior portion of the brain known as the visual word form area. This area relates visual

(occipital lobe) and language (temporal lobe) neural systems that develop even before birth and far

before learning to read. It does so by transforming visual input into letters and words. These letters

and words are then transformed into meaning using two separate processes (routes).

The phonological route decodes a string of letters and translates them into a sound pattern that

may match a speech pattern which is meaningful. This route is reserved for words that are regular

2 Carr, Nicholas, (2010). The shallows : what the Internet is doing to our brains

Page 11: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

i.e. – that follow the typical correspondence between graphemes and phonemes. If a word is

irregular (rare or novel) we use the phonological route try to sound it out. This is a relatively slow,

systematic route that relies on the left posterior temporoparietal brain regions.

The second process is the direct route and it by-passes the sound pattern stage and attempts to

match the printed word directly with its meaning. The words we read using the direct route are

‘sight words’, ones that we frequently encounter and know so well that we can jump from sight to

meaning. We also use this route for irregular words that we have memorized because regular

pronunciation rules do not apply. This route, which lies on the left posterior occipitotemporal

region, tends to be must faster – in typical adult readers it responds to a word in about 180

milliseconds but only when the word is in the written language that one as learned.

The typical healthy reader is thought to use both routes constantly and interactively. As we learn to

read it seems logical that we would use the direct route more often if the words become part of

our ‘sight word’ complement. Thus the selective response of the visual word form area is education

dependent. This could explain why, for healthy children, reading becomes easier with more

practice.

Timeline of Reading in the Brain

It is important to note that visual recognition (seeing) differs based upon what we are looking at.

We are genetically predisposed to perceive faces and places much more so than words. This means

that the areas that enable us to recognize faces and places are located in relatively fixed places in

the brain, while the word form area is less precisely located. This supports the belief that

specialization for perceiving faces and places is genetically guided, whereas specialization for

letters and words is not. It is possible that human evolution has caused areas within the neocortex

that are devoted to the perception of objects to become specialized for recognizing faces and

places and that the word form area is still evolving as a result of neuronal recycling. If so, we may

not have to teach reading in a few thousand more years!

The process of making sense of a visual stimulus is sometimes referred to as the N1 response3. This

process occurs whenever a person sees a face, place, or word. In typical adult readers the response

is left lateralized for words and right lateralized for faces. When an adult sees a word the response

time is typically 400-500 msecs between perceiving a word and recognizing it. For skilled adult

readers this response time reduces to 150-200 msecs. During these periods of time higher-order

cognitive processing also occurs, processing that extracts sound and meaning from the printed

word. Interestingly, these higher-order processes do not wait for visual analysis to be completed.

Tihs may be why we can raed wrods that have all the crroect letrtes but in the worng odrer.

3 The N1 response is an event related potential (ERP) used in cognitive neuroscience to study the physiological responses to sensory stimulus when the brain processes information. The N means the wave response is negative and the 1 means it occurs about 100 msec after the stimulation.

Page 12: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Cross-Linguistic Differences

Orthographical transparency is one way of describing language. This is a measure of how much a

single letter or group of letters (grapheme) represent a single sound (phoneme). Italian and Spanish

have nearly a 1:1 relationship which means the spelling of a word enables its correct pronunciation. If

you can spell a word, you can pronounce it and vice versa. Languages like English have many

exceptions and therefore have poor transparency. In English there are nearly thirty pronunciations

for every grapheme! Some languages such as Japanese and Cherokee use syllables rather than letters

to form written words [e.g. – In Japanese, San means three, Dan means degree, so Sandan means

third degree. Likewise the term for foreigner, Gaijin is a combination of gai (outside) and jin

(person)]. Other languages, like Chinese, represent words with symbols and are referred to as

logographic systems. These languages contain many symbols and many can be interpreted in multiple

ways. For example, the Chinese word for foreigner is

鬼佬 or 老外 where the symbols 老 (lǎo, "old, always") + 外 (wài, "outlander, foreign")

老外4 can also mean ‘foreign devil’ which is an insult, or ‘ghost man’ which alludes to white skinned

complexion of many foreign visitors.

Development of the Reading Brain

As the reading brain develops it changes by: 1. increasing its specialization of the left hemisphere, 2.

decreasing its use of the left anterior area, and 3. increasing its use of left posterior. The increased

specialization of the left hemisphere is likely due to the brain’s increased ability to recognize a wide

variety of symbols as the same letter. The multiple visuospatial patterns of the various ways of

representing each letter of the alphabet are transformed into twenty-six categories of the English

alphabet. The shift from the use of the anterior to the posterior may be due to the shift from

phonological decoding to automatic word recognition. This means that the working memory

processes of the left anterior area are no longer required because the direct route is capable of

processing a word quickly. As the posterior region matures it supports fluent, automatic reading.

At the same time that these changes occur, the brain increases it capacity to respond to printed

stimuli as compared to other visual inputs. In non-reading kindergartners the N1 word response shifts

from zero toward the typical 150 – 200 msecs of a high functioning adult reader. The larger the

difference between N1 word response and N1 symbol response the faster the children read. This

integration between print and sound continues to develop for many years.

Reading Difficulty in the Brain – Developmental Dyslexia

Dyslexia, the most commonly identified reading disability, is defined as difficulty in reading or spelling

words accurately and/or fluently given average or higher than average cognitive ability. This is

associated with a weakness in phonological processing skills. It is a heterogeneous disorder that may

4 Pronounced kwai lo

Page 13: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

result from a variety of specific underlying difficulties that vary from child to child, including specific

deficits in automaticity or auditory and visual perception.

Dyslexic children consistently exhibit decreased or absent activations in the left posterior brain

regions when performing tasks that require phonological or orthographic processing, when compared

to reading-matched and age-matched readers. Readers with dyslexia often exhibit increased

activation in frontal brain regions and the right-hemisphere posterior regions. This may be due to a

compensation for weaker posterior reading networks. Frontal activations for these readers do not

differ from reading matched children. Adolescents with dyslexia who improve or compensate appear

to do so by exploiting this atypical use of frontal-lobe regions rather than by the development of left

posterior reading systems. Children with dyslexia show less word-specific response to print (N1) and

less left hemisphere lateralization than non-dyslexic readers. They do not respond to words

differently than symbols.

Structural Brain Differences that Reflect Functional Brain Differences

The brain consists of two types of matter – gray matter and white matter.

Gray matter is composed of neuronal cell bodies while white matter is composed of myelinated axon

tracts. Readers with dyslexia show less gray matter volume in several regions associated with reading.

Even when compared to younger reading-matched children, readers with dyslexia show less gray

matter in the left hemisphere temporoparietal region in which they show reduced activation. Thus

there is some correspondence between functional and structural brain differences in dyslexia. Better

organized white matter in the left posterior region is associated with better reading skill among

healthy individuals. White matter tracts in the left frontal regions also reflect weaker connections in

readers with dyslexia. These individuals also have greater than normal white matter connectivity in

the corpus callosum, which connects regions of the left and right hemispheres. These findings suggest

Page 14: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

that, in dyslexia, white matter pathways supporting reading project too weakly within the primary

reading pathways of the linguistic left hemisphere, but they project too strongly between

hemispheres (which may reflect an atypical reliance on right hemisphere regions for reading).

How Intervention Affects the Struggling Reader’s Brain

Reading interventions can change the structure and function of the brain. Due to brain plasticity left-

hemisphere brain regions that are typically under activated in dyslexia exhibit a gain in activation

after effective intervention (Lindamood-Bell program for adults, FastForWord for children). Children

with dyslexia who had under activated left temporoparietal and frontal brain regions showed gains in

activation in those regions after effective remediation. Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing program

and Phon-Graphix interventions resulted in a shift from greater activation in the right hemisphere to

greater left hemisphere activation and normalization of white matter structure. Effective

interventions with dyslexic readers can also strengthen activation in brain regions not typically

engaged in reading. Effective interventions may act in two ways – normalization of the brain and

brain compensation. These affects can be long lasting.

Prevention or early treatment of dyslexia yields better outcomes that later treatment. Neuroscience

methods have shown surprising strength in predicting future reading difficulty. A near-term goal

could be the prediction and prevention of dyslexia. In general, brain imaging combined with familial

information may facilitate preventive interventions that allow more children to succeed at learning to

read.

Chpt 7 - Constructing a Reading Brain

Expectations for neuroscience-based easy-to-follow recipes for classroom practice are unrealistic but

in combination with what we know from cognitive, developmental, and other learning sciences,

neuroscience can provide a new perspective on education.

There is no single part of the brain that ‘does’ reading. The brain is simply not designed for reading.

As we learn to read, we are borrowing from and building upon multiple neural systems, each with

their own specialization and actually constructing a brain that can read. Learning to read involves the

development of several constituent systems and then connecting those systems so that they work in

concert automatically and fluently. This process takes years, beginning before formal schooling and

extending throughout the school years.

Page 15: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

The key systems that are required to read include the orthographic system that enables the visual

processing of text, the phonological system used to process the sounds of language, and the semantic

system used to connect meaning to words.

Visual Processing: Orthography

The first task for a reader is to make sense of the marks on a page. This begins with recognizing the

orthographic symbols of the language (for us the Roman alphabet) – a task that is difficult because

the symbols are arbitrary, abstract and sometimes easily confused. Distinguishing between letters

requires a highly sensitive visual system. The occipital lobes in the visual cortex enable us to identify

lines, curves, angles, terminals and junctions that form written symbols. These visual elements are

combined to create letters and words that we see on the page. In order to read we must make

meaning of the patterns that we see.

ɡet RAY away

English small letters for the verb move Capital letters for your instructor’s first name Small letters for a place that is at a distance Two Chinese symbols for Ai (the English word ‘love’ ) Chinese symbols for Wǒ and Xiànzài5 (the English words ‘Me’ and ‘Now’)

As we ‘read’ each of these patterns the pattern that we ‘see’ is processed by two separate regions of

the brain. One pathway (ventral pathway) determines ‘what’ is being seen and transfers the

information to the temporal lobes. The ventral pathway is specialized for processing colour, form,

texture, patterns, and fine detail. Learning to read appears to involve adapting and specializing the

ventral visual stream through practice with printed words.

The other (dorsal pathway) determines ‘where’ we are seeing the symbol, which allows us to place

what we in some order. (i.e. - to place parts of a symbol, letter or word in order from left to right or

up to down. The dorsal pathway enables a reader to move across the page in a complicated pattern

of fixations (periods of relative stillness) and saccades (brief jumps across the text). The dorsal stream

is involved in controlling eye movements and dorsal stream deficits have been associated with

reading difficulties. Activation of the dorsal stream is much reduced in adults with dyslexia. Given that

5 Pronounced who wa and shia si

爱 我

Page 16: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

English is read from left to right and Chinese from top to bottom we can see that both pathways are

essential for making meaning of the symbols.

Building a brain that reads involves developing these two pathways (streams). Therefore children

who are learning how to read are truly changing their brains.

Page 17: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

The fusiform gyrus is a structure that runs along the base of the brain and contains a sub-region that

has become known as the word form area. This area is activated by stimuli that are word-like (i.e. –

for us this means they follow the rules of English language). If removed surgically the individual loses

their ability to read. The fusiform gyrus enables us to recognize faces, a skill that far precedes reading

skills. The current belief is that a portion of this part of the brain has become specialized for word

processing. This specialization progresses over time and with experience with words. It is absent in

kindergartners, present in second graders, and continues to develop through adolescence. Its

development is correlated with the recognition of familiar letter patterns (decoding automaticity)

which is an essential skill in reading. Adults who are not experts at reading (particularly those with

dyslexia) show no activation of this area during reading.

Auditory Processing Phonology

Phonology involves the sound system of a language. Phonological processing systems related to

rhymes develop early. In fact the region associated with speech processing, the superior temporal

sulcus is sensitive to speech very early in the course of typical development. This area is used for

both spoken and written language processing which has been proven by studies that show that silent

reading activates this region.

Activities that emphasize the sound structure of language help to develop phonological awareness.

This is important because phonological awareness appears to be positively correlated with reading

skill throughout school. This may be explained by the “Matthew Effect’ which suggests that lack of

ability cause lack of performance/participation, which then leads to lower performance compared to

those who particpate at higher levels.

The connection between speech and reading is supported by the belief that learning to read, by

changing the phonological processing systems changes the way speech is anlayzed and phonemes are

remembered. As a result of reading, whole word sounds are automatically broken up into sound

constituents thereby changing language processing. The bonus is that by keeping track of phoneme

constituents a reader is can remember novel word sounds more accurately.

Connectivity: Mapping Orthography to Phonology

The two best predictors of reading achievement in early elementary school are letter identification

(knowing graphemes)and phonological awareness (knowing phonemes). These two sets of knowledge

map onto one another. Learnin g these mappings is a skill we call decoding, a skill that is required for

a child to learn to read. Even this step is difficult when learning to read in English because the English

language has a ‘deep orthography’; the mapping of grapheme to phoneme is not one-to-one like it is

in some languages. This is also why it is difficult to learn how to spell in English. In fact, studies show

that there are two areas of the brain responsible for this task. For easy, familiar orthgrapic-to-

phonological mappings the posterior regions are activated, while more difficult mappings rely on

more anterior regions. Other differences in mapping indicate that adults and older children activate

Page 18: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

regions associated with automaticity while younger children do not. Furthermore, children who lack

activation in the posterior regions exhibit reading disability. Fortunately, we find that this activation

can be increased through the use of phonologically-based interventions that focus on letter-sound

mappings and this improves reading ability. It is important to realize that the activation of individual

regions is not enough to learn to read well. These regions are part of a more complex interconnected

system and the dynamics of the system may either enable us to read well or experience dyslexia.

Uncoordinated processing may be characteristic of poor reading.

Meaning Processing: Semantics

Studies of family environments provide us with valuable information for understanding and assisting

poor readers. By age three, 86 to 98 percent of a child’s vocabulary consists of words in his or her

caregivers’ vocabulary. Children who grow up in low-income households in which they are not spoken

to extensively and are not exposed to a variety of words begin school with many fewer words than

their peers from higher-income households. This may be related to the finding that children in

professional families hear eleven million words per year while those in families receiving welfare hear

only three million. Alarmingly, the vocabulary gap created from this difference remained for years

later (Matthew Effect ?)

The development of speech results from hearing words and developing a spoken-vocabulary which

we refer to as lexicon. This lexicon enables children to make meaning not only when speaking but

also when reading. If a word grapheme activates a phoneme that is common in spoken-vocabulary it

triggers the activation of other words that the brain has connected to that phoneme. When this

happens the reader goes from decoding a word to developing a robust meaning of the word in

context to other words. The capacity to relate written words to spoken words as well as other written

words depends upon the same brain regions. Since spoken and written lexicon are interconnected, a

child expands both speaking and reading at the same time. In fact, by the time a child is in third

grade, there is a shift from spoken to written language as the source of most new entries in his or her

lexicon.

Semantics is the development of meaning of words or phrases in our vocabulary. Understanding

semantics is therefore very valuable when teaching a child to read. It appears as if the lexical

information that enables semantics is stored in a distributive fashion throughout the brain and the

locations depend upon a variety of elements associated with words (action-orientation, kinesthetic,

tactile, visual, auditory, orthographic, and phonological). When a word is read our brain associates it

with each of the elements that it has attached to the word – how it feels, looks, smells, moves, etc..

This belief about how lexical information is stored supports the current beliefs of the value of multi-

sensory instruction. Although it is distributed throughout the brain vocabulary knowledge appears to

be organized into semantic networks. Words that are conceptually related to one another are linked

into semantic networks. The richer your vocabulary, the richer your semantic networks, the more

connections you can make among lexical items, and the fuller your understanding of what you are

reading.

Page 19: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Vocabulary and conceptual knowledge are also organized in terms of schemata. A schema is an

organized way of perceiving cognitively and responding to a complex situation or set of stimuli – a

way that the brain connects perceptions/experiences that have something in common. Activation of

one stimuli (seeing the word ‘restaurant’) activates the schemata that contain the word and thereby

enable the reader to put the word in context and provide it with greater meaning. Schemata not only

expand our understanding of a word, they enable us to predict related words that may follow as we

read a passage. If readers do not have densely developed semantic networks and rich schemata, their

strategies for determining meaning when they read are limited.

Making Sense of It All: Comprehension

Learning how to decode and the development of vocabulary are necessary but they do not constitute

comprehension. This difference was clear to Benjamin Bloom who defined knowledge

as observation or recall both of which fall below comprehension (understanding) on his taxonomy. In

fact if we examine the definition of the word ‘read’ we see that it is ‘to look at and comprehend the

meaning of (written or printed matter) by mentally interpreting the characters or symbols of which it

is composed’.

Reading is an active, interactive and thoughtful dynamic transaction between the reader and the text

that consists of both a process and a product. The processes include critical analysis, making

inferences based on the use of prior knowledge, generating and answering questions, imaging,

predicting, clarifying and summarizing. This depth of reading can be enhanced by teachers who focus

on ‘dialogic reading’6. Reading for comprehension not only activates more of the regions of the brain

than reading without comprehension (simply looking at the words) it actually activates another

region (medial ventral orbitofrontal cortex) which is associated with our brain’s reward process.

Reading for comprehension is perceived by the brain as a rewarding experience.

6 Arnold, Lonigan, Whitehurst, & Epstein (1994), Accelerating language development through picture book reading.

Page 20: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that
Page 21: EDUC 5953 - Educational Psychology Ipeople.stu.ca/~raywilliams/Ed Psych 5953/Ed Psych Text notes.pdf · Understanding these two concepts is fundamental when planning lessons that

Chpt 8 - The Mathematical Brain

Chpt 9 - The Calculating Brain

Chpt 10 - The Computing Brain

Chpt 11 - The Creative-Artistic Brain

Chpt 12 - The Future of Educational Neuroscience