Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits...

8
PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased Creative Achievement in High-Functioning Individuals Shelley H. Carson Harvard University Jordan B. Peterson University of Toronto Daniel M. Higgins Harvard University Reductions in latent inhibition (LI), the capacity to screen from conscious awareness stimuli previously experienced as irrelevant, have been generally associated with the tendency towards psychosis. However, “failure” to screen out previously irrelevant stimuli might also hypothetically contribute to original thinking, particularly in combination with high IQ. Meta-analysis of two studies, conducted on youthful high-IQ samples, demonstrated that high lifetime creative achievers had significantly lower LI scores than low creative achievers (r effect size .31, p .0003, one-tailed). Eminent creative achievers (participants under 21 years who reported unusually high scores in a single domain of creative achievement) were 7 times more likely to have low rather than high LI scores, 2 (1, N 25) 10.69, .47, p .003. How do you know but ev’ry Bird that cuts the airy way, Is an immense world of delight, clos’d by your senses five? —William Blake, “A Memorable Fancy,” The Prophetic Books Creative individuals appear characterized in part by the ability to perceive and describe what remains hidden from the view of others. Individual variation in latent inhibition (LI), a cognitive inhibitory mechanism discovered by animal experimentalists in the late 1950s, may account for the apparent revelation to the creative mind of what appears “clos’d by the senses five” to others. LI refers to the varying capacity of the brain to screen from current attentional focus stimuli previously experienced as irrelevant (Lubow, 1989). The LI phenomenon appears robust across a variety of mammalian species, and its biological underpinnings have been extensively studied (Lubow & Gewirtz, 1995). In hu- mans, reduced LI has generally been associated with susceptibility to or actual acute-phase schizophrenia (Baruch, Hemsley, & Gray, 1988a, 1988b; Lubow, Ingberg-Sachs, Zalstein-Orda, & Gewirtz, 1992). Recent evidence has suggested, however, that reductions in LI are also associated with the personality trait Openness to Experience (Peterson & Carson, 2000; Peterson, Smith, & Carson, 2002). Openness, in turn, has been consistently associated with divergent thinking and trait creativity (McCrae, 1987) and with creative achievement (King, Walker, & Broyles, 1996). It there- fore appears possible that reductions in LI may be associated with increases in human creativity, as suggested by Eysenck (1995). Many researchers (e.g., Simonton, 1988, 1999) have proposed that the cognitive processes of individuals capable of creating the highest achievements in their fields are both qualitatively and quantitatively different from those of normal thinkers (although some, like Weisberg, 1993, dispute the “qualitative” distinction). If qualitative differences do exist, however, one potential source of difference in the cognitive processes between eminent creative achievers and other intelligent thinkers may be in the relative attenuation of LI. Such attenuation could well increase the number of available mental elements, described by Simonton (1988) as key, in part, to the process of creative discovery. Other avenues of research have buttressed this hypothesis. Dykes and McGhie (1976) demonstrated, for example, that cre- ative subjects and schizophrenic subjects were better than controls at identifying items presented on the irrelevant channel of a di- chotic shadowing task. This finding appears to support Dellas and Gaier’s (1970) observation that creative individuals tend not to screen out so-called “irrelevant details.” Furthermore, in a study conducted by Martindale, Anderson, Moore, and West (1996), a Shelley H. Carson and Daniel M. Higgins, Department of Psychology, Harvard University; Jordan B. Peterson, Department of Psychology, Uni- versity of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. We thank Melanie Glickson for her research assistance in this study. We also thank Robert Sternberg for his helpful comments. This research was supported by grants from the Harvard University Department of Psychol- ogy and the University of Toronto Department of Psychology. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shelley H. Carson, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138. E-mail: [email protected] Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, Vol. 85, No. 3, 499 –506 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499 499

Transcript of Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits...

Page 1: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES

Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased CreativeAchievement in High-Functioning Individuals

Shelley H. CarsonHarvard University

Jordan B. PetersonUniversity of Toronto

Daniel M. HigginsHarvard University

Reductions inlatent inhibition(LI), the capacity to screen from conscious awareness stimuli previouslyexperienced as irrelevant, have been generally associated with the tendency towards psychosis. However,“failure” to screen out previously irrelevant stimuli might also hypothetically contribute to originalthinking, particularly in combination with high IQ. Meta-analysis of two studies, conducted on youthfulhigh-IQ samples, demonstrated that high lifetime creative achievers had significantly lower LI scoresthan low creative achievers (reffect size � .31, p � .0003, one-tailed).Eminent creative achievers(participants under 21 years who reported unusually high scores in a single domain of creativeachievement) were 7 times more likely to have low rather than high LI scores,�2(1, N � 25)� 10.69,� � .47,p � .003.

How do you know but ev’ry Bird that cuts the airy way,Is an immense world of delight, clos’d by your senses five?

—William Blake, “A Memorable Fancy,”The Prophetic Books

Creative individuals appear characterized in part by the abilityto perceive and describe what remains hidden from the view ofothers. Individual variation inlatent inhibition (LI), a cognitiveinhibitory mechanism discovered by animal experimentalists in thelate 1950s, may account for the apparent revelation to the creativemind of what appears “clos’d by the senses five” to others. LIrefers to the varying capacity of the brain to screen from currentattentional focus stimuli previously experienced as irrelevant(Lubow, 1989). The LI phenomenon appears robust across avariety of mammalian species, and its biological underpinningshave been extensively studied (Lubow & Gewirtz, 1995). In hu-mans, reduced LI has generally been associated with susceptibilityto or actual acute-phase schizophrenia (Baruch, Hemsley, & Gray,

1988a, 1988b; Lubow, Ingberg-Sachs, Zalstein-Orda, & Gewirtz,1992). Recent evidence has suggested, however, that reductions inLI are also associated with the personality trait Openness toExperience (Peterson & Carson, 2000; Peterson, Smith, & Carson,2002). Openness, in turn, has been consistently associated withdivergent thinking and trait creativity (McCrae, 1987) and withcreative achievement (King, Walker, & Broyles, 1996). It there-fore appears possible that reductions in LI may be associated withincreases in human creativity, as suggested by Eysenck (1995).Many researchers (e.g., Simonton, 1988, 1999) have proposed

that the cognitive processes of individuals capable of creating thehighest achievements in their fields are both qualitatively andquantitatively different from those of normal thinkers (althoughsome, like Weisberg, 1993, dispute the “qualitative” distinction). Ifqualitative differences do exist, however, one potential source ofdifference in the cognitive processes between eminent creativeachievers and other intelligent thinkers may be in the relativeattenuation of LI. Such attenuation could well increase the numberof available mental elements, described by Simonton (1988) askey, in part, to the process of creative discovery.Other avenues of research have buttressed this hypothesis.

Dykes and McGhie (1976) demonstrated, for example, that cre-ative subjects and schizophrenic subjects were better than controlsat identifying items presented on the irrelevant channel of a di-chotic shadowing task. This finding appears to support Dellas andGaier’s (1970) observation that creative individuals tend not toscreen out so-called “irrelevant details.” Furthermore, in a studyconducted by Martindale, Anderson, Moore, and West (1996), a

Shelley H. Carson and Daniel M. Higgins, Department of Psychology,Harvard University; Jordan B. Peterson, Department of Psychology, Uni-versity of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.We thank Melanie Glickson for her research assistance in this study. We

also thank Robert Sternberg for his helpful comments. This research wassupported by grants from the Harvard University Department of Psychol-ogy and the University of Toronto Department of Psychology.Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shelley

H. Carson, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 KirklandStreet, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138. E-mail: [email protected]

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, Vol. 85, No. 3, 499–506Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499

499

Page 2: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

small group of high-creative subjects showed reduced galvanicskin response habituation rates to auditory stimuli compared withlow-creative subjects. This finding, which has been replicated inpsychosis-prone populations (Raine, Benishay, Lencz, & Scarpa,1997), may indicate that highly creative people do not precatego-rize stimuli as irrelevant in the same manner as less creativeindividuals. Finally, Eysenck (1995) has noted thatoriginality (theability to produce statistically unusual ideas) is conceptually sim-ilar to the looseness of associations symptomatic of psychosis.Such looseness is presumably a byproduct of the failure of aninhibitory filtering mechanism, functioning to limit associations tothose relevant to current task performance. Reduced LI scores aretheoretically associated with relaxation of this inhibitory mecha-nism (Gray, Feldon, Rawlins, Hemsley, & Smith, 1991).If reduced LI represents a predisposing factor common to psy-

chosis and to creativity, what then distinguishes the psychotic fromthe poet? Studies that have associated decreased LI with schizo-phrenia or schizotypy have typically failed to report subject IQ.However, among individuals with a predisposition to psychosis,low IQ generally functions as an unfavorable moderating variable(David, Malmberg, Brandt, Allebeck, & Lewis, 1997; Jones &Offord, 1975). Furthermore, several investigators (e.g., Claridge,1997; Eysenck, 1995; Berenbaum & Fujita, 1994) have suggestedthat superior intellectual qualities, such as high IQ, may moderatethe expression of a predisposition to psychosis in the highlycreative individual. It seems reasonable to propose that somepsychological phenomena might be pathogenic in the presence ofdecreased intelligence, psychometrically defined, but normative oreven abnormally useful in the presence of increased intelligence.The purpose of the present studies was therefore to (a) deter-

mine if a variety of indicators of individual creativity, particularlycreative achievement, would be associated with reduced LI; (b)determine if reduced LI in tandem with high IQ would predicthigher general creative achievement scores than either factor inisolation; and (c) determine if a combination of high IQ andreduced LI would identify those participants who have madeespecially noteworthy contributions to their respective creativefields.We limited our two participant samples to a creatively diverse

but high-IQ population to substantively increase the probability ofassessing individuals who were genuinely highly creative, becausea wide body of research (summarized in Eysenck, 1995) hasalready identified IQ as a critically important factor in creativeachievement. This body of research has suggested that there maybe an IQ threshold (usually described as an IQ of approximately120 points) associated with true creative ability (for a review, seeSternberg & O’Hara, 1999).We also present a meta-analysis of the combined results of both

of our studies to provide a more accurate estimate of effect size forLI and, as well, we describe the methods and results of an addi-tional analysis of eminent creative achievers versus highly intelli-gent noncreative individuals from the pooled sample.

STUDY 1: LI, CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT, ANDOTHER MEASURES OF CREATIVITY

One of the major obstacles to creativity research has been thevariety of diverse concepts and definitions associated with the termcreative(Brown, 1989), a descriptor that applies with equal facility

to achievement, mental processes (Torrance, 1968), a personalitytrait or combination of traits (Gough, 1979; McCrae, 1987), andconsensually approved novel products (Amabile, 1996). We areparticularly interested in lifetime creative achievement, per se,because such achievement is beneficial not only to the individualbut to society as well. Furthermore, other measures of creativity,including creative personality traits and creative mental processmeasures (divergent thinking), have been found to be elevated incertain nonproductive disordered populations as well (e.g.,Cramond, 1994; Keefe & Magaro, 1980) and thus may not beunique to highly creative individuals.However, our initial examination of the LI–creativity associa-

tion used multiple measures of creativity designed to cover a widerange of concepts, including a lifetime creative achievement mea-sure (the Creative Achievement Questionnaire [CAQ]; Carson,Peterson & Higgins, 2003), a mental process measure (divergentthinking tasks; Torrance, 1968), and a personality measure (theCreative Personality Scale [CPS]; Gough, 1979).1

For Study 1, we hypothesized that (a) high scorers relative tolow scorers on the CAQ would be characterized by attenuated LI.We also hypothesized that (b) high scorers on the CPS and thedivergent thinking tasks would demonstrate attenuated LI relativeto low scorers. Finally, we hypothesized that (c) low LI scorescombined with high IQ scores would predict high CAQ scores.

Method

Participants

Eighty-six Harvard undergraduates (33 men, 53 women), with a meanage of 20.7 years (SD� 3.3) participated in the study. All were recruitedfrom sign-up sheets posted on campus. Participants were paid an hourlyrate.

Procedure

Participants were assigned randomly to either the preexposed (n � 57)or the nonpreexposed (n � 29; a ratio of approximately 2:1) LI condition.During a pretest interview in which demographic information was col-lected, participants were assessed for outward signs of depression, anxiety,or boredom. They were also questioned about alcohol and/or caffeineintake prior to the lab testing. Two participants were rescheduled becauseof recent caffeine intake. All participants then completed creativity mea-sures, IQ tests, personality tests, and the LI task.

Creativity Testing

Creativity testing consisted of three phases: the CAQ, the divergentthinking tasks, and the CPS.CAQ. The CAQ is an empirically sound measure of lifetime creative

accomplishment in the fields of art and science. The instrument hasdemonstrated test–retest reliability in the range ofr � .85 as well as goodconvergent, discriminant, and predictive validity. Participants check offactual achievements in 10 separate domains of creative accomplishment(e.g., “My work has won a prize or prizes at a juried art show”). Scores forindividual accomplishments within a domain are weighted according to the

1 An additional measure related to creativity, the personality variableOpenness to Experience, was also assessed for its relationship to LI in thissample. However, analysis of this relationship has been reported elsewhere(Peterson & Carson, 2000).

500 CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS

Page 3: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

level of achievement attained, as ranked by experts within that domain.Weighted scores for each domain are summed to provide a total creativeachievement score (Carson et al., 2003).Divergent thinking tasks. Four validated divergent thinking tasks were

adapted from Torrance (1968). Participants were given 3 min to write downtheir responses to each task, such as naming alternate uses for a commonobject. Three aspects of divergent thinking were assessed: fluency (numberof responses generated), originality (unusualness of responses, based on thestatistical infrequency of each individual response within the current sam-ple), and flexibility (number of different categories of response and thenumber of category changes). Fluency, originality, and flexibility scoreswerez scored and summed to produce a divergent thinking score for eachsubject.CPS. The CPS (Gough, 1979) consists of a set of 30 items from the

Adjective Check List (Gough & Heilbrun, 1965), which has predicted highlevels of creativity across multiple studies and diverse samples. Partici-pants describe themselves by checking off 18 positively and 12 negativelyscored adjectives.

IQ Testing

Participants completed the Vocabulary and Block Design subtests of theWechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981).Raw scores were age scaled, combined to form a composite, and convertedto a full-scale equivalent, using standard guidelines (Brooker & Cyr, 1986).IQ estimates compiled from this “short form” correlate atr � .91 withfull-scale WAIS-R IQ scores (Brooker & Cyr, 1986). Because IQ scoresusing the short form typically overestimate IQ by 3 points (Brooker & Cyr,1986), IQ scores were adjusted by subtracting 3 points from the total foreach participant.

LI Task

Participants were seated in a quiet, semidarkened room and told that theywere to participate in two auditory discrimination tasks. They then donnedstereo earphones and adjusted the volume to ensure clarity and compre-hensibility. Participants in the preexposed condition were then shown atwo-part video version of a well-validated and commonly used auditory LItask, after Lubow et al. (1992).In Part 1, the preexposure phase, a list of 30 nonsense syllables (the

masking material) was presented 5 times at a normal rate of speech with noindication of the termination and start of each repetition. White noise bursts(the target stimuli) from 3 to 6 s in duration were superimposed ran-domly 31 times over the course of the recording, at approximately two-thirds the volume of the nonsense syllables. Participants listened to therecording through earphones and were asked to determine how many timesthey heard a selected nonsense syllable.In Part 2 (the test phase), the recording of the nonsense syllables and the

white noise bursts was replayed in identical form while yellow disksarranged in rows on a black scoreboard were revealed individually on thevideo screen. Each yellow disk appeared prior to the offset of the targetstimulus. Participants were asked to determine what auditory stimulussignaled the appearance of the yellow disks. The individual’s score for thetask (trials to rule identification) was determined by the number of yellowdisks visible on the screen (maximum 31) when the correct answer wasgiven.Participants in the nonpreexposed condition were shown an identical

videotape, except that the target stimulus was absent from the preexposurephase of the task.Traditionally, LI is demonstrated (and the LI test validated) within a

population when participants of that population tested in the preexposedcondition take more trials to learn the association of the target stimuluswith the yellow disks than similar participants tested in the nonpreexposedcondition (Lubow et al., 1992). Thus, lower LI scores in the preexposed

condition suggest attenuated LI, whereas higher scores suggest intact or“normal” LI.

Results

Table 1 lists the zero-order correlations of all creativity mea-sures with each other as well as with IQ.

Creative Achievement and LI

Participants were divided into low- (n� 45,M � 6.7,SD� 3.9)and high- (n � 41,M � 27.0,SD� 11.3) creative achievementgroups by CAQ score median split.2 Members of the low-creativeachievement group in the preexposed condition (n � 28) scoredsignificantly higher on the LI task than the low-creative achieversin the nonpreexposed condition (n � 17), indicating a significantLI effect, t(43)� �2.03,p� .04,d� .62 (see Figure 1). Membersof the high-creative achievement group in the preexposed condi-tion (n� 29), by contrast, did not score significantly higher on theLI task than their counterparts in the nonpreexposed condition(n � 12), indicating an attenuation of LI in the high-creativeachievement group,t(39)� �.020,p � .96,d � .01. An analysisof variance (ANOVA) indicated significant differences in LIscores due to the effects of high- and low-creative achievement,F(1, 82)� 4.09,p � .05,�2 � .05. The main effect for condition,F(1, 82)� 2.12, p � .15, �2 � .03, and the interaction effectbetween CAQ and condition,F(1, 82)� 2.04,p � .16,�2 � .02,were not significant.Because LI scores were clearly bimodal rather than normally

distributed (see Figure 2), nonparametric analyses (Mann–WhitneyU) were also conducted and yielded similar results: Low-creativeachievement participants demonstrated intact LI (Z � �1.86,p �.06), whereas high-creative achievement participants demonstratedreduced LI (Z � �.043,p � .96).As hypothesized, the mean LI score of the low-creative achieve-

ment group (n � 28, M � 21.8,SD � 10.4) was significantlyhigher than that of the high-creative achievement group (n � 29,M � 14.3,SD� 8.8) in the preexposed condition,t(55)� 2.93,p � .006,d � .79 (nonparametric Mann–WhitneyU: Z � �2.54,p � .01) (see Figure 3).

LI and Measures of Divergent Thinking and CreativePersonality

A comparison of means indicated that LI scores in the preex-posed condition were significantly reduced in the high-scoringgroups (determined by median split) of the CPS and the originalitydimension of the divergent thinking tasks as well as in overalldivergent thinking (see Table 2). Nonparametric Mann–WhitneyUtests revealed similar results.

LI and IQ as Factors of Creative Achievement

The mean IQ of the sample was 128.1 points (SD� 10.3), witha range of 97 to 148 points. To determine whether LI and IQ would

2 “Median split” is traditionally used in LI research (e.g. Baruch et al.,1988a, 1988b; Lubow et al., 1992) and thus is included here. However,median splits arguably lack theoretical meaning. A theoretically drivendivision of high- and low-creative achievers is presented in the ANALY-SIS OF EMINENT CREATIVE ACHIEVERS section.

501LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT

Page 4: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

jointly predict creative achievement scores, we regressed LI scores(preexposed condition) and IQ scores on the creative achievementscores of the highest and lowest quartiles of CAQ scorers (n� 30).Negative LI scores and positive IQ scores jointly predicted 26% ofthe variance in creative achievement scores,F(2, 27)� 4.89,p �.006,R2 � .26, with LI scores alone accounting for 18% of thetotal CAQ variance,F(1, 28)� 6.09, p � .02, R2 � .18.3 Anadditional analysis of the CAQ, IQ, and LI relationship is offeredbelow in Study 2 and, cumulatively, in the section ANALYSIS OFEMINENT CREATIVE ACHIEVERS.

STUDY 2: LI AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT

In Study 2, we attempted to replicate our findings within theHarvard population, concentrating primarily on the relationship ofLI to lifetime creative achievement. Because we had alreadyestablished the existence of an LI effect for this population usinga nonpreexposed control group (and because the LI scores of highcreative achievers did not differ significantly from low achieversin the control group), we decided to concentrate on individualdifference scores within the preexposed condition.We also wished to control for the chance that participants would

respond to the self-report measure of creative achievement, theCAQ, in a self-enhancing manner. We therefore administered the

Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSD; Crowne &Marlowe, 1960), a measure of the tendency of responders to tailortheir responses to appear socially acceptable.

Method

Participants

Ninety-six Harvard undergraduates (53 men, 43 women), with a meanage of 20.1 years (SD� 1.6) participated in the study. All were registeredin a psychology course and received course credit for participation.

3 The regression of IQ and LI (preexposed) on the entire sample was asfollows: F(2, 58) � 2.84, p � .07, R2 � .10, with LI (preexposed)accounting for 9% of the variance.

Table 1Correlations of Creativity and IQ Measures

Measure CAQ CPS Diverg Fluency Orig Flex

CPS .33*Diverg .47* .29*Fluency .38* .11 .85*Orig .46* .32* .86* .59*Flex .37* .34* .87* .62* .63*IQ .13 .26* .10 �.03 .19 .08

Note. CAQ � Creative Achievement Questionnaire; CPS� CreativePersonality Scale; Diverg� divergent thinking; Orig� originality; Flex�flexibility.* p � .05.

Figure 1. Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) and latent inhibi-tion (LI) scores in the preexposed and nonpreexposed conditions. Errorbars represent the standard error (positive value only) for the mean LI scoreof each high–low CAQ� Condition group.

Figure 2. Bimodal distribution of latent inhibition scores in the preex-posed condition in Studies 1 and 2.

502 CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS

Page 5: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

Procedure

All participants completed a computerized version of the CAQ identicalto the paper-and-pencil version used in Study 1 as well as a computerizedversion of the MCSD. In subsequent lab sessions, participants were ad-ministered the LI task in the experimental condition and the IQ tests. Boththe LI and the IQ procedures were identical to those used in Study 1.

Results

Four participants were eliminated from the study because ofMCSD scores more than 2 standard deviations above the mean,indicating a strong possibility of self-enhancement in responses onthe CAQ.

Creative Achievement and LI

Participants were divided into low-creative achievement (lowCAQ; n � 47,M � 4.9,SD� 3.1) and high-creative achievement(high CAQ;n � 45,M � 22.3,SD� 9.7) groups by median split,as in Study 1 (see Figure 3). As predicted, participants in thehigh-CAQ group had significantly lower LI scores (M � 12.8,SD � 10.1) than did participants in the low-CAQ group(M � 17.6,SD� 11.4); t(90)� 2.13,p � .04,d � .45 (Mann–WhitneyU: Z � 2.09,p � .04).

LI and IQ as Factors of Creative Achievement

Eighty-two of the 96 participants completed the IQ tasks. Themean IQ of the sample was 124.6 points (SD � 11.4), rangingfrom 100 to 148 points. As in Study 1, we regressed LI scores(preexposed condition) and IQ scores on the upper and lowerquartiles of CAQ scores (n � 46). Negative LI scores and positiveIQ scores jointly predicted 20% of the variance in CAQ scores,F(2, 43) � 5.33, p � .006, R2 � .20, with LI scores alone

accounting for 13% of the total CAQ variance,F(1, 43)� 6.54,p � .01,R2 � .13.4

META-ANALYSIS: LI AND CREATIVEACHIEVEMENT

The results of analyses of the relationship between creativeachievement scores and LI scores from Studies 1 and 2 werecombined using meta-analytic techniques (Rosenthal & Rosnow,1991). Because both studies used samples of similar IQ andcreative achievement, Studies 1 and 2 were weighted equally in themeta-analysis. When LI scores were compared between high-CAQand low-CAQ groups (median split), the combined results of thetwo studies yielded highly significant differences with an effectsize of r � .31 (p � .0003, one-tailed; see Table 3).

ANALYSIS OF EMINENT CREATIVE ACHIEVERS

The results of Studies 1 and 2 indicate that participants whoscored higher in overall creative achievement exhibited signifi-cantly lower LI scores than participants who did not score high increative achievement. However, a high score in combined areas ofcreative achievement may not necessarily be indicative of thelikelihood of truly eminent achievement in a specified field. One ofthe defining characteristics of the truly eminent creative achiever,however, is the production of a significant accomplishment in hisor her respective field at a young age (Ludwig, 1995). By poolingthe participants from both studies, we hoped to identify enougheminent creative achievers(those who made a truly substantivecontribution to a single domain of creative endeavor before the ageof 21) to allow for analysis of LI and IQ.We defined a score of at least 12 points on the CAQ in any one

of the 10 individual domains of measured creative achievement asconstituting a clearly significant contribution to a creative field.The minimum levels of achievement that qualified as a significantaccomplishment according to this criterion included having anovel or book of poetry published and sold, having a musicalcomposition recorded and sold, having a prototype invention pat-ented and built, having a private showing of original artwork at arecognized gallery, or winning a scholarship or national prize fora scientific discovery. We then compared the LI scores of theidentified group of eminent creative achievers with the scores ofthe group of individuals who demonstrated minimal creativeachievement.

Method

Participants included pooled subjects from Studies 1 and 2 who met thefollowing criteria: (a) They were members of the preexposed condition ofthe LI task, and (b) they had scored either 12 or more points on any singledomain of the CAQ (eminent creative achievers;n � 25), or had a totalCAQ score of 3 or below (controls;n � 23). Eminent creative achieversincluded 4 artists, 5 composers, 2 writers, 2 inventors, 3 dramatists, 7scientists, and 2 choreographers. Mean LI scores of eminent achievers werethen compared with the LI scores of the control subjects.

4 The regression of IQ and LI (preexposed) on the entire sample was asfollows: F(2, 79) � 2.70, p � .07, R2 � .07, with LI (preexposed)accounting for 5% of the variance.

Figure 3. Latent inhibition (LI) scores (preexposed condition) of high-and low-creative achievers as measured by the Creative AchievementQuestionnaire (CAQ). Error bars represent the standard error (positivevalue only) for the mean LI score (preexposed) of each high–low CAQgroup.

503LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT

Page 6: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

Results

LI and Eminent Creative Achievers

The LI scores of the eminent creative achievers (M � 11.1,SD � 7.6) were significantly lower than the LI scores of thecontrol group (M � 19.4,SD� 10.5),t(46)� 3.17,p� .004,d�.93 (Mann–WhitneyU: Z � 2.65, p � .008). Whereas controlsubjects were equally likely to display either high or low LI scores(on the basis of the manifest division in the LI distribution),eminent creative achievers were seven times more likely to havelow rather than high LI scores,�2(1, N � 25)� 10.79,� � .47,p � .001 (see Figure 4).

LI and IQ as Factors of Eminent Creative Achievement

As mentioned above, a number of researchers have noted acorrelation between creative ability and IQ levels below 120,whereas correlations between creativity and IQs above 120 aregenerally less significant. This threshold theory suggests that an IQof 120 may be necessary but not sufficient for creative achieve-ment (for a review, see Sternberg & O’Hara, 1999; for an alter-native view, see Lubinski, Webb, Morlock, & Benbow, 2001). Inour sample, the mean IQ for the eminent creative achiever groupwas above the 120 threshold (M � 128.6,SD� 8.3), whereas the

mean IQ for the control group was just below the 120 threshold(M � 118.3,SD� 11.9). Twenty-one of the 25 eminent achievershad IQs above the 120 threshold, whereas 11 of the 23 controls hadIQs above the threshold.The combined sample of eminent creative achievers and con-

trols was divided into high-IQ (M � 130.0,SD� 6.6,n� 32) andmoderate-IQ (M � 111.1,SD� 7.6,n � 16) groups on the basisof the proposed threshold score of IQ 120. The combined samplewas also divided into high-LI (M � 27.6,SD� 3.8,n � 16) andlow-LI (M � 6.6,SD� 4.7, n � 32) groups on the basis of thenaturally occurring split in bimodal LI scores in the preexposedcondition. A 2� 2 factorial ANOVA examined the CAQ scoresusing high LI/low LI and high IQ/moderate IQ as factors. Theresults indicated a significant difference among the creativeachievement scores of the groups,F(3, 44)� 8.58,p� .001,�2�.37, with the low-LI/high-IQ group demonstrating substantiallyhigher CAQ scores than all other groups, as predicted (see Figure

Table 2Latent Inhibition (LI) Scores of the High Versus Low Groups of Individual Measures ofCreativity (Based on Median Split)

MeasureLI score ofhigh group

LI score oflow group t df p d Z p

CPS 3.63 47 � .01 1.06 �3.48 � .01M 11.0 20.8SD 7.0 10.1

Diverg 2.09 52 .04 0.58 �1.78 .08M 15.6 21.4SD 9.5 10.9

Orig 2.34 52 .02 0.65 �2.26 .02M 15.1 21.5SD 9.7 10.3

Fluency 1.58 52 .12 0.43 �1.37 .18M 16.4 21.1SD 9.5 11.5

Flex 1.01 52 .32 0.28 �0.88 .38M 16.7 19.6SD 9.5 11.4

Note. Z� Mann–WhitneyU test; CPS� Creative Personality Scale; Diverg� divergent thinking; Orig�originality; Flex� flexibility.

Table 3Difference in Mean Latent Inhibition Scores of High Versus LowCreative Achievers: A Meta-Analysis

Study t Effect size p

1 2.93a .400 .00252 2.13b .219 .018Total .314 .0003

Note. All p values are one-tailed.adf � 55. b df � 90.

Figure 4. Contingency table of high and low latent inhibition (LI) scores(preexposed) for eminent creative achievers versus controls.

504 CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS

Page 7: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

5). The main effects for both LI,F(1, 44)� 7.30,p � .01,�2 �.14, and IQ,F(1, 44)� 5.44,p � .02,�2 � .11, were significant,and the interaction between the two factors approached signifi-cance,F(1, 44)� 3.47,p � .07,�2 � .07.When we regressed LI (preexposed condition) and IQ scores on

the CAQ scores, negative LI scores and positive IQ scores jointlypredicted almost one third of the variance in creative achievementscores,F(2, 45)� 9.55,p� .0003,R2� .30, with LI scores aloneaccounting for 19% of the total CAQ variance,F(1, 46)� 10.53,p � .002,R2 � .19. The LI� IQ interaction was highly signifi-cant,F(1, 46)� 15.81,p � .008,�2 � .98.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

The results of these studies and analyses indicate a substantialand significant relationship between a variety of indicators ofcreativity and reduced LI. In Study 1, significant differences in LIscores emerged between high- and low-creative participants onmeasures of creative achievement, creative personality, and theoriginality facet of divergent thinking. The results of Study 2replicated the relationship between creative achievement and re-duced LI. A meta-analysis of the two studies indicated a nontrivialand highly significant relationship between creative achievementand reduced LI, and an analysis of eminent creative achievers(relative to noncreative control subjects) suggested a near-universal reduction in LI in this group.How do we reconcile the fact that reduced LI scores in humans,

which have previously been associated with psychotic states orwith psychotic proneness, are associated with high levels of cre-ativity in these studies? The results provide substantial althoughcircumstantial evidence that high IQ might serve as a beneficialmoderating factor in the expression of LI as either a deficit inselective attention or a facilitator of creativity. In all of our studiesand analyses, high IQ, when combined with low LI, was associatedwith increased creative achievement. These results are particularlystunning in the analysis of eminent achievers and high-functioningcontrols. High IQ clearly appeared to augment the tendency towardhigh creative achievement characteristic of low-LI individuals.

These results lend support to the theory that there may bequalitative (e.g., failure to filter out irrelevant stimuli) as well asquantitative (e.g., high IQ) differences in the processes underlyingcreative versus normal cognition. These results also support thetheory that highly creative individuals and psychotic-prone indi-viduals may possess neurobiological similarities, perhaps geneti-cally determined, that present either as psychotic predisposition onthe one hand or as unusual creative potential on the other on thebasis of the presence of moderating cognitive factors such as highIQ (e.g., Berenbaum & Fujita, 1994; Dykes & McGhie, 1976;Eysenck, 1995). These moderating factors may allow an individualto override a “deficit” in early selective attentional processing witha high-functioning mechanism at a later, more controlled level ofselective processing. The highly creative individual may be priv-ileged to access a greater inventory of unfiltered stimuli duringearly processing, thereby increasing the odds of original recombi-nant ideation. Thus, a deficit that is generally associated withpathology may well impart a creative advantage in the presence ofother cognitive strengths such as high IQ.One of the limitations of the present research was the use of a

single measure of LI. We are currently developing a multimethodprocedure for assessing LI that will hopefully further clarify therelationship between reduced LI and creativity in future research.We are also investigating other possible moderating factors (in-cluding working memory capacity and personality dimensions)that may have an impact on the creativity–LI relationship.

References

Amabile, T. M. (1996).Creativity in context.Boulder, CO: WestviewPress.

Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988a). Differential perfor-mance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176,598–606.

Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988b). Latent inhibition and“psychotic proneness” in normal subjects.Personality and IndividualDifferences, 9,777–783.

Berenbaum, H., & Fujita, F. (1994). Schizophrenia and personality: Ex-ploring the boundaries and connections between vulnerability and out-come.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,148–158.

Brooker, B. H., & Cyr, J. J. (1986). Tables for clinicians to use to convertWAIS-R Short Forms.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42,983.

Brown, R. T. (1989). Creativity: What are we to measure? In J. A. Glover,R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.),Handbook of creativity(pp.3–32). New York: Plenum Press.

Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. (2003).Validity, reliability, andfactor structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire.Manuscriptsubmitted for publication.

Claridge, G. (Ed.). (1997).Schizotypy: Implications for illness and health.New York: Oxford University Press.

Cramond, B. (1994). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and creativ-ity: What is the connection?Journal of Creative Behavior, 28,193–210.

Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirabilityindependent of psychopathology.Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24,349–354.

David, A. S., Malmberg, A., Brandt, L., Allebeck, P., & Lewis, G. (1997).IQ and risk for schizophrenia: A population-based cohort study.Psy-chological Medicine, 27,1311–1323.

Dellas, M., & Gaier, E. L. (1970). Identification of creativity: The indi-vidual.Psychological Bulletin, 73,55–73.

Dykes, M., & McGhie, A. (1976). A comparative study of attentional

Figure 5. Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) scores of high–low latent inhibition (LI) and high–moderate IQ groups in pooled eminentachievers and controls. Error bars represent the standard error (positivevalue only) for the mean CAQ score of each high–low LI� IQ group.

505LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT

Page 8: Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased ......including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be

strategies of schizophrenic and highly creative normal subjects.BritishJournal of Psychiatry, 128,50–56.

Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Creativity as a product of intelligence and person-ality. In D. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.),International handbook ofpersonality and intelligence: Perspectives on individual differences(pp.231–247). New York: Plenum Press.

Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the Adjective CheckList. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,1398–1405.

Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. B. (1965).The Adjective Check List manual.Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Gray, J. A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N. P., Hemsley, D. R., & Smith, A. D.(1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia.Behavioral and BrainSciences, 14,1–84.

Jones, M. B., & Offord, D. R. (1975). Independent transmission of IQ andschizophrenia.British Journal of Psychiatry, 126,185–190.

Keefe, J. A., & Magaro, P. A. (1980). Creativity and schizophrenia: Anequivalence of cognitive processing.Journal of Abnormal Psychol-ogy, 89,390–398.

King, L., Walker, L., & Broyles, S. (1996). Creativity and the five-factormodel.Journal of Research in Personality, 30,189–203.

Lubinski, D., Webb, R. M., Morelock, M. J., & Benbow, C. P. (2001).Top 1 in 10,000: A 10-year follow-up of the profoundly gifted.Journalof Applied Psychology, 86,718–729.

Lubow, R. E. (1989).Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory.Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Lubow, R. E., & Gewirtz, J. C. (1995). Latent inhibition in humans: Data,theory, and implications for schizophrenia.Psychological Bulletin, 117,87–103.

Lubow, R. E., Ingberg-Sachs, Y., Zalstein-Orda, N., & Gewirtz, J. C.(1992). Latent inhibition in low and high “psychotic-prone” normalsubjects.Personality and Individual Differences, 15,563–572.

Ludwig, A. M. (1995).The price of greatness: Resolving the creativity andmadness controversy.New York: Guilford Press.

Martindale, C., Anderson, K., Moore, K., & West, A. N. (1996). Creativity,

oversensitivity, and the rate of habituation.Personality and IndividualDifferences, 20,423–427.

McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness toexperience.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,1258–1263.

Peterson, J. B., & Carson, S. (2000). Latent inhibition and openness toexperience in a high-achieving student population.Personality andIndividual Differences, 28,323–332.

Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. (2002). Openness and Extra-version are associated with reduced latent inhibition: Replication andcommentary.Personality and Individual Differences, 33,1137–1147.

Raine, A., Benishay, D., Lencz, T., & Scarpa, A. (1997). Abnormalorienting in schizotypal personality disorder.Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23,75–82.

Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991).Essentials of behavioral research:Methods and data analysis.New York: McGraw-Hill.

Simonton, D. K. (1988).Scientific genius: A psychology of science.NewYork: Cambridge University Press.

Simonton, D. K. (1999).Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives oncreativity.New York: Oxford University Press.

Sternberg, R. J., & O’Hara, L. A. (1999). Creativity and intelligence. InR. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of creativity(pp. 251–272). New York:Cambridge University Press.

Torrance, E. P. (1968). Examples and rationales of test tasks for assessingcreative abilities.Journal of Creative Behavior, 2,165–178.

Wechsler, D. (1981).WAIS-R Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised.San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Weisberg, R. W. (1993).Creativity: Beyond the myth of genius.New York:Freeman.

Received February 6, 2002Revision received January 16, 2003

Accepted January 21, 2003�

506 CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS