COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP...

76
Thesis for Doctoral degree in Psychology, Östersund 2018 COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE ROLES OF CONTEXT, SITUATION AND GENDER IN SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors: Patrick Millet, Richard Ahlström Department of Psychology Mid Sweden University, SE-831 25 Östersund, Sweden ISSN 1652893X Mid Sweden University Doctoral Thesis 277 ISBN 9789188527431

Transcript of COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP...

Page 1: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

   

 

 Thesis  for  Doctoral  degree  in  Psychology,  Östersund  2018  

 

   

 

COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING:

THE ROLES OF CONTEXT, SITUATION AND GENDER IN SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING

Pär Löfstrand

Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson

Co-supervisors: Patrick Millet, Richard Ahlström

Department of Psychology Mid Sweden University, SE-831 25 Östersund, Sweden

ISSN 1652-­‐‑893X

Mid Sweden University Doctoral Thesis 277 ISBN 978-­‐‑91-­‐‑88527-­‐‑43-­‐‑1  

Page 2: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

Akademisk   avhandling   som   med   tillstånd   av   Mittuniversitetet   i   Östersund  framläggs  till  offentlig  granskning  för  avläggande  av  Filosofie  Doktorsexamen  fredagen   den   6:e   april,   2018,   klockan   10.15,   i   sal   F234,   Mittuniversitetet  Östersund.  Seminariet  kommer  att  hållas  på  svenska  

COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING AND STEREOTYPING: THE ROLES OF CONTEXT, SITUATION AND GENDER IN

SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING

Pär Löfstrand    

©  Pär  Löfstrand,  2018  Printed  by  Mid  Sweden  University,  Sundsvall  ISSN:  1652-­‐‑893X  ISBN:  978-­‐‑91-­‐‑88527-­‐‑43-­‐‑1    Department  of  Psychology  Mid  Sweden  University,  SE-­‐‑831  25  Östersund,  Sweden  Phone:  +46  (0)10-­‐‑142  80  00  Mid  Sweden  University,  Östersund,  Sweden,  Doctoral  Thesis  277

Page 3: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

 

To  Daniel        

Page 4: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:
Page 5: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

 

TABLE  OF  CONTENTS  

 

ABSTRACT  ..........................................................................................................  viii  

SVENSK  SAMMANFATTNING  .........................................................................  x  

LIST  OF  PAPERS  ..................................................................................................  xii  

1.   PREFACE  ...........................................................................................................  1  

2.   INTRODUCTION  ...........................................................................................  2  

2.1 Brief historical background to experimental social psychology ............................ 5

2.2 Group decision-making (GDM) ............................................................................. 6

2.2.1 Groups in real life versus ad-hoc groups? ................................................. 8

2.2.2 What makes a group decision-making process effective, successful and democratic? ......................................................................................................... 8

2.3 Stereotypes, attitudes and prejudice ..................................................................... 11 2.3.1 Stereotyping .............................................................................................. 11

2.3.2 The stereotype dimensions warmth and competence ................................ 12

2.4 Communication and stereotyping ........................................................................ 14

2.4.1 Social constructionism and small group communication ......................... 14

2.4.2 Analysis methods for small group communication ................................... 15

2.5 The complexity of gender and gender stereotypes .............................................. 16

3.     AIM  ...................................................................................................................  19  

4.    METHODOLOGY  ........................................................................................  20  

4.1 Mixed-method approach ...................................................................................... 21

4.2 Group size of decision-making groups ................................................................ 22

4.3 Ad-hoc or real groups? ........................................................................................ 23

4.4 The experiments ................................................................................................... 23

4.4.1 The Relay Team ........................................................................................ 24

4.4.2 The Consultant .......................................................................................... 25

4.5 Quantitative data .................................................................................................. 26

4.6 Qualitative data analysis .................................................................................. 27

Page 6: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

vi  

4.6.1 Interaction process analysis (IPA) ........................................................... 27

4.6.2 Conversational argument coding scheme (CACS) ................................... 28

4.6.3 Stereotype content analysis ....................................................................... 28

4.6.4 Conversational pattern analysis ............................................................... 29

5.     EMPIRICAL  PAPERS  ...................................................................................  29  

5.1 Paper I .................................................................................................................. 29

5.1.1 Background and aim ................................................................................. 29

5.1.2 Methodology ............................................................................................. 30

5.1.3 Results ....................................................................................................... 30

5.1.4 Conclusion ................................................................................................ 31

5.2 Paper II ................................................................................................................. 31 5.2.1 Background and aim ................................................................................. 31

5.2.2 Method ...................................................................................................... 32

5.2.3 Results ....................................................................................................... 33

5.2.4 Conclusions ............................................................................................... 33

5.3 Paper III ............................................................................................................... 34

5.3.1 Background and aim ................................................................................. 34

5.3.2 Method ...................................................................................................... 34

5.3.3 Results ....................................................................................................... 35

5.3.4 Conclusion ................................................................................................ 35

5.4 Paper IV ............................................................................................................... 36 5.4.1 Background and aim ................................................................................. 36

5.4.2 Method ...................................................................................................... 36

5.4.3 Results ....................................................................................................... 37

5.5.4 Conclusion ................................................................................................ 37

6.     GENERAL  DISCUSSION  ............................................................................  38  

6.1 Stereotyping and context ..................................................................................... 38

6.2 The role of experiences ........................................................................................ 41

6.3 Methodological considerations ............................................................................ 42

6.4 Practical implications ........................................................................................... 43

Page 7: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

 

6.5 Future research ..................................................................................................... 45

6.6 The Bottom Line .................................................................................................. 46

7.          ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS  ...........................................................................  47  

8.   REFERENCES  .................................................................................................  49  

9.     APPENDIX:  EMPIRICAL  PAPERS  I-­‐‑IV  ...................................................  65  

   

Page 8: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

viii  

COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING AND STEREOTYPING: THE ROLES OF CONTEXT, SITUATION AND GENDER IN SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING

Pär Löfstrand Department  of  Psychology,  Mid  Sweden  University,  SE-­‐‑831  25  Östersund,  Sweden  

ISSN  1652-­‐‑893X  Mid  Sweden  University  Doctoral  Thesis  277;  

ISBN    978-­‐‑91-­‐‑88527-­‐‑43-­‐‑1  

 

ABSTRACT Making   decisions   together   in   groups   takes   an   important   role   in   society.  

Everywhere   and   in   many   different   contexts   people   meet   to   make   more   or   less  formal  decisions.  As  stereotypes  constitute  simplified  group  based  perceptions  of  other  people,  decision-­‐‑making  groups  risk  making  biased   judgments  and  commit  discriminating  decisions.  Stereotyping  often   follow   the   two  universal  dimensions  competence   and  warmth   (Cuddy,  Fiske  &  Glick,   2008).  How  people´s   judgments  are  affected  by  stereotypes  has  mainly  been  studied  on  individual  level  and  less  is  known  about  how   stereotypes   and  prejudice   is   communicated   and  negotiated   in  group  decision-­‐‑making  situations.  One  approach  to  study  this  is  to  investigate  how  different   contexts   may   lead   to   different   communication   patterns,   different  experiences,   and   different   decisions.     In   this   thesis   context   was   varied   in   two  different   ways   in   two   experiments.   In   the   first   experiment   the   goal   set   for   the  decision-­‐‑making  was  varied.  A   competitive  goal  was   contrasted   to   a   cooperative  goal  in  a  group  decision  task  using  a  sports  scenario  where  the  participants  had  to  select   members   to   a   relay   team.   In   the   second   experiment   different   information  was   used   as   a   context   variable.   This   was   done   by   varying   the   information   of  gender  and  parenthood  status  of  the  applicants  in  a  fictive  recruitment  scenario.  In  addition,   in   both   experiments   the   gender   composition   in   the   groups  was   varied,  forming  yet  another  variable   that  might  play  a   role   for  how   the  decision-­‐‑making  was   carried   out.   These   three   factors   were   assumed   to   influence   the   form   of   the  communication,   the   content   of   the   communication   in   terms   of   stereotyping,   and  how   the   decision-­‐‑making   process   was   experienced.   A   mixed-­‐‑method   approach  was  chosen  where  quantitative  and  qualitative  data  were  used  in  conjunction  with  each  other,  which  was  assumed  to  give  a  richer  picture  of  the  results.  

Page 9: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

 

In  paper  I  the  form  of  the  communication,  as  analyzed  with  interaction  process  analysis  (IPA),  did  not  differ  much  between  the  two  goals.  On  the  other  hand,  the  content   showed  more   systematic   patterns.  A   competitive   goal   seemed   to   lead   to  both  inclusion  and  exclusion  with  use  of  both  positive  and  negative  stereotypes.  A  cooperative  goal  seemed  to  lead  to  inclusion  mechanisms  and  only  use  of  positive  stereotypes.  In  paper  II  where  the  aim  was  to  investigate  what  was  experienced  as  constituting   a   successful   decision-­‐‑making   process   it   was   found   that   equality   of  influence  was  of  importance.  Furthermore,  qualitative  analyses  of  the  conversation  patterns,  by  use  of  the  conversational  argument  coding  scheme  (CACS),  seemed  to  validate   this.  The   successful  groups  had  a  more   complex   communication  pattern  than  the  less  successful  groups.  In  paper  III,  where  the  information  for  the  decision  task  was  varied  in  terms  of  gender  and  parenthood  status  of  the  applicants,  it  was  found  that  parenthood   information  triggered  a   lot  of  discussion.  The  participants  did  not  differentiate   between  mothers   and   fathers,   but   they   applied   attributes   of  competence   and   warmth   differently   to   the   targets.   Furthermore,   gender   and  gender   composition   seemed   to   matter   as   male   and   female   groups   applied   the  attributes   differently.   Paper   IV   used   data   from   both   experiments   in   order   to  investigate  how  the  context  variables  and  gender  composition  influenced  how  the  decision  situation  was  experienced.  The  results   indicate  that  the  context  variables  and  gender  composition  interacted  with  own  gender.  Men  seemed  more  content  in  male  groups  with  male  targets  and  a  male  parent  condition  while  women  seemed  more  content  in  mixed  groups  and  a  female  parent  condition.    

Context  seems  to  play  an   important  role,  as   it  provides   the  participants   in   the  group   discussions   with   different   information,   leading   to   different   patterns   of  stereotyping   in   the  discussions.  Also  how  the  decision  was  experienced  seems   to  be  related  to  the  context.  Furthermore,  group  composition  seems  to  function  in  this  way   too.   The   results   are   discussed   in   relation   to   practical   implications   and  suggestions  for  future  research.      

 Keywords:   Competition,   Cooperation,   Gender,   Group   Decision-­‐‑Making,  Stereotype  Content  Model      

       

Page 10: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

x  

KOMMUNICERA, FÖRHANDLA OCH ANVÄNDA STEREOTYPER: VILKEN ROLL SPELAR KONTEXT, SITUATION OCH GENUS I SMÅ GRUPPERS BESLUTSFATTANDE

Pär Löfstrand Department  of  Psychology,  

Mid  Sweden  University,  SE-­‐‑831  25  Östersund,  Sweden  ISSN  1652-­‐‑893X  Mid  Sweden  University  Doctoral  Thesis  277;  

ISBN  978-­‐‑91-­‐‑88527-­‐‑43-­‐‑1  

SVENSK SAMMANFATTNING Att   fatta   beslut   tillsammans   spelar   en   viktig   roll   i   samhället.   I   alla   möjliga  

sammanhang   träffas   människor   för   att   ta   mer   eller   mindre   formella   beslut.  Beslutsfattande  grupper  riskerar  att  fatta  felaktiga  och  diskriminerande  beslut  om  beslutsfattarna   påverkas   av   psykologiska   mekanismer   som   stereotyper,   d.v.s.  förenklad   information  baserad  på  upplevelser  av  andra  människor  och   till  vilken  grupp  de  associeras  till.  Hur  människors  bedömningar  påverkas  av  stereotyper  har  tidigare  framförallt  studerats  ur  ett  individuellt  perspektiv  men  vi  vet  mindre  om  hur  stereotyper  och  de  universella  stereotypdimensionerna  kompetens  och  värme  (Cuddy,   Fiske   &   Glick,   2008)   samt   fördomar   kommuniceras   och   förhandlas   när  beslut   fattas   i   grupp.  Ett   sätt   att   studera  detta   är   genom  att  undersöka  hur  olika  kontext   kan   leda   till   olika   kommunikationsmönster,   olika   erfarenheter,   och   olika  beslut.  I  denna  avhandling  varieras  kontexten  på  två  olika  sätt  i  två  experiment.  I  det   första   experimentet   där   deltagarna   hade   till   uppgift   att   sätta   samman   ett  stafettlag  varierades  målinriktningen.  Ett  tävlingsinriktat  mål  jämförs  med  ett  mål  inriktat  mot  gemenskap.  I  det  andra  experimentet  användes  olika  information  som  kontextvariabel.  Detta  gjordes  genom  att   information  om  genus  och   föräldraskap  för   en   fiktiv   aspirant   till   ett   jobb   som   konsult   i   en   fiktiv   anställningssituation  varierades.   Därtill   studerades   i   båda   experimenten   vilka   effekter   gruppers  könssammansättning  och  deltagarnas  genus  kan   spela   i   en  beslutsprocess.  Dessa  faktorer  förväntades  påverka  hur  beslutsfattande  grupperna  resonerade,  innehållet  i   kommunikation   påverkades   i   termer   av   stereotyper   och   hur   beslutsfattande-­‐‑processen   upplevdes.   För   att   ge   en   bredare   bild   användes   både   kvalitativ   och  kvantitativ  data  som  kopplades  samman  med  gruppernas  givna  målsättning.    

I   artikel   I   analyserades   formen   av   kommunikation   genom   en   interaktions-­‐‑processanalys   (IPA),   här   skilde   det   inte   mycket   mellan   de   två   olika  målsättningarna.  Däremot  visade  en  innehållsanalys  ett  mer  systematiskt  mönster.  

Page 11: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

 

Grupper   med   tävlingsinriktade   mål   använde   sig   av   både   inkludering   och  exkludering   genom   användande   av   såväl   positiva   som   negativa   stereotyper.  Grupper   med   en   gemenskapsinriktad   målsättning   använde   inkluderande  målsättningar   och   enbart   positiva   stereotyper.   I   artikel   II   där   målet   var   att  undersöka   vad   som   skapar   en   framgångsrik   beslutsprocess   visade   det   sig   att  upplevelsen   av   en   jämlik   beslutsprocess   spelar   stor   roll.   En   kvalitativ   analys   av  konversationsargumenten  (CACS)  stärkte  denna  slutsats.  

 I   artikel   III,   där   informationen   av   genus   och   föräldraskap   för   de   fiktiva  aspiranterna  varierades,  visade  det  sig  att  föräldraskap  gav  upphov  till  omfattande  diskussion.  Deltagarna  gjorde   ingen   större   skillnad  mellan  mammor  och  pappor,  men  de  använde  sig  av  attribut  relaterade  till  stereotypdimensionerna  värme  och  kompetens  olika  beroende  på  om  de  talade  om  mammor  eller  pappor.  I  artikel  IV  användes  data   från   båda   experimenten   för   att   undersöka  hur   kontextvariablerna  och   könssammansättning   av   grupperna   påverkade   hur   beslutssituationen  upplevdes.   Resultaten   visar   att   kontextvariablerna   och   könssammansättning  samspelar   med   deltagarens   eget   genus.   Män   upplevde   sig   mer   nöjda   när   de  arbetade   tillsammans  med   andra  män   som   hade   en  manlig   target   person   och   en  fiktiv  manlig   förälder  att   ta   ställning   till.  Kvinnor  kände  sig  mer  nöjda   i  grupper  bestående  av  både  kvinnor  och  män  som  diskuterade  om  en  kvinnlig  förälder.    

Kontext  spelade  en  stor  roll  eftersom  variationen  av  information  ledde  till  olika  mönster   av   hur   stereotyper   användes   i   diskussionerna.   Vidare,   köns-­‐‑sammansättningen   av   grupperna   fungerade   på   ett   liknande   sätt.   Resultaten  diskuteras  i  relation  till  hur  detta  kan  påverka  beslutsfattandesituationer.  Slutligen  gavs  förslag  till  framtida  forskning  inom  området.    

                             

Page 12: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

xii  

 LIST OF PAPERS This  thesis  is  mainly  based  on  the  following  four  papers,  herein  referred  to  by  their  Roman  numerals:    

 Paper  I     Löfstrand,   P.,   &   Zakrisson,   I.   (2014).   Competitive   versus   non-­‐‑   competitive   goals   in   group   decision-­‐‑making.   Small  Group  Research,     45  (4),  451-­‐‑464.    

Paper  II   Löfstrand,   P.,   (2015).   Conversational   arguments   in   small   group     decision-­‐‑making:   reasoning   activity   and   perceived   influence   over     the  decision  are  keys  for  success.  In  R.  Thornberg  &  T.  Jungert  (Eds.),     Independent   in   the   heard:   Inclusion   and   exclusion   as   social     processes:   Proceedings   from   the   9th   GRASP   conference,   Linköping     University   (pp.64-­‐‑81).   (Research   report   in   electronics)   (Linköping     Electronic  Conference  Proceedings).    

 Paper  III   Löfstrand,   P.,   &   Zakrisson,   I.   (2017).   “What   about   the   child     issue?”  Group  negotiations   of   gender   and  parenthood   contracts   in     recruitment  situations,  Society,  Health  &  Vulnerability,  8  (1),  19-­‐‑30.    

Paper  IV   Zakrisson,   I.,   &   Löfstrand,   P.   (2018).   The   multidimensionality   of     gender   –   implications   for   group-­‐‑based   decision   making.   Manuscript     submitted  

Page 13: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

1  

1. PREFACE ”With   the   possible   exception   of   childbearing,   no   aspect   of   social   life   is  more   strongly  associated  with  gender  than  power.”          (Pratto  &  Walker,  p.242,  2004)    ”People  who  work  together  will  win,  whether  it  be  against  complex  football  defenses,  or  the  problems  of  modern  society.”         (Vince  Lombardi,  football  coach,  Benson,  p.217,  2008)  

 It   is   a   privilege   to   be   part   of   a   free   research   community   and   to   have   the  opportunity  to  communicate  research  results  as  well  as  to  defend  them.  The  right  to   express   our   research   and   take   part   in   the   political   debate  might   be   taken   for  granted.  However,  many  researchers   today   live  and  work   in   societies  where   free  research  and  freedom  of  speech  are  a  distant  vision.  For  us  to  have  the  opportunity  to   write   and   do   research   on   what   we   think   is   necessary,   without   control   from  authorities,   is   important   if   we   are   to   continue   our   research   applied   to   human  interaction   and   communication   to   shed   light   on   different   democratic   dilemmas.  My  thesis   is  connected  to  the  basics  of  social  psychology;  how  human  interaction  affects  our  views  of   each  other,   and  how,   in  a  broader   sense,   it  might   also  affect  society.  

During   the  years   I  have  worked  on   this   thesis   I  have  often   thought  about   the  philosophical  dilemma,  “Which  came  first,  the  chicken  or  the  egg?”  This  is  something  I  am   reminded   of   daily;   both   by   myself   but   also   by   people   I   meet.     I   often   hear  questions  like:  ”Why  are  you  in  so  active  in  a  sports  club?”  “Why  do  you  spend  so  much  time  getting  involved  in  social  issues?”,  and  so  on.    

Certainly,  when  I  started  writing,  I  was  already  to  some  extent  involved  in  non-­‐‑profit  work  in  different  associations.  I  was  a  football  coach  for  one  of  my  children’s  team   and   I   was   also   a   nominee   for   a   laidback   position   in   the   local   elections.  However,   during   the   time   I   have   been   working   with   this   thesis,   my   non-­‐‑profit  commitments  has  expanded  a  lot.  One  led  to  another  and  today  I  am  chairman  of  one   of   the   regional   football   clubs,   while   also   being   engaged   in   the   municipal  council   as  a  deputy   chairman  of   the  Municipality  Educational  Board.   I   also  have  various  missions  at  the  national  political  level.  So  let  us  go  back  and  try  to  answer  the  initial  question,  ”which  came  first,  the  egg  or  the  chicken?”  As  a  graduate  student  in  psychology,  with,  as   teenagers  would  say,   ‘a  morbid’   interest   in   the  nuances  of  communication,   I   am   thinking,   “it   requires   a   deeper   understanding   outside   the  laboratory  to  get  a  better  grip  of  my  research  questions”.  My  ambitions  are  inspired  by  Kurt   Lewin,   willing   to   face   theoretical   issues   to   get   the   necessary   in-­‐‑depth  

Page 14: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

2  

knowledge  of  practical  implications  of  group  decision-­‐‑making  and  its  implicit  and  explicit   outcomes.   For  my   case,   I   am   not   sure  whether   the   scientific   interest   has  increased  my  civic  interest  or  if  it  is  the  other  way  around.  This  curiosity  and  wish  for  a  deeper  understanding  has  of  course  influenced  me  in  my  activities  in  society.    

2. INTRODUCTION

When  people   interact  with  each  other,  e.g.   in  decision-­‐‑making  situations,   they  sometimes  discuss  and  evaluate  individuals  who  are  not  present  in  the  room.  This  is  done   in  more  or   less   formal  situations.   Informally  when  friends  discuss  absent  friends,   and   formally,   for   example,   in   team   selections   or   recruitment   situations  where   decision-­‐‑makers   evaluate   and   make   decisions   about   candidates.   In   every  situation  when  people  speak  of  persons  not  present  they  use  a  number  of  attributes  applied   to   describe   the   absentees   and   to   justify   opinions   of   how   they   can   be  expected  to  act.  Those  attributes  are  related  to  the  stereotype  dimensions  warmth  and   competence   (Fiske,  Cuddy,  Glick  &  Xu,   2002).   The   two  dimensions   together  form   “the   stereotype   content  model”   (Fiske, et al., 2002).   Stereotypes   are   something  that  helps  people  use  simplified  categorizations  or  a  collection  of  characteristics  to  understand  other  people  (Allport,  1954).    Stereotypes  are  beliefs  and  they  serve  the  purpose   of   identifying   and   discriminating   people   from   other   groups   based   on  attributes   such   as   age,   gender,   occupation,   race,   etc.   (Haslam,   Turner,   Oakes,  Reynolds,  &  Doosje,  2002;  Tajfel,  1981).  In  other  words,  stereotypes  help  a  person  understand  how  another  person  might  act  or  behave  without  making  a  totally  new  evaluation   each   time   he/she  meets   a   new   person.   Stereotypes   are   biased   against  another   person   according   to  which   group   he/she   belongs   to   (Fiske,   et   al.,   2002).    Furthermore,  stereotypes  assist  people  in  defending  or  rationalizing  their  feelings  and  behaviors  (Jost  &  Major,  2001).  They  are  easy  to  adapt  but  harder  to  avoid  and  are  at  high  risk  to  be  used  in  situations  when  people  feel  threatened  or  experience  anxiety  and  stress  (Macrae,  Hewstone  &  Griffiths,  1993).  Groups  with  people  that  are  associated  farthermost  away  from  power  in  society  are  rated  the  lowest  on  the  two   stereotype   dimensions.   Elderly   and   disabled   people   are   positioned   high   in  warmth  but   low  on   the   competence  dimension.  People  with  a  business   career  or  people   in   a   power   position   are   often   positioned   high   in   competence   but   low   in  warmth.  Groups  that  are  positioned  the  highest  on  both  dimensions  belong  to  the  dominant   norm   group   in   society,   the   in-­‐‑groups,   or   “us”   (Fiske,   Cuddy  &  Glick,  2007).  Previous  research  has  also  revealed  that  the  stereotype  dimensions  are  stable  across   cultures   (Fiske   et   al,   2007).   However,   different   groups   are   positioned  differently  in  different  cultures  (Fiske  et  al,  2002).    Another  view  is  that  how  people  view  other  people  is  a  constantly  ongoing  process.  Attitudes  and  opinions  change  

Page 15: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

3  

all  the  time,  even  within  the  same  individual  and  in  the  same  conversation  (Potter,  1988).  How  people  view  the  world  around  them  and  how  they  view  other  people  are   factors   built   in   since   early   childhood   (Gergen,   2015)   through   the   interaction  with  parents,  neighbors,   friends  and  teachers  and  later   in   life  through  politicians,  managers   and   colleagues   (Summers,   2017).   People   are   not   aware   of   their  attribution   processes,   as   these   are   quick   and   occur   automatically   (Ambady   &  Rosenthal,  1992;  Bodenhausen  &  Macrae,  1998;  McNeill  &  Burton,  2002).    

One   of   the   first   things   people   notice  when   they  meet   a   new   person   is  which  gender   the  person   is  associated  with   (Fiske,  Haslam  &  Fiske,  1991).  This  starts   in  early  age  (Lindsey,  2012)  and  children  are  framed  by  gender  from  early  childhood  (Ridgeway,   2011).  This  means   that  gender   is  one  of   the  most  general   stereotypes  permeating  people’s  perceptions.  Depending  on  how  gender  relevant  information  is  framed  in  a  situation  it  leads  to  biased  judgments  of  women  and  men  (Fiske,  et  al.,  2002;  Okimoto  &  Heilman,  2012).    

Not  only  are  men  and  women   judged  differently,   it   is  also  possible   that   there  are   differences   between  men   and  women   in   how   they   perceive   and   judge   other  people.   For   example,   men   have   been   found   to   have   higher   levels   of   social  dominant   attitudes   and   prejudice   than  women   (Pratto,   Sidanius   &   Levin,   2006).  But  it  is  also  possible  that  what  is  seen  as  differences  at  individual  level  is  indeed  a  result   of   what   is   expected   due   to   norms   in   society   and   perhaps   that   men   and  women  positioned  in  the  same  gendered  situation  act  in  the  same  way.  It  has  been  found  that  men  and  women  in  situations  with  an  asymmetric  distribution  of  men  have   similar   social  attitudes   (Zakrisson,  2008).   In  addition,  previous   research  has  revealed   differences   in   performance   and   interaction   dependent   on   gender  composition   in  decision-­‐‑making   (Apesteguia,  Azmat,  &   Iriberri,   2012;  Fenwick  &  Neal,   2001;   Hannagan   &   Larimer,   2010;   Van   Vugt,   De   Cremer,   &   Janssen,   2007;  Raghubir  &  Valenzuela,  2010).    

Sometimes  humans  are  described  as  “social  animals”  (Aronson,  1984),  meaning  that  people  are  more  or  less  born  into  groups  that  they  live  in  and  work  in  (Gavac,  Murrar   &   Brauer,   2017).   Working   together   with   other   people   and   making   joint  decisions  in  groups  is  also  something  that  starts  in  early  childhood.  This  continues  through   the   educational   system   and   later   through  work   life,   where   group  work  and   joint   decision-­‐‑making   takes   an   important   part.   Social   psychological   research  on  groups  and  decision-­‐‑making  has  a  long  record  and  research  has  been  conducted  both  in   laboratory  settings,  with  temporal,  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups  (Ahn,  Ostrom,  Schmidt  &  Walker,   2003)   and   in   the   field   on   real   decision-­‐‑making   groups   (Barr   &   Gold,  2014).   In   the   laboratory   it   is   common   with   experimental   designs   that   focus   on  different   parts   of   the   decision-­‐‑making   process,   e.g.   free   riding   effects   (Kameda,  Tsukasaki,   Hastie,   &   Berg,   2011),   majority   and   minority   influence   (Sinaceur,  

Page 16: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

4  

Thomas-­‐‑Hunt,  Neale,  O'ʹNeill,  &  Haag,  2010),  or  framing  effects  (Leong,  McKenzie,  Sher  &  Müller-­‐‑Trede,  2017).    

Research   conducted   in   the   field   sometimes   focuses   on   processes   within   the  group   (Wheelan,   2009)   or   on   factors   such   as   group   climate   (Schultz,   Sjøvold   &  Andre,   2017)   or   with   an   ambition   to   achieve   a   change   (Lewin,   1947).   Lewin’s  research   has   been   important   in   the   work   with   group   dynamics   and   even   if   not  often   cited   it   is   still   in   use   (Hermann,   2015).   When   you   are   studying   group  interaction  in  experimental  situations  it  is  hard  not  to  think  of  what  it  is  that  makes  decision-­‐‑making   processes   more   or   less   successful.   The   results   of   a   “good   group  process”   can   be   measured   in   terms   of   achievement.   How   much   did   the   group  achieve   (van  Woerkom  &  Sanders,  2010)  or  how  efficient  were   they   (Lira,  Ripoll,  Peiró,   Zornoza,   2008),   or   if   different   groups   might   outperform   others   (Hong   &  Page,   2004).   Another   approach   to   study   group   processes   is   to   study   democratic  factors  in  the  decision-­‐‑making  process,  such  as  majority  vs.  minority  influence  and  how   much   influence   each   participant   has   over   different   parts   of   the   session  (Meyers,  Brashers  &  Hanner,  2000;  Nemeth  &  Goncalo,  2005).    

This   short   introduction   leads   us   to   the   core   of   this   thesis.  How   do   people   in  groups   negotiate   and   how   are   stereotypes   constructed   in   different   contexts?   Do  women  and  men  communicate  and  negotiate  stereotypes  differently?  Are  decision-­‐‑making   groups   affected   by   the   gender   of   the   object?   Does   gender   composition  affect  communication  and  communication  patterns?  Moreover,  there  is  a  need  for  studies   on   communication   in   experimental   settings   to   control   how   stereotyping  mechanisms   take   shape   in   different   situations.   Experimental   designs   have   an  advantage  over  field  studies  as  they  offer  a  better  opportunity  to  control  threats  to  the  internal  validity  (Moyer  &  Gross,  2011).  Finally,  all  this  reasoning  leads  to  the  question  whether  communication  and  communication  patterns  might  explain  what  it  is  that  makes  a  group  decision-­‐‑making  process  more  successful.    

In  decision-­‐‑making  research,  relatively  little  empirical  attention  has  been  given  to  manipulation  of  the  goal-­‐‑settings  of  the  decision-­‐‑making  task  and  most  often  the  goal  is  expressed  in  extrinsic,  achievement  terms.  As  such,  the  task  is  set  to  appeal  to   achievement   motivation,   which   would   lead   to   less   responsiveness   to   other  people’s  views.  What  if  the  goal  was  expressed  in  intrinsic  terms,  as  for  example;  what   is   good   for   the   group”?   Would   that   lead   the   group   members   to   more  responsiveness   towards   each   other?   All   group   decision-­‐‑making   situations   are  framed   by   different   aspects,   such   as   different   outcomes   and   contingencies  associated  with  a  particular  choice  (Tversky  &  Kahneman,  1981).  One  way  to  study  framing   effects   is   to   give   groups  different   information,  which   can   be  done   in   an  experimental  setting  (e.g.  Levin,  Johnson  &  Davis,  1987).  

Page 17: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

5  

The  following  sections  in  the  thesis  aim  to  give  a  theoretical  background  to  the  main   areas   and   research   questions   that   the   thesis   rests   upon.   Section   2.1   gives   a  brief   historical   background   to   social   psychological   experimentation.     Section   2.2  discusses   groups   and   decision-­‐‑making   (GDM),   as   groups   and   decision-­‐‑making  form   the   fundamental   base   of   this   thesis.   The   chapter   also   focuses   on   framing  effects  and  raises  the  question  of  what  a  successful  decision-­‐‑making  session  could  be,  and  how  it  can  be  measured.  Section  2.3  discusses  the  concept  of  stereotypes,  as  stereotypes   are   the  main  psychological   component   studied   in   this   thesis.   Section  2.4   discusses   communication   and   how   interaction   has   an   impact   on   groups   and  decision-­‐‑making  and  how  this  might  affect  stereotyping.  Moreover,  the  purpose  of  the   chapter   is   to   describe   some   research   methods   that   can   be   used   to   conduct  research   on   group   interaction.   Next,   section   2.5   discusses   gender   and   gender  composition   and   how   it   might   have   effect   on   communication   and   stereotyping.  Chapter   3   describes   and   motivates   the   aim   of   the   thesis.   Chapter   4   handles  methodology.  In  chapter  5  are  the  empirical  papers  summarized.  Finally,  the  thesis  ends  with  a  discussion  in  chapter  6.    The  empirical  papers  can  be  read  in  full  in  the  appendix  at  the  end  of  this  thesis.    

2.1 Brief historical background to experimental social psychology

How   people   interact   and   make   decisions   together   has   interested   social  psychologists   for   more   than   a   century.   Since   the   first   social   psychological  experiment   on   social   facilitation   (how   performance   is   affected   by   the   mere  presence  of  others)  was  conducted  in  the  late  19th  century  (Triplett,  1898),  research  on   the   psychology   of   group   interaction   has   been   frequent.  Mustafa   Sherif   (1936)  was  among  the  first  to  study  intergroup  conflicts,  such  as  group  norms,  and  how  individuals  were  affected  by  conformity.    After  the  atrocities  that  took  place  during  World  War  II,  there  were  many  social  psychologists  (e.g.  Asch,  1956;  Lewin,  1951;  Milgram,  1974)  that  understood  that  it  was  important,  using  scientific  methods,  to  reach   a   better   understanding   of   the   acts   carried   out   by   nations,   soldiers   and  civilians   during   the   war.   How   could   a   democratic   country   vote   for   a   political  leadership   that   ended   up   carrying   out   genocide   and  war   against   other   nations?  How   could   soldiers   commit   the  most   heinous   crimes?   These   questions  were   the  starting  point  for  studies  with  ethically  problematic  designs  by  modern  standards.  Kurt  Lewin  (Lewin  &  Lippit,  1938;  Lewin,  1947),  conducted  field  studies   through  action  research  with  the  aim  to  achieve  a  change  in  people'ʹs  attitudes,  values  and  behavior.   It  was   also   in   the   aftermath   of   industrialization   that   Lewin   along  with  Lippit   began   studying   various   forms   of   leadership   (Billig,   2015;   Lewin  &  Lippit,  1938;  Lippit,  1940;  Lewin,  Lippit  &  White,  1939).    

Page 18: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

6  

Later,   social   psychological   researchers   moved   into   the   laboratory   and   in  experimental   settings   studied   different   components   of   human   behavior   and  interaction.   The   famous   conformity   studies   that   were   conducted   in   the   1950s  demonstrated  our  human  willingness   to   repeat  other  people’s  behavior  and  how  difficult   it   is   for   us   to   avoid   conformity   (Asch,   1956).   Other   experiments   were  related  to  obedience  in  the  face  of  authority  (Milgram,  1974).  Even  if  these  studies  were  ethically  questionable,  they  gave  us  important  knowledge  about  how  people  obey   authority   and   diminish   themselves   in   decision-­‐‑making   situations.   In   the  aftermath   of   the   obedience   experiment,   an   essential   discussion   started   about  research  ethics  and  what  a  researcher  can  expose  a  subject  to  in  a  study  (Russell  &  Picard  2013).  

One   often   mentioned   quasi-­‐‑experiment   was   the   Stanford   Prison   Experiment   in  which   university   students   were   randomly   assigned   to   the   roles   of   prisoners   or  prison  guards  (Zimbardo,  1973;  2007).  The  purpose  of  this  experiment  was  to  study  how  the  participants  were  shaped  by  their  roles.  The  results  were  that  they  quickly  fell   into   destructive   patterns,   as   the   participants   who   acted   as   prison   guards  abused   the   participants   who   were   set   to   act   as   prisoners.   The   experiment   was  canceled  in  advance  after  just  six  days  for  safety  reasons.    

All   these  experiments  are   important,  not  only   from  a  research  perspective  but  also  from  the  perspective  of  work  life,  as  people  often  work  together  in  groups  and  struggle  with   factors   such   as   conformity   and   obedience.     Thus   it   is   important   to  perform   experimental   research   on   groups   to   investigate   the   interaction   between  the  individual  in  the  group.    

2.2 Group decision-making (GDM) Among  common  definitions  of  a  group  is  that  it  needs  to  be  composed  of  more  

than   one  member   (Brown,   1999).   Furthermore,   to   become   a   group,   the  members  need   some   kind   of   social   relationship   (Forsyth,   2006).   Moreover,   a   group   by  definition  also  needs  one  or  more  joint  goals  (Lennéer-­‐‑Axelsson  &  Thylefors,  2018).    

Working  together  with  other  people  and  making   joint  decisions  starts  early   in  life   and   continues   through   the   educational   system.   It   also   follows   people   into  working   life,   where   collaboration   and   decision-­‐‑making   play   an   important   role.  Groups   are   important   at   all   levels,   from   the   factory   floor   to   the   boardrooms   of  multi-­‐‑national  companies.    

How   people   communicate,   negotiate   and   make   decisions   together   has  interested   social   psychologists   for   more   than   a   century.   Early   in   the   1920’s,  research  was  conducted  on  group  influence  (Allport,  1920).  Previous  research  has  revealed  a  number  of  advantages  and  disadvantages  with  group  decision-­‐‑making  compared   to   individual   decision-­‐‑making.   The  main   reason   to  make   decisions   in  

Page 19: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

7  

groups  is  to  take  advantage  of  the  different  experiences,  strengths  and  expertise  of  the  members  (Simons,  Pelled  &  Smith,  1999).  Another  purpose  is  to  evaluate  more  alternatives   (Nemeth   &   Nemeth-­‐‑Brown,   2003).   This   could   have   a   democratic  advantage,   as   more   people   feel   that   they   understand   and   are   involved   in   the  decisions  (Hinsz  &  Nickell,  2004).  There  are  also  many  possible  disadvantages  that  must   be   handled   properly.   The   process   is   generally   time-­‐‑consuming  when  more  people   are   involved   in   the  decision-­‐‑making  process.   There   is   also   a   risk   that   the  members  minimize  themselves  and  just  act  the  way  the  leader  wants  them  to  (Janis,  1972).  Another  discovered  risk  with  group  decision-­‐‑making  is  group  polarization,  where   a   group   might   reach   more   extreme   solutions   to   a   problem   than   an  individual  would  (Myers  &  Lamm,  1976;  Van  Swol  &  Lyn,  2009).  

For   a   decision-­‐‑making   situation   to   appear,   there  must   be   different   options   to  choose  from  (Lau,  2003).  By  applying  so-­‐‑called  rational  choice  or  prisoner’s  dilemma  designs,  researchers  can  determine  how  and  why  the  decision  maker/s  act  the  way  they  do  (Brewer  &  Kramer,  1986;  Ostrom,  2003;  Russel  &  Fiske,  2008).  Such  tasks  are  often  quite  unrealistic,  demanding  a  solution  in  mathematical  (points,  grades,  tokens,   etc.)   or   financial   (money)   terms.   These   research   designs   have   improved  understanding  of  group  mechanisms,  such  as   trust  and  strategies  affecting  group  interaction   (Ahn,   et   al.,   2003).  A   common  method   to  manipulate   interaction   is   to  tell   a   subject   that   he/she   is   interacting   with   other   group   members   through   a  computer  screen,  while  they  in  fact  are  not  interacting  with  other  participants  at  all  (e.g.   Ostrom,   2003;   Yi,   2003).   Researchers   use   this   type   of   design   to   gain   a   high  level  of  control  over  the  interaction  and  to  achieve  high  internal  validity  (Moyer  &  Gross,  2011).  There  are,   in  addition,  variations  in  which  participants  interact  with  other  people,  either  colleagues  that  they  work  with  normally  or  other  persons  not  previously  known  (e.g.  Kahai,  Huang  &  Jestice,  2012;  Nawata  &  Yamaguchi,  2011).  Previous   research   has   revealed   that   most   people   begin   an   interaction  collaboratively   and   that   more   cooperation   and   trust   is   formed   when   people  interact   face-­‐‑to-­‐‑face   than   in   implicit   interaction   settings,   when   they   interact  through  a  computer  screen    (Ostrom,  2003).  It  has  also  been  found  that  differences  in   goal   settings  might   affect   decision-­‐‑making   groups.   A   group  with   competitive  goals   seems   to   reach   lower   decision   quality   than   groups  with   cooperative   goals  (Toma,  Bry  &  Butera,  2013).  Participants  given  competitive  goals  also  shared   less  information  with  each  other   (Toma,  Vasiljevic,  Oberlé  &  Butera,  2013)  and  group  members  acted  more  often  competitively  and  were  less  likely  to  share  information  with  other  participants  (Toma  &  Butera,  2015).          

Page 20: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

8  

2.2.1 Groups in real life versus ad-hoc groups? Working   groups   can   be   seen   as   a   ‘living’   system   that   goes   through   different  

stages  of  development  (Wheelan,  Davidson  &  Tilin,  2003;  Wheelan,  2005).  The  first  thing   that   happens   when   a   group   is   established   is   that   the   participants   enter   a  phase  where   they  need   to   find   acceptance   and   community  with   the   other   group  members.   The   second   stage   involves   opposition   and   conflicts.   This   occurs  when  the   group  members   feel   that   they   need   to   conform   to   fit   into   the   group.   In   this  stage,   disagreement   and   conflict   occur   that   sometimes   lead   to   sub-­‐‑groups.   If   the  group   can   handle   the   second   stage,   they   enter   stage   three.   This   stage   is  characterized  by  tolerance  and  structure.  Group  members  will  understand  that  the  individuals   in   the   group   have   different   needs,   and   feelings   of   confidence   grow.  Conflicts   are   worked   through   and   different   roles   are   formed   in   relation   to   the  group’s  goals  and  ambitions.  The  fourth  stage  is   the  work  and  production  phase,  where  members   take   and  give   feedback,  problems  are  defined  and  group  norms  strengthen  the  quality  (Wheelan,  Murphy,  Tsumura  &  Fried-­‐‑Kline  1998;  Wheelan,  et  al.,  2003;  Wheelan,  2005).    

However  important  and  beneficial  it  is  to  study  group  processes  in  real  groups,  where   their  maturation  can  be   followed,   sometimes,   for  example  when  emergent  phenomena   are   in   focus,   ad-­‐‑hoc   groups   are   preferred.   To   study   mechanisms  within  the  group  and  factors  such  as  status,  gender  and  power  there   is  a  need  to  make   controlled   studies   to   reach   a   higher   level   of   internal   validity   even   if   there  may  be  problems  with  ecological  validity.      2.2.2 What makes a group decision-making process effective, successful and democratic?

A   considerable   body   of   research   has   studied   group   effectiveness   or   group  successfulness   (DeStephen   &   Hirowaka,   1998;   Kong,   Konzak   &   Bottom,   2015,  Michie  &  Williams,   2003).   But  what   do   those   concepts  mean   and  what   is   a   good  group  result?    The  ambition  here  is  to  sort  these  questions  out  and  briefly  discuss  the  concept  of  group  successfulness.  

 Satisfied   group  members   are   an   important   factor   for   a   group   to   reach   good  results  (DeStephen  &  Hirokawa,  1998).  Besides  satisfaction,  experienced  influence  also  leads  to  positive  outcomes  (Kong,  et  al.,  2015;  Michie  &  Williams,  2003).  Other  studies   have   found   that   negative   experiences   and   lack   of   influence   lead   to  frustration  and  conflict,  making  it  difficult  for  the  group  to  move  forward  (Mason  &  Griffin,  2002;  2003;  Spector,  1988).  When  participants  perceive  that  information  is  exchanged   generously,   it   leads   the   group   to  make   correct   decisions,   while   poor  exchange   of   information   has   the   opposite   effect   (Larson,   Christensen,   Franz,   &  Abbot,  1998).  Extensive  information-­‐‑sharing  processes  are  also  found  to  strengthen  

Page 21: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

9  

the   quality   of   the   decisions   (Peterson,   Owens,   Tetlock,   Fan,   &  Martorana,   1998;  Tasa  &  Whyte,  2005).    

There  are  many  different  aspects   that  have  an   impact  on  group  efficiency.  An  efficient   process   is   not   only   one   where   the   decision   is   reached   in   the   shortest  amount  of  time.  For  a  decision-­‐‑making  process  to  be  efficient,   there  also  needs  to  be   a   constructive   communication   process   (Kong,   et   al.,   2015).   A   quick   process  without   reasoning,   questioning   and   objecting   might   obstruct   the   group   from  achieving   their   goals   (Gelfand,   Major,   Raver,   Nishii   &   O’Brien,   2006;   Graziano,  Jensen-­‐‑Campbell  &  Hair,  1996).    

Several   components   are   essential   for   a  group   to  be   successful,   such  as   correct  understanding   of   the   problem,   what   the   group   requires   to   reach   the   best  alternatives  and  how  they  evaluate  them  (Gouran  &  Hirokawa,  1996;  Kolbe  &  Boos,  2009).  Previously,  research  has  revealed  different  results  according  to  how  factors  in   the   group   interaction   affect   decision-­‐‑making   in   groups.   Some   studies   debates  about  if  and  how  differences  in  opinion  between  the  group  members  have  positive  effects   (Schweiger,   Sandberg   &   Ragan,   1986;   Simons,   Pelled   &   Smith   1999;  Slotegraaf   &   Atuahene-­‐‑Gima,   2011).   Others   argue   that   different   opinions   in   the  group   likely   have   a   positive   impact   on   the   group   results   (Barr   &   Gold,   2014;  Gouran,   1982).   Moreover,   other   researchers   claim   that   different   opinions   might  lead   to   pressure,   frustration   and   tension   amongst   the   group   members   (Behfar,  Mannix,  Peterson  &  Trochim,  2011;  De  Dreu  &  Weingart,   2003;   Jehn,  1995).  With  these   conflicting  arguments   it   is   easy   to  argue   for   the  need   for  more   research  on  group  dynamics  conducted  in  controlled  situations.    

How   efficient   or   successful   the   group   process   becomes   also   depends   on   the  definition   of   the   outcome.   In   psychological   experimental   research   this   is   seldom  problematized,  and  the  outcome  is  often  measured  in  achievement  terms  (Brewer  &  Kramer,  1986;  Ostrom,  2003).  The  need  for  studies  with  different  goal  terms  has  been  argued  for  previously  (Kray  &  Thompson,  2005).  

How  group  members  experience  the  opportunity  for  influence  is  an  important  factor   in   decision-­‐‑making   (Baird  &  Wang,   2010).  However,   it   is   also   essential   to  take   into   account   that   influence   might   look   differently   in   various   parts   of   the  decision-­‐‑making  process.   There  might   be   a   difference   between  how  people   view  influence   over   the   actual   decision   and   how   they   perceive   influence   over   the  decision   process.   Research   thus   needs   to   take   into   consideration   influence   over  both   decision   outcome   and  decision   process.  Next   question   is  whether   influence  should  be  studied  in  an  objective  sense  or  from  a  subjective  perspective.  To  study  influence  in  an  objective  sense  can  be  done  by  observing  the  group  interaction  (e.g.  Schmid  Mast,   2001).  On   the  other  hand,   if  we  ask   the  participants  we  get  a   first-­‐‑hand   account   of   the   group   process   and   how   the   influence  was   experienced.   An  

Page 22: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

10  

observer  can  evaluate  the  group  as  a  whole  but  cannot  judge  what  the  participants  experience   as   individuals  during   the  decision-­‐‑making  process.  As  participants   in  the   same   group   can   have   different   experiences   it   is   important   to   capture   these  differences  by  asking  for  their  evaluations  of  different  parts  of  the  decision-­‐‑making  process.  

Framing  is  a  cognitive  bias  when  people  react  differently  depending  on  how  a  question  or  a  problem  is  explained  (Tversky  &  Kahneman,  1981).  Previous  research  has  shown  that  differences  in  framing  have  fundamental  effects  on  the  way  people  make   decisions   (Tversky   &   Kahneman,   1981).   Framing   helps   people   handle  complex   information.   Our   cognitive   processes   are   framed   since   childhood   and  when   we   receive   information   or   facts   that   fit   into   the   frame,   it   is   strengthened.  However,  when  we  encounter   information  or   facts   that  do  not   fit   into   the   frame,  we  may  ignore  it.  The  psychological  term  for  this  is  cognitive  dissonance  (Festinger,  1962).   How   information   is   framed   affects   how   people   make   their   decisions  (Kahneman   &   Tversky,   1981).   The   results   from   Tversky   and   Kahneman’s  experiments   have   been   replicated   and   it   has   been   found   that   framing   has  major  impact   on   the   decision-­‐‑makers   (Gächter,   Orzen,   Renner   &   Starmer,   2009;  Kühberger   &   Tanner,   2010).   Framing   effects   has   also   been   tested   on   groups   in  experimental   situations   (e.g.   Curseu,   Schruijer,   &   Fodor,   2016;   Rees,   2014).   Rees  (2014)  revealed  that  groups  who  received  instructions  with  a  deadline  were  better  to  predict  successfulness.    Curseu,  et  al.  (2016)  studied  different  rules  in  decision-­‐‑making   groups   and   they   found   that   a   collaborative   decision   rule   decreased   the  sensitivity  of  framing  effects  in  decision-­‐‑making.      

Examples   that  demonstrate   the   role  of   framing  on  social   judgments   in  groups  sometimes   involve  role-­‐‑playing   (Lawson,  McDonough,  &  Bodle,  2010;  McGregor,  1993).  The   most   famous   example   is   probably   “A   Class   Divided”,   an   experiment  conducted   in   the   seventies   by   Jane   Elliot   (Peters,   1987)   in   which   children   were  divided  into  two  different  groups  and  treated  differently  based  on  fictive  eye  color.  Follow-­‐‑up  studies  have  confirmed   that   role-­‐‑playing  activities  affect  attitudes  and  behavior   even   if   the   activity   causes   stress   to   the   participants   (Byrnes   &   Kiger,  1990).  The  Stanford  Prison  Experiment  also  revealed  how  easily  people  adopt  roles  and   fall   into  destructive  patterns   (Zimbardo,  2007).  These  examples  of   the  classic  scientific   and   non-­‐‑scientific   experiments   on   how   people   are   affected   by   framing  factors  teach  us  a  lot  about  how  easily  people  adapt  to  stereotypic  norms  without  questioning  their  validity.    

Different   aspects   frame   group   decision-­‐‑making   situations;   among   them   are  different  outcomes   (Tversky  &  Kahneman,  1981).  For  groups   in   sports   the  aim   is  often  to  perform  well  and  to  achieve  good  results.  For  a  group  of  students  at  school  the  goal  might  be  good  grades,  and  for  the  board  of  a  multi-­‐‑national  company  the  

Page 23: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

11  

goal   could   be   a   high   yield   and   solid   financial   performance.   The   goals   also   often  have  a  cooperative  character,  to  have  fun  and  to  develop  together.  Likely,  thoughts  and   feelings   about   the  group  processes  will   be   affected  by  which  goals   are  most  prominent   in   the   group.   How   goals   for   the   decision-­‐‑making   situation   are  conceptualized   then   form   the   context   against   which   the   participants   make   their  judgments   One   context   that   have   been   elaborated   on   are   competition   vs.  cooperation.  A  more  competitive  context  fosters  a  will  to  gain  a  positive  individual  outcome,  while  a  more  cooperative  context  leads  to  mutual  gain  (van  Knippenberg,  van   Knippenberg   &   Wilke,   2001).   Time   limit   also   influences   the   willingness   to  cooperate.  Lack  of  time  has  also  been  found  to  force  groups  to  be  more  cooperative  even   in   competitive   settings   (Cone   &   Rand,   2014).   It   has   also   been   found   that  decision-­‐‑makers   in   a   competitive   environment   feel   less   trust   when   crucial  information   is   absent   than   in   a   cooperative   environment   (Rode,   2010).   These  examples   indicate   that   goals   serves   as   context   and   that   they   impact   how  people  behave  and  experience  group  decision  processes.    

2.3 Stereotypes, attitudes and prejudice Social   psychologists   have   for   many   years   been   studying   attitudes   and   the  

effects  they  have  on  people,  interaction  and  society.  One  definition  of  an  attitude  is  that   it   is  an   evaluative   judgment   about   a   stimulus   object,   that   attitudes   differ   in  valence   and   strength,   and   that   attitude   objects   can   be   anything   that   is   liked   or  disliked   (Maio  &  Haddock,   2014).   To   complicate   it   further,   attitudes   can   also   be  seen   as   context-­‐‑driven,   which   mean   that   they   are   influenced   by   what   groups  individuals   belong   to   and   are   affected   by   (Erber,   Hodges   &   Wilson,   1995).  Moreover,   other   social   psychologists   argue   that   attitudes   are   not   stable   and   that  they   can   change   even  within   the   same   individual   and   in   the   same   conversation  (Potter,  1987).  It  is  also  necessary  to  take  into  consideration  that  the  strength  of  the  attitude  varies  and  that  this  is  critical  for  how  it  might  change  (Howe  &  Krosnick,  2017).   In   other   words,   meetings   with   other   people   influence   attitudes   and   they  might   change   when   people   communicate.   This,   in   turn,   might   explain   how   the  view  of  a  social  issue  can  change  among  a  group  of  people  over  time.     2.3.1 Stereotyping

Stereotyping   is   the   use   of   simplified   categorizations   or   a   collection   of  characteristics   describing   people   from   other   groups;   the   main   function   of  stereotypes  is  to  help  people  categorize  and  understand  others  (Allport,  1954).  It  is  an  automatic  process  and  stereotypes  based  on  social  group  are  easily  applied   in  interaction  with   others   (Brauer,   Judd  &   Jacquelin   2001;   Thompson,   Judd  &   Park  2000).   Stereotypes   are   easy   to   adopt   but   harder   to   avoid.   They   often   occur   in  

Page 24: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

12  

situations  where  a  person  feels  threatened  or  experiences  anxiety  or  stress  (Macrae,  et   al.,   1993).   One   such   situation   where   this   happens   is   in   decision-­‐‑making  (Bodenhausen,   Mussweiler,   Gabriel   &   Moreno,   2001;   Bodenhausen,   2005).   This  happens   because   avoidance   of   adoption   of   stereotypes   requires  mental   capacity,  which   must   be   focused   on   the   situational   demands   at   hand.   Moreover,   an  important   cognitive   function   of   stereotypes   is   that   they   defend   or   rationalize  feelings  and  behaviors   (Jost  &  Major,   2001).   Stereotypes   can  place   the  object   in   a  worse   or   a   better   position   than   he   or   she   deserves   depending   on   the   group   or  groups  that  the  individual  is  associated  with  (Fiske,  2010).      

As   simplified   group   based   categorizations   that   are   automatically   applied,  stereotypes   easily   lead   to   prejudiced   judgment   and   behaviors.   Prejudice   can   be  described   as   an   attitude   built   up   by   an   affective,   cognitive   and   behavioral  component  (Fiske,  2014).  One  explanation  for  why  people  hold  negative  attitudes  such  as  prejudice  is  that  the  world  is  often  seen  as  competitive,  and  that  prejudice  works   as   a   tool   for   a   group   to  maintain   a   higher   position   over   other   groups   in  society  (Dovidio,  Hewstone,  Glick,  &  Esses,  2010).  The  same  reasoning  is  found  in  the   social   dominance   theory,   which   claims   that   people   are   biased   to   various  degrees  due  to  structural  factors  such  as  the  power  over  social  resources  (Sidanius  &   Pratto,   1999).   Thus,   the   reason   to   hold   prejudiced   attitudes   towards   certain  groups   is   found   in   the   need   for   hegemonic   groups   to   uphold   power   differences  (Pratto,  et  al,  2006).  This  means   that  which  groups  will  be   the   target  of  prejudice  depends  on  the  power  distribution  within  the  society  and  may  thus  vary.  However,  one   such   power   distribution   not   supposed   to   vary   between   societies   is   the   one  between   men   and   women   (Pratto,   et   al.,   2006),   indicating   that   power   relations  between   men   and   women   are   stable   and   difficult   to   change,   and   hence   the  stereotypes  connected  to  them.        2.3.2 The stereotype dimensions warmth and competence

According  to  the  Stereotype  Content  Model  (SCM)  a  stereotype  consists  of  the  two  dimensions  warmth  and  competence  (Fiske  et  al,  2002;  Cuddy,  Fiske  &  Glick,  2004).  Both  dimensions  are  measurable  and  have  been  used  in  over  150  studies  (i.e.  Fiske,  2012;  Durante,  Tablante  &  Fiske,  2017).  The  competence  dimension  refers  to  the  extent  to  which  a  group  of  people  is  seen  as  more  or  less  efficient,  organized  or  ambitious.  The  warmth  dimension  refers  to  the  extent  to  which  a  group  is  seen  as  more  or  less  trustworthy,  friendly  or  emotional.  The  two  dimensions  together  form  four   different   positions   (Fiske,   2015):   Groups   rated   high   on   both   dimensions  belong  to   the   in-­‐‑group  members’  allies  or  reference  groups.  Groups  rated   low  on  both  dimensions  are  furthest  away  from  power  positions  in  the  society,  e.g.  poor  or  homeless  people.  Two  positions   in   the  model  describe  ambivalent  stereotypes,  as  

Page 25: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

13  

they  are  rated  as  high  in  one  dimension  and  low  on  the  other.  For  example  career  women  or  feminists  are  positioned  as  high  on  the  competent  dimension  but  low  on  warmth   dimension   and   are   seen   as   respected   but   disliked   (Eagly   &   Kite,   1987;  Glick  &  Fiske,  1996).  The  second  ambivalent  position  contain  groups  that  are  seen  as  non-­‐‑competitive,  e.g.,  older  or  disabled  people  (Cuddy  &  Fiske,  2002;  Fiske  et  al  2002).   The   stereotype   content  model   has   been   tested   in   different   cultures   and   is  found  to  be  stable  even  if  there  is  variation  regarding  which  groups  that  are  seen  as  warm  or   cold  and  more  or   less   competent   in  different   cultures   (Cuddy,   et   al.,  2009).   The   normative   and   most   dominant   groups   in   a   culture   are   always  positioned   highest   on   both  dimensions,  while   groups   furthest   away   from  power  positions  are  positioned   lowest   (Cuddy,   et   al.,   2004;  Fiske,   et   al.,   2002;  Fiske,  Xu,  Cuddy  &  Glick,  1999;  Lee  &  Fiske,  2006).  A  recent  study  conducted  in  South  East  Asia   found   that   foreigners   from   the   West   were   seen   as   more   competent   while  subgroups   from   South   East  Asia  were   seen   as   least   competent   (Ramsay  &   Pang  2017).  Another  study  has  found  that  it  is  important  to  consider  the  diversity  of  the  immigrant  population  when  studying  stereotypes;   this  as   the  stereotypes  slightly  differ   in  different  cultures   (Binggeli,  Krings,  &  Sczesny,  2014).  This   indicates   that  westerners  are  the  dominant  norm  group  not  only  in  their  own  context  but  also  all  over  the  world.  The  SCM  is  also  found  to  be  applicable  across  cultures  in  terms  of  social  class  as  people  with  high  socioeconomic  status  are  perceived  as  competent  but   cold,  while  people  with   low  socioeconomic   status  are   seen  as   less   competent  but  warmer  (Durante,  et  al.,  2017).    

The   model   is   predominantly   applied   on   individual   level   where   people   have  rated   different   groups   in   society   as   described   above.   Another   application   is   to  judge  individuals  on  these  dimensions  where  group  attributes  are  varied.  Research  in   this   respect   studied   the   effects   of   ethnicity   (Agerström,  Björklund,  Carlsson  &  Rooth,   2012),   sexual   orientation   (Everly,   Unzueta   &   Shih,   2016;   Fingerhut   &  Peplau,  2006;  Peplau  &  Fingerhut,  2004),  and  not  least  gender.  Cuddy,  et  al. (2004)  conducted  an  experiment  where  the  participants  were  set  to  rate  three  candidates  for   a   job   as   a   consultant.   The   last   candidate  was   either   described   as   a  man   or   a  woman  and  in  half  of  the  cases  information  was  added  that  the  candidate  recently  had  become  a  parent  (Cuddy,  et  al., 2004).  The  results  revealed  a  difference  in  the  judgment  of  motherhood  and  fatherhood  as  the  female  candidate  with  a  child  lost  in   competence   but   gained   in  warmth,  while   the   father   gained   in  warmth.   These  results   were   replicated   by   Heilman   and   Okimoto   (2008).   They   did   not   find   the  same  patterns   for  men   and   they   argue   that   the   ”bad  parent   assumption”   has   its  ground  in  gender  stereotyping  (Okimoto  &  Heilman,  2012)  

 As  seen  above,  the  stereotype  content  model  has  been  applied  in  experimental  situations  at  individual  level  within  different  contexts.  But,  what  will  happen  when  

Page 26: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

14  

people  meet  and  need  to  negotiate  their  personal  variants  of  such  stereotypes  in  a  decision-­‐‑making   situation?   Thus   it   would   be   of   interest   to   apply   this   model   on  communication  on  group  level.  

2.4 Communication and stereotyping The  empirical  work  in  this  thesis  leans  on  two  experiments  (described  in  section  

4.4).  Each  investigates  communication  and  how  people  negotiate  when  they  make  judgments  and  decisions  about  fictive  persons.  The  aim  with  this  section  is;  first,  to  give  a  brief  theoretical  introduction  to  why  it  is  important  to  study  communication;  and   second,   to   describe   how   communication   can   influence   perceptions   of   other  people   (stereotyping);   and   third,   to   briefly   describe   two   communication   analysis  methods  that  can  help  researchers  analyze  group  communication.    

Communication  plays  an  essential  part  of  people’s  daily  life.  People  in  general  spend  around  75  percent  of  their  waking  hours  to  communicate  with  other  people  (Tubbs,  Moss  &  Papastefanou,  2011).  Language  varies  according  to  the  context  of  the  communication  (Tubbs,  2007).  The  basic  communication  context  is  interpersonal  communication,  where   two   people   communicate   with   each   other   (Tubbs,   et   al.,  2011).   Other   contexts   are   intercultural,   small   group,   public,   organizational,   mass  communication,  and  interviewing  (Tubbs,  et  al.,  2011).  This  thesis  primary  focus  is  on  communication  in  small  groups,  which  can  be  defined  as:      “The  process  by  which  three  or  more  members  of  a  group  exchange  verbal  and  non-­‐‑

verbal  messages  in  an  attempt  to  influence  one  another”  (Tubbs,  2007,  p.5)    2.4.1 Social constructionism and small group communication

According   to   social   constructionism   (Gergen,   2015),   the   perception   of   the  outside  world  is  based  on  many  different  conditions.  For  example,  communication  with   experts,   researchers,   politicians,   teachers,   colleagues,   neighbors   and   friends  has  an   impact  on  how  people  view  the  outside  world   (Potter  &  Wetherell,  1987).  Some   facts   or   values   are   seen   as   common   sense,   and   few  would   dispute   claims  such   as   “the  world   is   round”  or   “the  world   is   divided   into  different   continents”  (Gergen,  2015).  However,  beliefs  and  stereotypes  of  groups  of  people  are  not  ‘true  facts’  as  they  are  based  on  personal  experiences  and  beliefs,  how  other  people  talk  about   groups   of   people   etc.   One   reason   for   a   social   psychologist   to   study   small  group   interaction   (communication)   is   to   get   a   better   understanding   about   how  a  phenomenon   is  constituted  (Billig,  1997),  since  how  people  communicate   impacts  the   way   they   view   the   world   (Knippendorf,   2005;   Potter   &   Wetherell,   1987).  Moreover,  the  way  people  think  is  formed  and  constructed  in  different  discourses  (Potter   &   Edwards,   2001).   The   need   for   more   knowledge   of   how   people   act   in  different   contexts   is   perhaps   more   important   than   ever,   as   the   spread   of  

Page 27: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

15  

information  in  modern  society  happens  faster  and  in  more  complicated  ways  than  ever  before.  To  study  small  group  conversation  offers  an  opportunity   to   find  out  how  context  affects  how  stereotypes  emerge.    

When   small   group   communication   is   studied,   the   context   is   sometimes  manipulated  (Rode,  2010)  and  sometimes  differences  in  communication  according  to   gender   are   studied   (Hawkins  &   Power,   1999).  At   other   times,   the   focus   is   on  communication  related  to  power  (van  Knippenberg,  et  al.,  2001).  This  as  power  is  used  differently  in  competitive  and  cooperative  contexts.  In  a  cooperative  context  power  is  used  to  make  joint  decisions  while  in  a  competitive  context  the  purpose  is  to  use  power  to  make  a  better  position  for  oneself  (Van  Knippenberg,  et  al.,  2001).    

To  get  a  better  understanding  of  how  factors  such  as  external  influence,  group  composition  etc.  might  affect  communication  and  which  consequences  these  might  have,  it  is  important  to  have  an  innovative  scientific  approach.  One  such  approach  could  be  to  study  interaction  and  how  communication  and  language  affect  the  use  of  stereotypes  (Kurz  &  Lyons,  2009;  Lyons  &  Kashima,  2003).  With  this  approach,  communication   is   seen   as   social   interaction   that   occurs   in   a   specific   context  (Edwards   &   Potter,   1992;   Potter   &   Wetherell,   1987).   Language   is   from   this  perspective   seen   as   something   that   people   use   for   doing   things,   for   example,  persuading   others   or   accusing   someone   (Potter   &   Wetherell,   1987).   Human  communication  may  change  how  the  social  world   is  viewed   (Potter  &  Wetherell,  1987).  From  this  perspective,  stereotypes  are  reproduced  through  communication  in  for  example  the  choice  of  different  phrases  when  people  talk  about  other  people  that  are  not  present  (Kite  &  Whitley,  2012;  von  Hippel,  Wiryakusuma,  Bowden,  &  Shocket,  2011).    With  the  awareness  that  language  is  a  constructing  force  and  that  attitudes   might   be   seen   as   changeable,   it   is   of   importance   to   get   a   better  understanding  of  how  language  affects  how  stereotypes  are  used  and  how  they  are  constructed.   If  we  agree  with   this  assumption   from  discursive   social  psychology,  there   is   a   need   to   study   how   people   negotiate   and   communicate   about   other  people  and  how  stereotypes  can  be  applied  and  are  affected  in  different  contexts.      2.4.2 Analysis methods for small group communication

Communication   can   be   studied   through   different   methodologies.   One  traditional  method   to   study  communication   in  groups   is   through   so-­‐‑called   ‘turn-­‐‑taking’   processes   (Coates,   2004;   Schegloff,   2000;   Duncan   &   Fiske,   1977).   All  conversations  are  divided  into  various  parts  (turns);  first  someone  says  something  and  then  another  person  takes  over  or  chooses  to  pass  on  the  right  to  speak,  and  so  on.   If   no  one   else   says   anything,   the   speaker   is   able   to   continue.  There   are  other  more  elaborate  methods   to  study  a  group  dialogue.  A  frequently  used  method   is  the  Interaction  Process  Analysis  (IPA)  (Bales,  1950;  1999).  The  first  part  of  the  IPA  

Page 28: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

16  

explores   socio-­‐‑emotional   reactions   (positive   or   negative),   how   the   participants  communicate   in   terms   of   agreements,   laughter,   giving   help,   disagreeing   or  avoiding  helping  behavior.  The  second  part  of  the  IPA  focuses  on  attempts  to  give  information  and  how  questions  are  raised  and  answered.      

Another   method   to   study   group   conversations   is   by   the   Conversational  Argument  Coding   Scheme   (CACS)   (Canary  &   Seibold,   2010;  Meyers  &  Brashers,  2010).  This  method  has  a  history  in  different  scientific  fields,  including  philosophy,  psychology,   cognitive   studies,   advertising,   marketing   and   political   psychology  (Seibold  &  Meyers,  2007).  CACS  has   for  example  been  used  with   the  ambition   to  study   differences   between  men   and  women   in   argumentation   patterns   (Meyers,  Brashers,   Winston   &   Grob,   1997).   The   results   indicate   that   there   are   some  differences   in   conversation  between  men  and  women,   especially  as  women  were  found  to  be  more  agreeable  towards  other  group  members  than  men  were  (Meyers,  et  al.,  1997).  

The  benefits  with  conventional  methods  like  these  are  that  communication  can  be   investigated   in   its   smallest  parts   and  answer  different  questions.  But   they  are  less   adequate   to   study   the   content   of   an   interaction   process.   In   order   to   capture  how   stereotypes,   for   example,   about   men   and  women,   are   manifested   in   group  conversations   more   content   focused   analyses   have   to   be   applied.   Furthermore,  how  such  stereotypes  are  built   into  different  discourses  and  negotiated  in  groups  broader   perspectives   have   to   be   employed   for   example   to   use     “gender   glasses”  throughout  the  whole  analyses  (Ridgeway,  2011).    

2.5 The complexity of gender and gender stereotypes The  first  thing  that  people  notice  when  they  meet  a  new  person  is  whether  that  

person   is   male   or   female   (Fiske,   et   al.,   1991).   The   gender   categorization   evokes  associations  and  expectations  of  how  a  person  will  act.  Men  are  expected  to  act  and  behave  more  or  less  similar  to  other  men,  and  women  are  expected  to  act  more  or  less   similar   to   other  women.   Previous   research   proclaims   that   the   ‘gender   issue’  has  extensive  effects  on  an  individual’s  chances  in  life,  and  that  there  is  an  invisible  ‘glass  ceiling’  that  women  need  to  break  through  to  earn  a  place  in  power  positions  in  society  (Barreto,  Ryan  &  Schmitt,  2009).  Furthermore,  in  all  societies  in  the  world  with   power   differences  men   has   in   average  more   power   than  women   (Pratto   &  Walker,  2004).  Women  are  also  underrepresented  in  the  most  competitive  areas  in  society   and   the   inequality   increases   the   higher   up   in   an   organization   you   look  (Latu  &  Schmid  Mast,  2016).  

 The  aim  with  this  section  is  to  discuss  and  problematize  the  gender  concept  and  

briefly  introduce  research  related  to  gender  and  group  decision-­‐‑making.    

Page 29: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

17  

 ”With   the   possible   exception   of   childbearing,   no   aspect   of   social   life   is   more   strongly  associated  with  gender  than  power”          (Pratto  &  Walker,  p.242,  2004)    

Gender  stereotyping  starts  in  early  childhood  when  for  example  boys  and  girls  are   divided   into   gender-­‐‑stereotyped   groups.   This  might   lead   to   a   pattern  where  children   play   with   other   children   of   the   same   gender   as   this   a   ‘social   norm’  (Lindsey,   2012).   This   norm   might   then   follow   from   childhood   to   adolescence.  Furthermore,   this   pattern   runs   the   risk   of   being   manifested   when   children   are  divided   into   different   groups   based   on   gender   when   they   for   example   exercise  sports.   This   in   turn  might   reinforce   the   social   norms  where   it  might   be   odd   for  boys  to  play  with  girls  and  vice  versa  (Lindsey,  2012).  According  to  the  social  role  theory  two  sets  of  beliefs  are  predominately  associated  to  gender;  community  and  agency   (Eagly   &   Sczesny,   2009).   Women   are   supposed   to   display   communal  behaviors   such   as   compassion   (e.g.   being   friendly,   kind   and   sympathetic)   while  men  according  to  the  social  role  theory  seen  more  as  agents  (e.g.  being  aggressive,  ambitious,  dominant,  self-­‐‑confident).  As  such  these  two  dimensions  correspond  to  the   more   general   dimensions   of   competence   and   warmth   (Cuddy,   et   al.,   2008),  although   applied   specifically   to   gender   relations.   Gender   stereotypes   withhold  both  descriptive  and  prescriptive  stereotypes   (Bobbitt-­‐‑Zeher,  2011;  Glick  &  Fiske,  2007;  Meyer  &  Gelman,  2016).  The  descriptive  stereotyping  are  preconceptions  of  traits   related   to   a   specific   gender   (e.g.   women   are   warm),   the   prescriptive  stereotype   indicates   how  women   should   behave   (e.g.   caring).  Moreover,   gender  stereotypes  are  related  to  prejudice  as  they  put  women  and  men  in  positions  based  on  which  gender  they  are  associated  with  (Bobbit-­‐‑Zeher,  2011).  As  men  as  a  group  in  most  societies  are  in  a  power  position  women  may  be  viewed  as  a  marginalized  group  (Bergh,  Akrami,  Sidanius  &  Sibley,  2016).  Moreover,  gender  discrimination  should  not  be  viewed  as  an  asset  of  evilness   from  the  perpetrator   (Glick  &  Fiske,  2007)   it   should   instead  be  viewed  as  something   that   is   related   to  situation  and   is  affected  by  the  interaction  between  people  (Diekman  &  Eagly,  2000;  Glick  &  Fiske,  2007).    

As   stereotypes   influence   perceptions   of   other   people,   how   information   about  men   and  women   is   framed  might   evoke   different   stereotypic   beliefs.   The   above  mentioned   research   paradigm   where   information   about   parenthood   was  introduced   (Cuddy,   et   al.,   2004)   is   an   example   of   such   framing,   evoking  stereotypes  about  the  normative  roles  for  men  and  women  in  such  a  situation.  It  I  reasonable   to   assume   that   these   norms   also   are   present   and   communicated   in   a  group  decision  situation.  But  gender  might  also  serve  as  a  situational  cue,   in  that  

Page 30: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

18  

the   gender   composition   in   a   group   evokes   different   gender   information.   For  example,  in  single  gendered  groups  the  gender  composition  might  not  contain  any  particular   information  about  how  to  behave,  while   in  a  mixed  gender  group   it   is  evident   that   the   group   differ   as   to   the   combination   of   men   and   women,   thus  highlighting  gendered  expectations,  to  which  they  could  either  submit  or  challenge.  As  concluded  previously  there  is  a  need  to  study  situational  factors  and  how  they  affect  stereotypes,  one  such  could  be  gender  composition.  

Some   studies  have   investigated  which  effects  gender   composition  might  have  on   decision-­‐‑making   groups   (Lovaglia,  Mannix,   Samuelson,   Sell   &  Wilson,   2005).  Research   has   explored   how   masculine   or   feminine   communication   styles   are  affected   under   stereotype   threat   (von  Hippel,   et   al.,   2011).  One   result   from   their  study  reveals  that  women  under  stereotype  threat  who  adopted  a  more  masculine  communication   style   were   rated   as   less   warm   and   likeable   (von   Hippel,   et   al.,  2011).  According  to  social  role  theory  the  distribution  of  men  and  women  in  social  roles   is   important   for   the   understanding   of   why   men   and   women   behave  differently  in  different  circumstances  (Eagly,  Wood  &  Johannesen-­‐‑Schmidt,  2004).  For  example,  in  western  societies  the  norm  is  that  men  are  more  responsible  for  the  family  income  and  women  more  responsible  for  taking  care  of  the  household.  The  gender  roles  then  influence  behavior  and  how  men  and  women  are  viewed,  this  in  turn   leads   to   differences   in   the   gender   roles   and  men   are  more   associated  with  agentic  roles  including  for  example  leadership  (Eagly,  et  al.,  2004).  

Previous   studies   have   found   that  men  more   often   prefer   inequality   in   status  and   have   less   problems  with   power   in   social   groups   as   they  more   easily   adopt  hierarchies   (Schmid   Mast,   2001;   2004).   Men   are   also   more   motivated   to   take  leading  positions  in  hierarchical  organizations,  while  women  have  a  stronger  strive  for  egalitarian  values  (Schmid  Mast,  2004).  Previous  studies  show  diverging  results  concerning   gender   composition   and   group   decision-­‐‑making.   Some   of   them  indicate  that  groups  with  men  outperform  groups  with  women  (Apesteguia,  et  al.,  2012;   Raghubir   &   Valenzuela,   2010),   while   others   argue   that   groups   with   only  women  or  mixed-­‐‑gender  groups  perform  better  (Fenwick  &  Neal,  2001).  Moreover,  men  have  been  found  to  cooperate  more  when  they  were  under  threat  (competitive  context),   while   women   were   not   affected   (Van   Vugt,   et   al.,   2007).   This   might  explain   why   men   make   more   competitive   choices   in   social-­‐‑dilemma   games.  Experiments  on  groups  in  laboratory  settings  have  found  that  men  and  women  act  differently   when   they   hold   power   positions.   Men   were   found   to   be   more  dominating  and  women  more  compromising  (Nelson,  Bronstein,  Shacham  &  Ben-­‐‑Ari,   2015).   Finally,   previous   studies   has   found   that   there   is   a  difference   between  men  and  women  in  how  they  view  mixed  gendered  groups,  men  were  found  to  be  more  sensitive  to  gendered  groups  than  women  (Hannagan,  2006).    

Page 31: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

19  

Therefore,  gender  needs  to  be  considered  a  more  complex  variable  in  research.  Structurally,   as   an   aspect   that   explains   how   men   and   women   are   expected   to  behave   in   different   contexts,   situational,   as   an   aspect   that   a   person   acts   in  conjunction   with,   and   finally,   individually,   as   an   aspect   according   to   which   a  participant  identifies  him-­‐‑/herself.  

3. AIM As   argued   in   the   introduction,   stereotypes   are   simplifications   of   information  

concerning   other   persons   that   people  use   in  daily   life.   Stereotypes   are   biased   on  factors   such   as   ethnicity,   sexual   orientation   and   gender,   which   might   lead   to  discrimination  (Glick  &  Fiske,  2001).  There  is  a  risk  for  decision-­‐‑making  groups  of  making   biased   judgments   and   commit   discriminating   decisions   if   they   use  stereotypic   information   in   their  decision-­‐‑making  processes.  Previous  research  has  revealed   how   individuals   are   affected   by   stereotypes   (Agerström,   et   al.,   2012;  Cuddy,   et   al.,   2004;   Okimoto   &   Heilman,   2012)   but   less   is   known   about   how  stereotypes   and   prejudice   is   communicated   and   negotiated   in   group   decision-­‐‑making   situations.   This   is   of   importance   as   communication   is   a   way   of   doing  things  (Potter  &  Wetherell,  1987).  One  approach  to  study  how  stereotypes  emerge  and   are  manifested   in   group  decision-­‐‑making   is   to   study   how  different   contexts  may  lead  to  different  communication  patterns.    

One  way  of  doing   this   is   to  vary   the  goal   set   for   the  decision-­‐‑making.  This   is  seldom  problematized,  and  is  often  expressed  only  in  achievement  terms  (Ahn,  et  al.,   2003).   Besides,   there   are   reasons   to   believe   that  men   and  women   respond   to  different  goals  in  different  ways  (Van  Vugt,  et  al.,  2007).  Another  way  is  to  vary  the  information   given   to   see   how   this   influence   judgments.   Previous   research   has  revealed   that  variation  of   information  affects  how   individuals  use   information  of  gender   and   parenthood   in   a   stereotypic   pattern   (Cuddy,   et   al.,   2004;  Heilman  &  Okimoto,   2008).   But,   how   is   it   processed   in   groups?   Another   factor   to   take   into  account   is   the   situation   in  which   the  decision   is   being  made.  One   such   situation  could   be   gender   composition   of   the   group   as   also   this   contains   gendered  information.   This   has   previously   been   studied,   but   there   is   no   consensus   about  which  affects  it  might  have  (Hannagan  &  Larimer,  2010).    

The   form   of   the   communication   can   be   studied  with   for   example   interaction  process  analysis  (Bales,  1999)  or  with  the  conversational  argument  scheme  (Canary  &   Seibold,   2010).   The   content   –   how   stereotypes   emerge   and   are   used   –   can   be  studied  with  an  application  of  the  stereotype  content  model  and  the  dimensions  of  competence  and  warmth  (Fiske,  et  al.,  2007;  Fiske,  2015).  

Page 32: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

20  

 Finally,   although   group   communication   patterns   might   vary   and   there   are  more  or   less   stereotypic  ways   to  make  decisions   in  groups,  how   the  participants  experience   the  patterns  may  differ.  As   reported   above,  what   kind  of   experiences  that  is  of  importance  might  correspond  both  to  task  and  process,  and  could  refer  to  satisfaction,  efficiency  and  various  forms  of  influence.      Therefore  there  is  a  need  to  study  how  perceived  influence  over  different  parts  in  decision-­‐‑making  is  perceived  and  which  outcome  the  decision-­‐‑making  might  lead  to.      This  leads  to  a  summary  of   the   aims   in   this   thesis,   which   was   to   investigate   how   stereotyping   emerge,  manifest,   and   change   in   group   decision  making   by   using   two   kinds   of   framing  contexts;  (1)  the  goal  set  for  the  task  but  with  the  same  information  given,  and  (2)  the  same  goal  set  but  with  different  information  provided.  The  form  and  content  of  the   communication   could   thus   be   analyzed,   as   well   as   how   the   group   decision  situation  was  experienced  by  the  participants.      4. METHODOLOGY

The  aim  of  this  chapter   is   to  give  a  brief  summary  of  the  methods  used  in  the  thesis.   At   first   general   considerations   of   the   approaches   taken   in   this   thesis   are  reported.  Then  the  outline  of  the  experiments  is  described  followed  by  descriptions  of  the  qualitative  and  quantitative  data  used.  Table  1  gives  a  summary  of  how  the  methodology  corresponds  to  the  four  different  papers.                                          

Page 33: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

21  

Table  1.    Key  elements  in  respective  paper  

 

4.1 Mixed-method approach This   thesis   rests   on   a   mixed-­‐‑method   design   approach.     The   mixed-­‐‑method  

approach  has  the  ambition  to  both  answer  questions  about  how  it  is  and  in  what  way  (Cresswell   &   Plano   Clark,   2007).  They  define  mixed  methods   as   a  methodology  that   involves   a   philosophical   assumption   and   as   a   method   that   focuses   on  analyzing  and  collecting  both  quantitative  and  qualitative  data  (Cresswell  &  Plano  Clark,  2007).  Another  view  is  that  there  is  a  misconception  among  social  scientists  

!

! Paper!I! Paper!II! Paper!III! Paper!IV!

Experiment! The!Relay!Team! The!Relay!Team! The!Consultant!! The!Relay!

Team/The!

Consultant!

Participants! 79,!20!groups! 81,!21!groups! 130,!35!groups! Study!1:!210,!

54!groups.!

Study!II:!272,!

71!groups!

!

Framing!

Variables!

Goal!setting! Goal!Setting! Parenthood! Goal!setting/!

Parenthood!

!

Gender!

composition!

! X! X! X!

!

!

Qualitative!

analyses!

Interaction!

Process!Analysis,!

Stereotype!

Content!Analysis,!

Conversational!

Pattern!Analysis!

!

Conversational!

Argument!Coding!

Scheme!

Stereotype!Content!

Analysis,!

Communication!

Pattern!Analysis!

!

!

Quantitative!

analyses!

! Equality!in!

influence!over!the!

discussion!and!

over!the!decision,!

Efficiency,!

Satisfaction!

! Democracy,!

Influence,!

Satisfaction,!

Efficiency,!

Outcome!

Page 34: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

22  

when   they   argue   that   statistical   analysis   is   technical   and   objective   while   other  forms   of   data   are   judgment   based   (Gorard,   2006).   Furthermore   Gorard     (2007,)  argues   that  mixed  methods   should  not   be   seen   as   a   new  paradigm  as   it   in   itself  preserves   the   schism   between   the   two   paradigms.   However,   the   philosophical  debate   about   the   advantages   and   disadvantages   of   quantitative   and   qualitative  methods  is  in  my  view  not  fruitful  and  will  not  lead  research  forward.  But,  on  the  other   hand,   mixed   methods   designs   should   in   my   point   of   view   be   seen   as  questions  of  design  more  than  a  philosophical  argument  about  the  need  of  a  more  overarching  approach.    

In  my   thesis   I   use  mixed  methods   in   three   different  ways   1),   simultaneously  collecting   qualitative   and   quantitative   data,   2),   within   (paper   I-­‐‑III)   having   an  interchange   between   quantitative   and   qualitative   analyses,   and   3)   by   using   an  experimental  design  allowing  for  both  quantitative  and  qualitative  analyses  within  this   context.   The   quantitative   research   questions   aim   to   find   patterns   and   reveal  differences   between   the   different   experimental   contexts,   and   further   to   explore  differences   according   to   gender   composition   of   the   groups.   The   qualitative  research   questions   aim   to   explore   how   the   suggestions   from   the   quantitative  hypothesis  work  out  in  communication.  This  thesis  qualitative  inquiry  has  the  aim  to   go   beyond   traditional   social   psychological   aspects   such   as   attitudes   and  behavior.    The  ambition  is  to  broaden  understanding  of  how  stereotypes  emerge  in  different  situations.    

4.2 Group size of decision-making groups The   groups   studied   in   this   thesis   consist   of   three   to   five   persons,  most   often  

four.  The  main  reason  for  this  choice  of  group  size  was  that  more  than  two  people  secure  a  more  elaborate  discussion   (Wheelan,   2009).  A  maximum  of   five  persons  was   chosen   for   practical   reasons,   since   more   participants   limit   each   person’s  chance  to  take  part  in  the  discussion,  and  when  group  size  increases,  cooperation  decreases  (Wheelan,  2009).  Early  research  argues  for  a  maximum  of  five  persons  in  small  decision-­‐‑making  groups  as  the  number  of  social  interactions  explodes  with  a  higher   number   of   participants   (Hackman  &  Vidmar,   1970).  More   recent   research  (Wheelan,   2009)   revealed   that  groups   consisting  of   3   -­‐‑   6  members  were   the  most  productive.   Based   on   this   reasoning   it   was   considered   suitable   to   use   groups  consisting   of   four   participants   in   these   experiments.   Furthermore   as   the   gender  composition  in  the  groups  was  to  be  varied,  an  even  number  makes  its  possible  to  have   same   number   of   men   and   women   in   the   mixed   groups.   Due   to   practical  reasons  the  group  size  came  to  vary  between  three  and  five.  

Page 35: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

23  

4.3 Ad-hoc or real groups Research   on   real   decision-­‐‑making   groups   and   temporary   (ad-­‐‑hoc)   groups   are  

both  common.  One  advantage  with  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups  is  that  it  is  possible  to  carry  out  these   studies   in   a   laboratory   setting  with   the   aim   to   frame   conditions.  However,  this   can   also   be   done   with   real   decision-­‐‑making   groups.   But   one   problem   with  experiments  on  real  groups  is  that  extraneous  factors  that  might  affect  the  results  could  be   impossible   to  control.   It   is  unclear   if  ad-­‐‑hoc  or   real  groups  are  better   in  making   joint   decisions.   Some   empirical   studies   have   shown   that   real   decision-­‐‑making   groups   perform   better   than   ad-­‐‑hoc   groups   (Hall   &   Williams,   1966).  However,   other   studies   have   found   that   ad-­‐‑hoc   groups   outperform   established  decision-­‐‑making  groups  in  terms  of  decision-­‐‑making  effectiveness  (Ford,  Nemiroff  &  Pasmore,  1977).  Other  researchers  argue  that  real  decision-­‐‑making  groups  might  have  a  richer  repertoire  of  decision-­‐‑making  strategies  as  the  group  members  might  be  motivated  by  plans   to  work   together   in   the   future   (Curşeu  &  Schruijer,   2012).  Moreover,  results  reached  with  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups  are  also  reached  with  real  decision-­‐‑making   groups   (Dubrovsky,  Kolla  &   Sethna,   1991).   The   difference  might   be   that  results   found   in  ad-­‐‑hoc  group  might  be  stronger   if   conducted  with  real  decision-­‐‑making  groups  (Dubrovsky,  et  al.,  1991).  

To   summarize,   real   groups  have   the   advantage  of   being   representative   of   the  reality,   for   example   is   it   possible   to   study   group  processes   across   time   and   how  groups  mature.  On  the  other  hand  in  an  ad-­‐‑hoc  group  it  is  possible  to  study  how  group  norms  begins,  as  the  members  have  no  common  history  or  future.  Thus,  in  order  to  study  how  a  specific  pattern  emerges  as  for  example  in  this  thesis  where  the  manifestations  and  negotiations  of  stereotypes  are  investigated  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups  might   be   preferable.   Moreover,   it   might   be   easier   to   secure   a   strong   internal  validity   by   studying   ad-­‐‑hoc   groups   due   to   stronger   control   over   extraneous  variables.    

4.4 The experiments This  thesis  consists  of  four  papers  that  are  based  on  two  experiments  conducted  

on  small  group  decision-­‐‑making  sessions.  The  basic  outline  of  the  two  experiments  is  quite  similar.  Groups  of  approximately  four  participants  are  assigned  to  a  task  to  come  to  a  joint  decision  under  different  conditions.  In  both  experiments  groups  are  formed   either   as   single   gendered   or   mixed   gendered.   They   complete   almost  identical   questionnaires   before   and   after   the   actual   experiment.   The   difference  between  the  two  experiments  concerns  the  setup  of  the  experimental  conditions.  In  the   first   experiment   “The   Relay   Team”   two   different   goals   was   set   as   the  experimental   conditions,   competition   vs.   cooperation.   In   the   second   experiment  

Page 36: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

24  

“The  Consultant”   two  dimensions  of   information  given,  gender  and  parenthood,  were  varied  forming  four  different  conditions.    

 4.4.1 The Relay Team

4.4.1.2 Participants A  total  of  210  university  students  (138  women  and  72  men)  participated  in  The  

Relay  Team  experiment.  The  mean  age  was  25  years  (SD  =  5.55).  They  were  divided  into   54   different   groups   consisting   of   3-­‐‑5   persons.     Paper   I   and   II   are   based   on  subsamples  of  the  participants  while  paper  IV  uses  the  total  sample.      4.4.1.3 Procedure

One  week  before  the  experiment  the  participants  were  approached  in  class  and  informed   about   the   aim  of   the   study   and   asked   to   participate.  At   the   same   time  they   completed   a   questionnaire   with   background   data.   After   arriving   to   the  laboratory,  they  were  divided  into  groups  of  three  to  five  persons,  most  often  four  either  as  all  female  (27),  all  male  (10)  or  mixed  gendered  groups  (17).  The  groups  were   then   randomly   assigned   to   one   of   two   conditions.   One   condition   was   to  assemble   a   team   for   a   relay   running   competition  with   the   goal   to  maximize   the  chance  of  winning  the  competition.  The  other  condition  was  to  assemble  a  team  for  a  relay  running  competition  with  the  goal  to  have  fun  and  to  maximize  the  sense  of  community  within  the  team.    

The   participants   were   placed   around   a   table   and   the   experimenter   read   the  instructions   to   the  participants.  The  groups  were   then  given   two  sheets  of  paper.  The   first   paper   consisted   of   instructions.   It   also   contained   pictures   of   20   target  persons   (10   men   and   10   women)   with   information   about   their   first   name,   age,  occupation   and   favorite   hobby.  Occupations   and   hobbies   assigned   to   the   targets  were  categorized  as  traditionally  feminine,  masculine,  or  gender-­‐‑neutral  based  on  ratings  made  by  another  group  of  students.  

The   second   sheet  was  a  protocol  where   the  different   relay   sections  of  various  lengths  were  outlined;  the  groups  were  told  that  they  could  use  as  many  or  as  few  runners  as  they  wished  and  that  they  were  allowed  to  split  the  relay  sections.  The  time  limit  was  set  to  15  minutes.  The  sessions  were  filmed.  Immediately  after  the  session   was   stopped   the   participants   completed   a   questionnaire,   which   asked  about  their  reflections  on  their  experiences  of  the  decision  task.  Finally,  they  were  debriefed  about   the  specific  aim  of   the  study  and  about  how  the  data  were   to  be  handled.   Participation   in   the   experiment   was   a   course   requirement   (i.e.  demonstration  of  experimental   research  methods)  but  being  part  of   research  was  voluntary.   After   debriefing,   if   the   participants   agreed   to   be   part   of   the   research  

Page 37: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

25  

they  signed  a  consent   form.  Otherwise   they  could  refrain  which  meant   that   their  material  was  not  to  be  used.    

This  procedure  emanated  in  a  design  with  two  independent  variables.  One  was  the   framing   condition;   either   as   a   competition   or   as   a   community   goal   for   the  decision  task.  The  other  one  was  gender  composition  in  the  groups;  male,  female,  or  mixed.  

 4.4.2 The Consultant

4.4.2.1 Participants A   total   of   272   students   (179   women,   93   men)   participated   in   The   Consultant  

experiment.   The   mean   age   was   26   years   (SD   =   5.72).   They   were   recruited   from  various   study   programs,   mainly   in   social   and   behavioral   sciences   and   were  divided  into  71  groups.  Paper  III  is  based  on  a  subsample  while  paper  IV  uses  the  whole  sample.    4.4.2.2. Procedure

One  week  before  the  experiment  the  participants  were  approached  in  class  and  informed  of  the  aim  of  the  study  and  asked  to  participate.  At  the  same  time  they  completed  a  questionnaire  with  background  data.  After  arriving  to  the  laboratory  they  were  divided  into  groups  of  3-­‐‑5  persons  (most  often  four),  either  as  all  male  (16),  all  female  (40),  or  mixed  gendered  groups  (15).    

The   groups  were   then   randomly   assigned   to   one   of   four   conditions.   The   task  was   to   select   one   of   three   candidates   to   a   position   as   a   consultant.   The   first  candidate  was  a  man  (Jörgen),   the  second  a  woman  (Lena),  and  the  third  (target)  was  in  half  of  the  cases  a  man  (Daniel)  and  in  half  of  the  cases  a  woman  (Katarina).  Information   was   given   about   each   candidate’s   age,   education,   work-­‐‑life  experience,  preferred  work  style  and  hobbies.  In  half  of  the  cases  the  target  person  had   a   newborn   child.   The   setup   was   modeled   after   Cuddy,   et   al.   (2004)   and  Heilman   and   Okimoto   (2008)   although   the   main   objective   here   was   that   the  decisions  was  to  be  made  in  groups  and  not  individually.  But  before  they  were  to  work   as   a   group   they   completed   a   questionnaire   where   they   evaluated   each  candidate  individually  on  items  related  to  competence  and  warmth  in  order  for  the  participants  to  form  their  own  individual  impressions  of  the  candidates.      

When  this  was  done  the  instructions  were  read  to  the  groups,  they  were  told  to  evaluate  the  candidates  of  four  items  (on  a  response  scale  ranging  from  1  to  5):  to  what   extent   they  were   considered   to   work   efficiently,   to   what   extent   they  were  supposed   to   create   a   good   atmosphere,   to   what   extent   they   would   likely   be  recommended   for  promotion,  and   to  what  extent   they  were   regarded  eligible   for  further   training.   Finally,   they   were   asked   to   rank   order   the   candidates   for   the  

Page 38: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

26  

position  as  a  consultant.  They  were  given  a  protocol   sheet  on  which   they  should  indicate   their   choices.     The   group  discussion  was   limited   to   15  minutes   and  was  filmed.   When   the   session   stopped   they   were   asked   to   complete   another  questionnaire   containing  questions   about   their   reflections  on   their   experiences  of  the  decision  task.  Finally,  they  were  debriefed  about  the  specific  aim  of  the  study  and  about  how  the  data  were  to  be  handled.  Participation  in  the  experiment  was  a  course   requirement   (i.e.   demonstration   of   experimental   research   methods)   but  being  part  of  research  was  voluntary.  After  debriefing,  if  the  participants  agreed  to  be  part  of   the   research   they   signed  a   consent   form.  Otherwise   they   could   refrain  which  meant  that  their  material  was  not  to  be  used.    

This   procedure   emanated   in   a   design  with   three   independent   variables.   Two  were  built  upon   the   framing  condition;   the   target  person’s  gender  and   the   target  person’s   parental   status.   The   other   one   was   gender   composition   in   the   groups;  male,  female,  or  mixed.  

4.5 Quantitative data Two   of   the   papers   (paper   II   and   IV)   report   quantitative   data   from   the  

questionnaire   completed   after   the   experimental   session.  The  dependent  variables  are  presented  below.      Perceived  equality  in  the  influence  over  the  decision  was  included  in  paper  II.  Each  

participant  rated  in  percentage  how  much  each  group  member  was  involved  in  the  decision.  Participants  who  assigned  the  distribution  of  influence  to  equal  (within  a  5  %   limit)  were  categorized  as  perceiving  an  equal  distribution.  Participants  who  assigned  an  unequal  percentage  were  categorized  as  perceiving  unequal  influence  over  the  decision.    Perceived  equality  in  the  influence  in  the  discussion  was   included  in  paper  II.  This  

variable  was  measured  similarly  to  the  previous  variable  but  with  the  question  of  how   much   each   participant   was   involved   in   the   discussion.   Participants   who  assigned  the  distribution  of  influence  in  the  discussion  as  equal  (within  a  5  %  limit)  were  categorized  as  perceiving  an  equal  distribution  of  influence  in  the  discussion.  Participants  who  assigned  an  unequal  percentage  were  categorized  as  perceiving  an  unequal  influence  over  the  discussion.  Perceived   task   difficulty   was   in   paper   II   embraced   as   a   control   variable.   The  

variable  served  the  purpose  to  control  if  task  difficulty  could  affect  satisfaction  and  group  efficiency.  It  was  measured  with  the  question  “how  hard  was  the  task  to  solve”.  It  was  answered  on  a  seven-­‐‑step  Likert  scale  ranging  from  one  (very  easy)  to  seven  (very  difficult).      

Page 39: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

27  

Perceived   satisfaction   with   the   decision  was   included   in   paper   II   and   IV.   It   was  measured  with  the  question  “How  good  do  you  think  the  decision  was”,  ranging  from  one  (very  bad)  to  seven  (Very  good).    Perceived  group  efficiency  was  included  in  paper  II  and  IV.  It  was  measured  with  

the   question   “How   efficient   do   you   think   the   group´s   work   was”,   ranging   from   one  (very  bad)  to  seven  (very  good).    Experienced   own   influence   was   included   in   paper   IV.   The   variable   was  

constructed  as  a  deviance   score   from  a  group  norm  of   total   equality   in   influence  (i.e.  25  %  in  groups  of  four,  33  %  in  groups  of  three,  and  20  %  in  groups  of  five).    Evaluated   democratic   character.   The   variable   was   included   in   paper   IV.   It   was  

measured   by   14   items   inspired   by  Michels   theory   of   “the   iron   law   of   oligarch”  (1911,   1984)   and   as   operationalized   by   Jonsson,   Roempke   and   Zakrisson   (2003).  The   questions   included   focused   on   how   the   participants   perceived   different  democratic  values  in  the  decision-­‐‑making  process.  Examples  of  items  were:  “There  was   a   closed   discussion   climate   and   everybody   was   afraid   to   say   the   wrong  things”  (reversed)  or    “All  members  of  the  group  took  equal  responsibility  to  lead  the  work  forward”.  The  items  were  measured  on  a  seven-­‐‑step  scale  ranging  from  1  “totally  disagree”   to  7  “totally  agree”,   and   the  variable  was   constructed  as  mean  across  the  included  items.  Decision  outcome   -­‐‑  proportion  of  women.  The  variable  was   included   in  paper   IV,  

study  1.    The  variable  measured  the  proportion  of  selected  women  in  each  group  in  relation  to  the  total  number  of  fictive  runners  selected  to  the  team.    Decision   outcome   –   ranking   of   target   candidates.   The   variable   was   included   in  

paper   IV,   study   2.     The   variable   measured   the   groups´   mean   ranking   of   the  candidates.  

4.6 Qualitative data analysis All   filmed   sessions   were   transcribed   and   subjected   to   different   forms   of  

qualitative  analyses.    

4.6.1 Interaction process analysis (IPA)

The   interaction  process   analysis   (Bales,   1950;   1999)   is   a  method   that   has   been  used  since  the  1950’s  to  study  communication  in  small  groups.  The  method  focuses  on  how  people  communicate,  how  they  use  positive  or  negative  reactions  and  how  they   ask   for   and   give   information.   The   IPA   has   been   used   in   several   studies   on  small   groups   (Keyton  &  Beck,   2009;  Nam,  Lyons,  Hwang,  &  Kim,   2009).   It   is   an  established   and   influential   methodology   (Hirokawa,   1988)   with   an   inclusive  coding  scheme  that  allows  all  statements  in  a  group  to  be  coded  (Keyton  &  Beck,  

Page 40: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

28  

2009)   and   it   has   been   tested   for   representational   validity   (Poole  &   Folger,   1981).  Results  from  the  analysis  can  be  represented  both  qualitatively  and  quantitatively.  Qualitatively   to   explore   communication   patterns   and   how   discussants  communicate   different   issues.   Quantitatively   to   explore   differences   between  various  kinds  of  groups  but  also  to  identify  differences  depending  on  experimental  conditions.    

Paper   I   applies   IPA   quantitatively   to   explore   differences   due   to   experimental  conditions   of   competition   vs.   cooperation.   In   order   to   do   so   the   transcribed  material   was   analyzed   qualitatively   in   order   to   find   examples   of   different  categories  and  sub-­‐‑categories.    

 

4.6.2 Conversational argument coding scheme (CACS) The  conversational  argument-­‐‑coding  scheme  (CACS;  Canary  &  Seibold,  2010)  is  

a  widespread  coding  scheme  with  the  ambition  of  exploring  arguments.  The  CACS  consists  of  five  major  categories.  Firstly,  arguables   is  divided  into  two  major  parts.  The  first  is  called  generative  mechanisms  containing  assertions  and  propositions.  The  second   is   called   reasoning   activities   and   consists   of   elaborations,   responses,  amplifications  and   justifications.  The   second  main  category   is  convergence  markers  containing   statements   representing   agreements   and   acknowledgments.   The   third  category   is   called   prompters,   containing   statements   such   as   objections   and  challenges.   The   fourth   category   is   labeled   delimitors,   containing   statements   that  provide   a   context   for   arguables   or   attempts   to   secure   common   grounds   or   to  remove  possible   objections.   The   last   category   is  non-­‐‑arguables,   containing  process  statements  or  statements  unrelated  to  the  task  or  incomplete  statements  impossible  to  categorize  (Canary  &  Seibold,  2010;  Meyers  &  Brashers,  2010;  Seibold  &  Meyers,  2007).   Previous   research   has   used   the  CACS   in   different   contexts:   to   explore   sex  differences  (Meyers,  et  al.,  1997),  to  study  majority  vs.  minority  influence  (Meyers,  et  al,  2000)  and  in  different  circumstances  such  as  city  commission  meetings  (Beck,  Gronewold  &  Western,  2012).    

This  method  was  used  in  paper  II  and  III.  The  transcribed  group  conversations  were   first   analyzed   qualitatively   in   order   to   find   examples   of   the   different  categories.   In   paper   II   it   was   used   quantitatively   to   explore   differences   between  groups  identified  as  either  as  successful  or  non-­‐‑successful.  Paper  III  applied  CACS  in  a  more  extensive  way  and  only  qualitatively.      

   

4.6.3 Stereotype content analysis The  stereotype  dimensions  of  warmth  and  competence   (Fiske,  et  al.,   2002)  are  

essential   to   this   thesis.   This   analysis   focuses   on   the   content   of   the   conversation  instead   of   the   form   of   the   conversation.   Unlike   IPA   and   CACS   there   exist   no  

Page 41: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

29  

established   coding   system.   In   order   to   carry   out   this   content   analysis   a  working  definition   of   how   a   stereotype   would   be   manifested   in   the   conversations   was  proposed.  A  stereotype  was   then  defined  as  a  statement  about  a   target   that  went  beyond   the   information   given   about   that   person.   Secondly,   from   the   stereotype  content   model   and   how   the   dimensions   of   competence   and   warmth   have   been  operationalized  in  previous  research  stereotypic  statements  were  categorized  into  these   dimensions   either   as   positive   or   negative   (cold   -­‐‑   warm,   competent   -­‐‑  incompetent).  Stereotype  content  analysis   in  this  way  was  applied  in  paper  I  and  III.    

 

4.6.4 Conversational pattern analysis A   final   kind   of   qualitative   analyses   was   applied   in   order   to   identify   how  

stereotypic   content   was   used   in   a   broader   pattern.   In   paper   I   this   was   done   by  analyzing  how  this  was  used  in  arguing  for  exclusion  or  inclusion  of  members  to  the  relay  team.  In  paper  III  the  communication  patterns  were  analyzed  in  order  to  discover  whether  and  how  gender  ideologies  were  negotiated.  

   

5. EMPIRICAL PAPERS  

This  section  will  present  a  brief  summary  of  the  studies  and  the  most  relevant  analyses  and  results.    

5.1 Paper I Löfstrand,  P.,  &  Zakrisson,  I.  (2014).  Competitive  versus  non-­‐‑competitive  goals  

in  group  decision-­‐‑making.  Small  Group  Research,  45  (4),  451-­‐‑464.      

5.1.1 Background and aim As  stated  in  the  aim  of  this  thesis  a  way  to  study  if  and  how  biased  information  

processing   appears   in   group   decision-­‐‑making   is   to   frame   the   context   for   the  decision   situation.   One   such   framing   is   to   vary   the   goals   set   for   the   decision-­‐‑making  session.  Groups  most  often  have  values  and  ambitions  that  vary  between  groups,  tasks  and  situations.  Often,  goals  are  related  to  achievement,  e.g.  receiving  high  grades,  making   a  profit   or  winning   a   competition.  Besides   the   achievement  goals,   groups   have   a   need   to   achieve   good   social   relations   among   the   group  members.  These  two  different  kinds  of  goal  setting  often  exist  at  the  same  time  and  this   sometimes   creates   a   dilemma.   This   is   especially   true   for   groups   in   the  

Page 42: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

30  

voluntary   sector   (Vogel,   Amnå,   Munk,   &   Häll,   2003).   This   paper   explores   how  differences  in  the  decision-­‐‑making  context  (competitive  or  non-­‐‑competitive  goals)  affect   the   decision-­‐‑making   process   simulating   a   sports   association   context.   As  reported   previously   in   this   thesis,   research   indicates   that   competitive   and  cooperative  goals  influence  decision-­‐‑making  groups  in  different  ways.  The  aim  of  this   study   was   to   explore   how   goal   setting   influenced   communication   patterns  both  as  to  form  and  to  content.  To  study  the  form  of  the  communication  interaction  process   analysis   (IPA;   Bales,   1999)   was   used.     The   content   was   analyzed   by  applying   the   stereotype   content  model   on   if,   and  how,   stereotypes  were  used   in  the  argumentation  in  relation  to  decisions  about  inclusion  and  exclusion.    

 

5.1.2 Methodology

5.1.2.1 Participants The   data   collected   at   the   time   for   the   compilation   of   this   paper   were   79  

university   students   (50   female   and   29   male)   from   different   study   program,  participating   in   the   experiment     “The   Relay   Team”.   They   were   divided   into   20  groups  of  3  to  5  persons  (most  often  4).    

5.1.2.2 Data analysis The   transcriptions   of   the   group   decision-­‐‑making   sessions   were   subjected   to  

three   different   analyses.   First,   the   transcriptions   were   coded   blindly   using   the  Interaction   Process   Analysis   into   our   main   categories;   information   sharing,  positive   reactions,   questions,   and   negative   reactions.   These   were   further  subdivided  into  three  sub-­‐‑categories  respectively.    Second,  a  content  analysis  was  carried   out   applying   the   stereotype   dimensions   of   competence   and   warmth,   to  which   examples   from   the   transcribed   material   were   matched.   Furthermore,  examples  of  inclusion  and  exclusion  statements  were  extracted.  Finally,  arguments  of  warmth  and  competence  were  analyzed  in  relation  to  statements  about  inclusion  and  exclusion.    

5.1.3 Results

Both   main   and   sub-­‐‑categories   of   the   IPA   were   subjected   to   a   quantitative  analysis,   which   revealed   only   minor   differences   between   the   two   goal   settings.    There  were  more   positive   reactions   in   groups  with   the   cooperative   goal   than   in  groups  with  the  competitive  goal.    

The   remaining   analyses   focused   on   the   content   of   the   discussions  within   the  groups.   The   main   results   were   that   the   context   had   implications   on   how  

Page 43: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

31  

stereotypes   was   negotiated   and   used   in   the   decision-­‐‑making   process.   In   the  competitive  context,  competence  was  positioned  at  the  beginning  of  the  discussion  where   the  goal  was   to  win   the  competition.  Competence  was  more  elaborated   in  groups   with   a   competitive   goal.   Inclusion   was   important   when   it   came   to  individuals  who  the  group  members  assumed  were  the  best  performers.  Exclusion  was  also  important,  but  mainly  to  exclude  the  worst  performers.  Exclusions  were  motivated  by  stereotypical  arguments  such  as  “old  people  run  slower”,  “wine  tasters  are   probably   bad   runners”,   etc.   In   the   non-­‐‑competitive   context,   competent   runners  were   included   in   the   team  with  arguments   such  as  “they  will  probably  be  happier”.  Groups  with  a  non-­‐‑competitive  goal  had  difficulties   relating   to  how   to  approach  the   task,  “what  does   it  mean   to  have   fun   together”,   etc.   The  warmth  dimension  was  discussed   often   in   the   non-­‐‑competitive   context.   Inclusion   was   discussed   to   find  positions  that  would  create  a  good  team  atmosphere.    

 

5.1.4 Conclusion While   the   form   of   the   communication   did   not   differ   much   between   the   two  

goals,  the  content  of  the  discussions  showed  more  systematic  patterns.  It  seems  as  a  competitive  goal  leads  to  both  inclusion  and  exclusion  with  use  of  both  positive  and   negative   stereotypes.   A   non-­‐‑competitive   goal   seems   to   lead   to   inclusions  mechanisms   and   only   use   of   positive   stereotypes.   That   it   seemed   harder   to  understand  how   to   approach   the   task   in   a  non-­‐‑competitive   context   suggests   that  competition  is  more  normative  and  thus  easier  to  relate  to.      

5.2 Paper II Löfstrand,   P.,   (2015).   Conversational   arguments   in   small   group   decision-­‐‑

making:  reasoning  activity  and  perceived  influence  over  the  decision  are  keys  for  success.   In  R.  Thornberg  &  T.   Jungert   (Eds.),   Independent   in   the  heard:   Inclusion  and   exclusion   as   social   processes:   Proceedings   from   the   9th   GRASP   conference,  Linköping  University   (pp.64-­‐‑81).   (Research   report   in   electronics)   (Linköping   Electronic  Conference  Proceedings).    

 

5.2.1 Background and aim Paper  I  showed  that  the  context  played  an  important  role  for  the  conversation  

pattern   in   decision-­‐‑making   groups.   However,   from   this   it   is   not   possible   to   say  which  kind  of  conversation  is  better  than  the  other.  Furthermore,  what  constitutes  more  constructive  decision-­‐‑making  is  also  difficult   to  tell.   It   is  not  always  easy  to  say   whether   a   group   process   is   good   or   successful.   Often   when   groups   are  evaluated   their   successfulness   is  measured   in   achievement   terms.  How  did   they  

Page 44: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

32  

perform  in  relation  to  other  groups?  How  much  did  they  gain?  Even  if  the  group  is  evaluated  for  its  outcomes,  the  group  members  might  not  share  the  same  view  of  the  group’s   successfulness.  Thus,   how   the   individual   group  members   experience  the   situation   is   essential   as   it  might   influence   their  motivation   to   cooperate   and  move  the  work  forward.  As  stated  in  chapter  2  what  people  experience  might  be  of  various  kinds,   it  could  be  related  to  the  outcome  as   for  example  satisfaction  with  the  decision  or  it  could  be  process  oriented  as  for  example  the  perceived  efficiency  with  which  the  group  has  worked.  It  could  also  be  related  to  how  the  participants  experience   own   and   other   members’   influence   over   the   actual   decision   or   the  decision-­‐‑making   process.   These   different   kinds   of   evaluations   do   not   necessarily  need  to  be  related  to  each  other.   It   is   for  example  possible   to  be  content  with   the  decision   outcome   but   without   experienced   influence   over   the   decision   process.    The   individual   experiences   both   influence   and   are   influenced   by   the   group.   For  example   efficiency   does   not   necessarily   mean   the   ease   or   speed   with   which   a  decision  is  reached.  Sooner  it  includes  how  the  problem  to  be  solved  is  elaborated  –  how  information  is  shared,  how  different  opinions  and  conflicts  are  handled  thus  the  form  of  the  communication  within  the  group.    In  paper  I  IPA  was  used  to  study  such  form  of  interaction.  However,  it  yielded  few  clear  results.  Another  way  is  to  focus  on  the  form  of  the  argumentation  within  the  group.    

This   paper   was   divided   into   two   parts.   First,   the   relationship   between  satisfaction,   perceived   efficiency,   perceived   equality   of   influence   over   the  discussion   and   perceived   equality   of   influence   over   the   decision   were   analyzed  quantitatively.   In   the   second   part,   the   form   of   the   communication   in   terms   of  argumentation  processes  was  analyzed   in  groups   that  differed   in   their   subjective  experience  of  satisfaction,  efficiency  and  influence.    

 

5.2.2 Method

5.2.2.1 Participants Data  from  81  participants  (53  women,  28  men)  forming  21  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups  of  3  to  

5  participants,  (most  often  4),  took  part  in  the  experiment  “The  Relay  Team”.  This  far   in   the  project   there  was  some  more  data  collected  compared   to   the  sample   in  paper   I.  However  due   to  missing  data  on   some  of   the  quantitative  measures   the  number  of  participants  differ  between  the  papers.    

5.2.2.2 Quantitative data Five   variables   based   on   answers   to   questions   in   the   final   questionnaire  were  

used   in   this   paper:   Perceived   equality   in   the   influence   over   the   decision,   Perceived  

Page 45: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

33  

equality  in  the  influence  in  the  discussion,  Perceived  task  difficulty,  Perceived  satisfaction  with   the   decision   and  Perceived   group   efficiency.  How   these  were   operationalized   is  presented  in  chapter  4.    

5.2.2.3 Qualitative data Based   on   the   variables   “perceived   satisfaction   with   the   decision”,   and   “perceived  

group   efficiency”   a   total   of   six   groups   were   selected   for   further   analysis,   three  among  those  with  the  highest  mean  values  on  both  variables  (labeled  “successful”)  and   three   among   those  with   the   lowest  means   on   both   variables   (labeled   “non-­‐‑successful”).  The  three  groups  in  each  category  were  also  matched  according  to  the  experimental  condition  and  gender  composition.    

The   transcripts   for   these   groups   were   subjected   to   conversational   argument  analysis  as  conversational  arguments  described  in  chapter  4.    

5.2.3 Results

The   quantitative   analyses   revealed   that   perceived   influence   over   the   decision  was   more   important   than   perceived   influence   over   the   discussion.   Perceived  equality  in  influence  over  the  decision  was  related  to  both  decision  satisfaction  and  perceived   group   efficiency.   The   qualitative   analysis   of   the   transcriptions   for   six  groups   (three   labeled   successful   and   three   labeled   unsuccessful)  was   carried   out  with   the   conversational   argument   coding   scheme.   Examples   of   all   types   of  conversational  arguments  were  found  in  the  present  material.  The  groups  defined  as   successful   displayed   overall   more   argumentation   examples   than   the   groups  defined   as   unsuccessful   although   significant   differences   were   only   found   for  reasoning   activities   and   non-­‐‑arguables.   More   specifically   the   successful   groups  used   both   justifications   of   propositions   and   responses   and   these   reasoning  activities   seemed   to   serve   the   function   of   moving   the   communication   forward.  These  kinds  of   reasoning  activities  were   found  regardless  of  goal   set   for   the   task  and  group  composition.    

5.2.4 Conclusions These  results   indicate   that  equality   in   the   influence  over   the  actual  decision   is  

important  for  how  the  decision  process  is  experienced.  The  qualitative  analyses  of  the   conversational   patterns   seemed   to   validate   this.   A   richer   and   more   varied  discussion   tends   to   incorporate  more   of   the   participants  making   them   feel  more  involved  and  thus  satisfied.    

Page 46: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

34  

5.3 Paper III Löfstrand,   P.   &   Zakrisson,   I.   (2017).   “What   about   the   child   issue?”   Group  

negotiations  of  gender  and  parenthood  contracts  in  recruitment  situations,  Society,  Health  &  Vulnerability,  8  (1),  19-­‐‑30.  

 

5.3.1 Background and aim In   paper   I   it   was   found   that   stereotypic   statements   were   used   in   the  

argumentation   for   and   against   inclusion   and   exclusion.   These   patterns   were  related   to   the   framing  context  of   competition  vs.   cooperation.  Paper   II   seemed   to  indicate   that  richer  discussions  were  experienced  as  more  constructive  regardless  of  the  framing  context.  What  these  two  studies  do  not  say  is  if  and  in  what  way  the  complexity   of   a   discussion   is   related   to   how   stereotypes   are   communicated.  Although  the  framing  context  of  competition  and  cooperation  implicitly  is  related  to  gender  perhaps  a  more  explicit  gender  related  framing  would  yield  a  better  test  of   gender   related   stereotypes   and   how   these   are   negotiated.   For   this   purpose  another  framing  situation  was  used  in  paper  III.    

Parenthood   is   a   concept   that   has   quite   specific   connotations   for   what   is  expected   from  men   and  women.   For   example  men   and  women   are   found   to   be  judged   differently   along   the   dimensions   of   competence   and  warmth,   which   has  implications   for   their   career   opportunities   (Cuddy,   et   al.,   2004).   Such   judgments  have   mainly   been   studied   on   individual   level   but   in   a   decision   situation   such  stereotypes  are  probably  manifested  and  negotiated.  Discourses  of  parenthood  and  work   have   mainly   been   studied   from   the   perspective   of   parents   and   more  specifically   implications   for   mothers.   In   the   present   study   how   parenthood   is  understood   in   relation   to   career   situations   is   investigated   by   applying   a  recruitment  paradigm  normally  used  on  individual  level  but  here  transferred  to  a  group  decision   situation   in  order   to   study  how  gender   in   relation   to  parenthood  was  negotiated.    

 

5.3.2 Method

5.3.2.1 Participants The  participants  in  this  paper  were  students  from  various  university  programs  

that  took  part  in  experiment  II,  “The  Consultant”.  Data  from  groups  in  the  parent  condition  was  included  for  further  analyses.    The  number  of  participants  was  130  (85  women  and  45  men)  forming  35  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups.    

Page 47: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

35  

5.3.2.2 Data and analysis The   transcriptions   for   the   decision   sessions   for   each   group  were   subjected   to  

two   kinds   of   analysis.   First   content   analysis   was   carried   out   applying   the  stereotype   content  model.   The   second   analysis  was   to   use   a   brief   version   of   the  conversational   argument   coding   scheme   (Canary   &   Seibold,   2010;   Meyers   &  Brashers,   2010)   in   order   to   investigate   how   statements   about   competence   and  warmth  formed  different  discourses  within  the  group.    

 

5.3.3 Results Parenthood   dominated   the   discussions   and   was   the   only   background  

information  that  was  discussed  in  all  of  the  groups;  this  was  more  often  than,  for  example,  work  experience,  which  was  the  second  most  common.  Parenthood  was  sometimes  seen  as  reducing  competence,  but  it  was  more  often  viewed  as  adding  to  competence.  Parenthood  was  also  considered  to  add  warmth  to  the  organization.  Interestingly,  all  groups  avoided  relating  the  parenthood  issue  to  gender.    

Parenthood  was  mainly  discussed  as  beneficial   for   the  organization.  This  was  especially  true  in  groups  with  only  women  discussing  a  female  target  or  in  groups  with   only  men  discussing   a  male   target.  Male   groups  discussing   a   female   target  shared  this  view  although  sometimes  with  a  reservation.  Warmth  attributes  were  discussed   more   in   the   female   groups   than   in   the   male   groups.   Regarding   the  female   target   both  groups  with   only  men   and  with   only  women   saw  warmth   as  general  traits  such  as  friendly  and  humble.  The  argument  that  warmth  is  an  asset  to  the  company  was  only  found  in  the  groups  with  only  women  and  only  for  the  female  target.        

Three  conversation  patterns  were  found,  differing  in  the  amount  of  elaboration  of   the   topic   of   parenthood   and   work.   The   first   pattern   involved   opinions   and  statements   that  were  not  negotiated  at   all.  The   second  pattern   involved  opinions  and  statements  that  were  supported  by  other  group  members.  The  arguments  gave  strength   to   the   initial   statements.   The   last   and   most   frequent   pattern   was   that  opinions   and   statements   were   challenged,   objected   to   or   disagreed   with.   This  pattern  sometimes  led  to  a  change  of  opinions.    

 

5.3.4 Conclusion It   seems   that   parenthood   evokes   a   lot   of   views   and   opinions.   Although   the  

participants  did  differentiate   between  mothers   and   fathers   they   seemed   to   apply  attributes  of  competence  and  warmth  differently  to  the  targets.  Furthermore,  it  was  also   evident   that   gender   and   gender   composition   seemed   to  matter   as  male   and  female  groups  applied  the  attributes  differently.    

Page 48: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

36  

Similarly   to   paper   II   it   was   found   that   a   richer   conversation   seemed   more  constructive,  here  the  complexity  constituted  not  only  reasoning  activities  but  also  objections   and   counter   arguments.   This   sometimes   led   to   a   change   of   initial  opinions  and  to  more  elaborated  decisions.    

5.4 Paper IV Zakrisson,   I.,   &   Löfstrand,   P.   (2018)   The   multidimensionality   of   gender   –  

implications  for  group-­‐‑based  decision  making.  Submitted  for  publication.    

5.4.1 Background and aim  Paper  II  showed  that  how  a  decision  situation  is  subjectively  experienced  is  of  

importance   in   the   decision   process.   Paper   I   and   III   showed   that   the   framing  conditions   had   impact   on   the   conversational   pattern   in   the   groups.   Paper   III  furthermore   implies   that   gender   composition   in   the   groups   plays   a   role   for   the  decision   process.   None   of   the   previous   presented   papers   deal   with   how   these  factors   are   related   to   the   decision   outcome.   Both   framing   contexts,   as   well   as  gender  composition  can  be  seen  as  gendered  variables.  The  aim  of  this  study  was  to  explore  these  factors  further  in  relation  to  decision  outcome  and  evaluations  of  the  decision  situation.    

The  norms  in  society  about  what  are  expected  from  men  and  women  functions  as  a  backdrop  against  which  men  and  women  are  perceived  and  evaluated.     In  a  decision   situation   societal   norms   form   the   context   against   which   decisions   are  made.    This   can  be   seen  as   structural   gender  variable.  But,   how   these  norms  are  interpreted   in   a   given   situation   is   also   influenced   by   situational   cues.   Gender  composition  might  function  as  such  a  cue  thus  constituting  gender  as  a  situational  variable.  These  can  of  course  interact  which  each  other  and  also  with  peoples  own  gender,  which  thus  serves  as  an  individual  gender  variable.  In  the  present  research  framing  as  goal  setting  and  framing  as  gender  and  parenthood  form  the  structural  level   of   gender   and   gender   composition   in   the   decision   groups   is   regarded   a  situational  level  and  own  gender  as  individual  level.    

 5.4.2 Method

5.4.2.1 Participants Paper  IV  consists  of  two  studies  based  on  both  experiment  I  (The  Relay  Team)  

and  II  (The  Consultant).    The  total  number  of  participants  that  took  part  in  study  I  was   210   (138  women   and   72  men),   forming   54   groups.   The   gender   composition  was   27   female,   10   male   and   17   mixed-­‐‑gender   groups.   The   total   number   of  

Page 49: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

37  

participants  in  study  II  was  272  (179  women  and  93  men).  The  gender  composition  of  the  groups  in  study  II  was  40  female,  16  male  and  15  mixed-­‐‑gender  groups.    

5.4.2.2. Quantitative data Decision   outcome   variables   were   analyzed   on   group   level.   In   study   I   the  

variable   constituted   the   proportion   of   women   chosen   in   relation   to   the   total  number  of  persons  to  choose  from.  In  study  II   it  was  the  mean  rank  of   the  target  person.    

On   individual   level   the   variables   used   were   task   satisfaction,   evaluated  efficiency,  experienced  influence  and  evaluated  democratic  character  as  described  in   chapter   4.   The   independent   variables   in   experiment   1   were   goal   setting   and  gender   composition   in   the   group,   and   in   experiment   2   they   were   gender   and  parental  status  of  the  target,  and  gender  composition  in  the  group.  

 

5.4.3 Results For  decision  outcome  the  independent  variables  had  very  little  effect.  In  study  I  

the  effect  of  gender  composition  was  marginally  significant.  Groups  with  only  men  chose  fewer  women  to  the  team  than  groups  with  only  women.  In  study  II  the  only  significant   effect   was   the   target   candidate’s   gender.   The   groups   considered   the  female  target  regardless  of  her  parental  status  as  a  better  candidate  than  the  male  one.  However,  the  experience  of  decision  situation  differed  depending  on  some  of  the   gendered   variables.   In   study   I   men   regarded   the   process   as   more   efficient,  democratic,   and   experienced   more   influence   in   groups   with   only   men,   while  women  seemed  to  prefer  mixed  gendered  groups  in  the  same  respect.    In  study  II  men  experienced  more  influence  in  male  target  and  male  parent  conditions  while  women  experienced  more  influence  in  the  female  parent  condition.    

 

5.5.4 Conclusion The  results  from  the  two  studies  do  not  present  a  clear  picture.  However  what  

is  similar  in  the  two  studies  is  that  different  conceptualizations  of  gender  seem  to  interact  in  various  ways  in  relation  to  how  a  decision  situation  is  experienced.  Men  seem  more  content  in  male  groups,  with  male  targets  and  male  parent  conditions,  while  women  seemed  more  content  in  mixed  gendered  groups  with  female  parent  conditions.  In  society  there  are  very  few  all-­‐‑female  decision  groups  while  there  are  very  many   all  male   or   to   some   extent  mixed   gendered   decision-­‐‑making   groups.  Hence,  men  and  women  may  differ   in   their   familiarity  with  decision  situations  –  men  more  familiar  with  male  contexts  while  women  are  more  familiar  with  mixed  contexts.  

Page 50: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

38  

 6. GENERAL DISCUSSION  

The   overall   aim   of   this   thesis   was   to   explore   how   stereotypes   were  communicated,   negotiated   and   constructed   within   communication   in   different  decision-­‐‑making   contexts.   Furthermore,   the   aim   was   to   explore   which   parts   of  group  communication  that  make  a  decision-­‐‑making  process  more  or  less  successful.  Finally,   the   aim   was   to   problematize   the   gender   concept   and   study   gender   in  decision-­‐‑making   situations   from   a   situational,   structural   and   individual  perspective.    

6.1 Stereotyping and context As   mentioned   in   the   introduction   a   dominant   paradigm   in   group   decision-­‐‑

making  is  to  use  a  context  presented  in  achievements  terms,  the  goal  is  most  often  operationalized  as  points,  grades  or  money  (Ahn,  et  al.,  2003),  and  a  broader  range  of   goals   has   been   inquired   for   (Kray   &   Thompson,   2005).   There   is   however  research  that  has  compared  competitive  and  non-­‐‑competitive  contexts,  which  has  found  differences  regarding  forms  of  interaction  within  groups  (van  Knippenberg,  et   al.,   2001;   Toma   &   Butera,   2015).   In   the   present   research   interaction   process  analysis   (Bales,   1999)   was   used   to   study   variations   in   communication   patterns  depending   on   the   goal   set   for   the   decision   task,   but  with   just  minor   differences  found.  Instead  what  seemed  to  matter  was  how  stereotypes  presented  themselves  within   the   communication.   In   groups   with   a   competitive   goal   stereotypes   were  used  to  include  and  exclude  people.  In  groups  with  a  cooperative  goal  stereotypes  were  used  only  to  include  people.  Furthermore,  for  groups  with  a  competitive  goal,  the  dimensions  of  warmth  and  competence  were  used  both  to  include  and  exclude  runners   to   the   team.   The   fictive   runners  were   described   as   high/low   on   the   two  stereotype  content  dimensions.  For  groups  with  a  cooperative  goal,  mostly  warmth  was  used  to  include  runners  in  the  team.  Furthermore,  the  non-­‐‑competitive  groups  rarely  talked  in  terms  of  competence.  The  conclusion  from  this  is  that  competitive  goals  lead  to  inclusion  and  exclusion  mechanisms  where  stereotypes  are  used  both  positively  and  negatively.  A  non-­‐‑competitive  goal  feeds  the  inclusion  mechanisms,  where   stereotypes   are  used  positively.  Groups  with   a  non-­‐‑competitive  goal  used  attributes   related   to   both   warmth   and   competence,   while   no   references   to   the  warmth   dimension   were   found   in   groups   with   a   competitive   goal   setting.   This  indicates   that   the   groups   in   the   non-­‐‑competitive   condition   could   not   free  themselves   from   the   nature   of   the   task   –   to   be   competitive.   They   even   had   to  remind  themselves  that  the  task  was  to  form  a  team  with  a  good  group  atmosphere  and  not  to  win  the  competition.    

Page 51: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

39  

How  stereotypes  are  used  when  judging  other  people  is  predominately  studied  on  individual  level  and  very  seldom  introduced  in  a  group  setting.  By  varying  the  goals  set  for  the  task  it  is  evident  that  this  forms  a  context  against  which  the  same  information   is   interpreted   in   different   ways   and   stereotypes   are   thus  communicated  differently.  The  goals   function  as   some  kind  of  meta-­‐‑information.  This   is   also   in   line   with   other   research   where   meta-­‐‑information   has   been   more  explicitly  varied  (Rijnbout  &  McKimmie;  2012;  Wolgast,  2017).  

A  more  direct  way  of  investigating  the  influence  of  context  is  to  explicitly  vary  gender   relevant   information.   This   was   done   in   experiment   II   “The   Consultant”.  The  setup  was  modeled  after  Cuddy,  et  al.  (2004)  and  Heilman  and  Okimoto  (2008)  but   transferred   to   a   group   situation   in   order   to   investigate   how   stereotype  dimensions   such   as   competence   and   warmth   were   communicated.   The   purpose  was  mainly   to  study  gender  stereotypes  and  where  parenthood  was  supposed  to  function  as  an  amplifier  of  gender  stereotypes.  However,  in  the  group  discussions  gender   seemed   to   be   relatively  unimportant,   the  discussions  were  dominated  by  the   “child   issue”,   far  more   than   other   variables   such   as   preferred  working   style,  hobbies   or   education.   By   use   of   a   brief   variant   of   the   conversational   argument  coding   scheme   (Canary  &  Seibold,   2010)   three  different   communication  patterns,  differing   in  depth  of  communication  and  argumentation,  were  revealed.  The  first  pattern   involved   opinions   and   statements   that   were   not   responded   to   from   the  other  discussants.  The  second  pattern  also   involved  opinions  and  statements,  but  instead,  they  were  met  by  arguments  to  give  strength  to  the  initial  statements.  The  third   pattern   was   the   most   frequent   communication   pattern.   This   pattern   also  involved  statements  and  opinions  that  were  responded  to,  but  here  the  arguments  were  challenged,  objected  to  and  disagreed  with.  

Similarly   to   the   framing   of   context   in   experiment   I   also   in   this   second  experiment  the  framing  of  context  resulted  in  different  conversational  pattern  and  how   stereotypes   manifested   themselves   in   the   discussions.   However,   what   was  surprising  was   that   gender   had   very   little   importance   and   the   discussions  were  dominated   by   parenthood   and   its   possible   benefits   and   disadvantages   for   the  individual   and   the   organization.  How   come?   There   could   be   several   answers   to  this   questions,   the   simplest   answer   are   that   it   is   only   a   question   of   social  desirability   (Bem,   Wallach   &   Kogan,   1965).   However,   the   results   in   paper   IV  regarding   the  decision  outcome   showed   that   the   female   candidate  was  preferred  over   the  male  one  but   that   there  was  no  difference   in   rank  between  parents   and  non-­‐‑parents.  If  it  were  only  a  question  of  social  desirability  they  would  be  assumed  to  discuss  the  benefits  of  the  female  target  and  disadvantages  of  the  male  target  to  a  greater  extent  and  parenthood  would  not  have  been  an   issue   in   the  discussion.  Sooner   they   seemed   reluctant   to   introduce   gender   in   the   discussions   and  

Page 52: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

40  

parenthood  seemed  to  be  a  safer  topic.  Another  explanation  could  be  that  men  and  women  having  a   job   in   fact   is  not  an   issue  according   to  young  adults   in  Sweden  today.  The  relative  frequency  of  men  and  women  working  is  of  about  the  same  size,  (SCB,  2016)  although   there  are   inequalities   in  other  ways,   for  example,   regarding  salaries  and  career  opportunities  (Kugelberg,  2006).  Nor  should  parenthood  be  an  issue   in   recruitment   situations   and   there   is   evidence   that   employers   do   not  discriminate  against  parents  in  Sweden  (Bygren,  Erlandsson  &  Gähler,  2017)  but  it  is   perhaps   less   common   that   information   about   parenthood   status   is   revealed   in  job   applications.   Thus,   that   the   applicants   differed   as   to   gender   in   the   present  situation  is  more  or  less  what  is  expected  but  information  about  parenthood  is  thus  something  extraordinarily  that  the  attention  of  the  groups  is  directed  to.  This  again  proves   that   the   context   is   an   important   factor   for  how   information   is   interpreted  and  negotiated  but   this   can   also   take  different   forms.  The   conversational  pattern  analysis   showed   that   a  more   elaborated   conversational   pattern   seemed   to   be   the  most  constructive  one  as  it  displayed  how  opinions  could  change  within  one  and  the  same  session.    

Gender  composition  was  introduced  in  the  experiments  as  a  situational  aspect  of   gender.   However,   this   variable   did   not   show   as   clear-­‐‑cut   patterns   regarding  stereotypes   as   the   two   framing   setups   did.   There   was   some   evidence   that   it  affected   the   decision   outcome   in   the   way   that   all   women   and   mixed   groups  showed   similar   decision   patterns   compared   to   groups   with   only   men,   although  these  differences  were  moderately  weak.    The  results  are  however,  in  line  with  the  scarce   research   investigating   gender   composition   in   relation   to   decision-­‐‑making  (Hannagan  &  Larimer,  2010).  Nor  showed  the  qualitative  analyses  in  experiment  I  any   explicit   pattern.   Gender   composition  was   not   explicitly   tested   in   relation   to  communication  patterns  and  stereotypes   in  paper   I  and  II,  but   from  an   inductive  perspective  it  would  have  manifested  itself  had  systematic  differences  existed  as  it  did  in  paper  III.  Here  group  composition  intersected  with  the  gender  of  the  target  both   regarding   competence   and   warmth.   So,   although   the   main   discussions  referred   to   parenthood,   when   the   material   was   analyzed   in   relation   to   gender  composition  it  seemed  as  the  target’s  gender  mattered  to  some  degree  after  all.  The  general   pattern   was   that   groups   with   only   women   and   groups   with   only   men  differed  but  that  mixed  gendered  groups  did  not  display  any  particular  pattern.  In  such   groups,  where   both  men   and  women   are   present,   the   gender   concept   cues  another   kind   of   information,   which   seems   to   be   of   consequences   for   the  communication  pattern  where  systematic  gender  differences  seem  to  disappear.      

Page 53: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

41  

6.2 The role of experiences The  previous  section  discussed  communication  patterns  both  as  to  content  and  

form  in  relation  to  stereotypes  and  context  variables  and  gender  composition.  This  says   nothing   about   how   the   participants   experience   and   evaluate   the   situation.  Even  though  these  various  factors   influences  how  constructive  a  decision  process  is,  the  participants  may  not  necessarily  experience  it  in  the  same  way.  Even  though  there   is   no   absolute   consensus   about  what   leads   to   positive   experiences   (Barr  &  Gold,   2014)   there   is   evidence   that   satisfaction   (DeStephen   &   Hirokawa,   1988),  influence   (Kong,   et   al.,   2015),   information   sharing   (Larson   et   al,   1998;   Tasa   &  Whyte,   2005),   and   the   extent   to  which  deviant  opinions   are   allowed   (De  Dreu  &  West,  2001)  are  of  importance.  The  results  in  paper  II  revealed  that  the  discussions  were   broader   in   groups   defined   as   successful   based   on   the   participants  experienced  satisfaction,  perceived  equality  of  influence  and  experienced  efficiency.  They   used   more   reasoning   activities,   gave   each   other   more   responses   and   also  argued   more   against   each   other   than   participants   in   groups   defined   as  unsuccessful.  The  successful  groups  also  displayed  more  of  all  of  the  conversation  categories,  which  suggest  a  more  complex  decision-­‐‑making  process.  According  to  the  research  presented  above  this  is  essential  for  the  quality  of  decisions  made.    As  discussed  before  a  more  complex  discussion  pattern,  as  revealed  in  paper  III,  also  seem  to  inhibit  the  use  of  stereotypes.  This  means  that  it  is  not  the  amount  of  input  information   that   is   important   for   the  decision-­‐‑making  process.   Instead   it   is  what  the  group  allows  the  members  to  do  with  it.    

Paper  II  showed  that  subjective  experiences  are  of  importance  for  the  decision-­‐‑making   process.   The   next   step  was   to   see   if   experiences  were   dependent   on   the  context   variables   and   the   gender   composition   in   the   groups.   It   is   reasonable   to  assume  that  a  situation  you  are   familiar  with   is  experienced  as  more  comfortable  (Rijnbout   &   McKimmie,   2012).   Given   that   a   competitive   context   is   a   norm   in  society  (Pratto,  et  al.,  2006)  together  with  that  career  decisions  are  more  often  about  men  where  parenthood  is  not  an  issue  (Cuddy,  et  al.,  2004)  men  and  women  might  be   differently   familiar   with   such   situations   and   hence   will   experience   them  differently.   Adding   to   this,   gender   composition   might   also   lead   to   different  experiences   (Hannagan,  2006).   Indeed  this  was  also  what  was  found  in  paper   IV.  Own   gender   seemed   to   interact   with   gender   composition   and   the   framing  variables.   Men   seemed   to   be   more   content   in   groups   with   only   men   and   male  target  conditions  and  male  target  conditions  while  women  seemed  to  prefer  mixed  gendered   groups   and   female   target   conditions.   This   is   not   surprising   as   male  dominated   decision   situations   are   a   norm   in   society   and   all   female   decision-­‐‑making  groups  rarely  exist   (International  Labour  Office,  2016;  Petersson,  Catásus  &  Danielsson,  2016).  

Page 54: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

42  

6.3 Methodological considerations A   number   of  methodological   issues   need   to   be   discussed   before   implications  

from  the  present  research  are  introduced.  First,  the  participants  were  students  from  different  study  programs.  Even  if  no  

differences  according  to  study  program  were  found,  the  choice  of  students  might  not  be  transferrable  to  other  groups  in  society.  With  another  sample,  for  example,  people   active   in   sports   associations   in   experiment   I,   and   people   working   with  recruitment   in  experiment   II,   the  results  could  have  been  different.  However,   the  results   might   not   have   been   weaker.   With   people   who   are   familiar   with   the  experimental  conditions,  the  results  could  instead  have  been  strengthened.    

The   second   limitation   is   the   choice   of  ad-­‐‑hoc  groups   instead  of   real   decision-­‐‑making   groups.  As   ad-­‐‑hoc   groups   are   immature   and   the   participants   know   that  they  will  not  work  together  in  similar  situations  in  the  future,  this  could  affect  the  results.   However,   previous   research   has   revealed   that   ad-­‐‑hoc   groups   in  experimental  settings  are  as  stable  as  real  decision-­‐‑making  groups  (Dubrovsky,  et  al.,   1991),   and   many   recent   studies   in   GDM   have   been   conducted   with   ad-­‐‑hoc  groups   (e.g.   Hogg,   Sherman,   Dierselhuis,   Maitner   &   Moffit,   2007;   Nawata   &  Yamaguchi,  2011;  Kahai,  Huang  &  Jestice,  2012).    

A  third  aspect  related  to  the  experimental  design  could  be  the  time  constraint  that   forces   the   group   to   be  more   cooperative   than   if   they  did  not   have   any   time  limit,   something   which   has   been   revealed   in   previous   research   (Cone   &   Rand,  2014).   However,   as   differences   were   found   it   seems   that   the   time   limit   set   was  enough  for  making  emergent  communication  patterns  and  subjective  experiences  visible.    

A   fourth   limitation   could   be   that   this   thesis   uses   an   unorthodox   design   and  perhaps   could   the   lack   of   relevant   previous   studies   be   a   limitation.   The  application   of   the   stereotype   content   model   may   be   an   innovative   approach   to  break   down   the   stereotype   dimensions   in   order   to   see   how   they   are   negotiated.  However,  this  is  a  quite  new  approach  as  the  SCM  mostly  is  used  on  an  individual  level   and   related   to   how   individuals   perceive   people   associated   with   different  groups.  There  is  in  other  words  a  need  for  more  future  research  to  give  strength  to  the  validity  of  the  stereotype  content  analysis  that  was  used  in  this  thesis.    

A  final  aspect  that  could  be  a  limitation  is  the  choice  of  a  qualitative  approach  within   an   experimental   design.   This   is   a   quite   unusual   design   to   conduct   group  research.  Most   often,   qualitative   research   is  made   in   the   field  with   real  decision-­‐‑making  groups  and  quantitative  methods   chosen   in   experimental   settings  on  ad-­‐‑hoc   groups.   A   strength   with   this   approach   is,   however,   that   qualitative   and  

Page 55: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

43  

quantitative  methods  were  used  in  conjunction  with  each  other.  This  gives  a  richer  picture  of  the  results.    

A  couple  of  remarks  regarding  the  research  ethics  have  to  be  made.  First  of  all  the  participants  were  not  informed  in  advance  of  the  specific  aim  of  the  research,  as   this   would   jeopardize   the   results.   If   they   had   knowledge   of   the   independent  variable  they  could  adjust  their  responses  in  line  with  this  information.  However,  they  were  debriefed  immediately  afterwards  and  were  told  that  they  could  refrain  from   participation.   A   second   remark   is   that   the   experiments   were   most   often  conducted   by   the   students´   teachers,   which   put   the   participants   in   a   dependent  situation.   Participation   was   in   many   cases   part   of   course   requirements,   but  participation   in   the   research  was   voluntary.   Again,   in   the   debriefing   afterwards  they  were   informed   explicitly   that   they   could   refrain   from  participation.   Finally,  the  decision   sessions  were   filmed  which  means   that   the   collected  data   contained  personal   information   to   be   stored   for   considerable  period.   The  participants  were  also   informed   of   this   and   if   they   consented   to   participation   in   the   research   and  handling  of  personal  information  they  signed  an  agreement  form.    

6.4 Practical implications One  of  the  things  that  this  thesis  revealed  was  that  competitive  goals  are  easier  to  understand  for  a  group  in  a  decision-­‐‑making  situation  than  a  non-­‐‑competitive  goal.  This   as   groups   in   a   non-­‐‑competitive   task   could   not   free   themselves   from   the  competitive   task.  So,  what  does   that   tell  us?   I   think   that   it   indicates   the  need   for  groups   to   discuss   goals   and   ambitions   and   how   it   might   affect   both   the   inner  mechanisms  in  the  group  and  how  it  might  affect  the  way  the  members  talk  about  each   other   and   persons   outside   the   group.   This   as   it   seem   as   the   normative  competitive  environment  triggers  inclusion  and  exclusion  mechanisms  and  the  use  of  stereotypes.  For  sports  associations,  especially  those  involved  in  youth  sports,  it  is   important   to   understand   that   a   competitive   setting   strengthen   stereotypes   of  warmth   and   competence   both   to   include   in-­‐‑group  members   and   to   exclude   out-­‐‑groups.  Moreover,  in  competitive  youth  sports  environments  there  is  a  risk  that  the  stereotypes  generalize  outside  the  actual  sports  team  and  follow  the  youngsters  to  other  environments  such  as  for  example  school.  The  need  for  sports  associations  to  understand   these   mechanisms   is   huge,   especially   since   seventy   percent   of   the  youth  in  Sweden  are  involved  in  sports  activities  at   least  once  a  week  (Statistiska  centralbyrån,  2017).  The  number  of  people  being  involved  as  trainers  and  leaders  in   sports  associations   is   considerable   (Riksidrottsförbundet,   2011).  How  goals  are  discussed  and  mediated  within  an  association,  and  how  they  influence  stereotype  use  and  inclusion  and  exclusion  mechanisms,  is  crucial  to  understand  and  should  be  included  in  the  educational  programs  of  sports  associations  at  national  and  local  

Page 56: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

44  

level.  My  answer  is  not  to  forbid  competitive  goals  but  they  need  to  be  tuned  down  and   the   cooperative   goals   need   to   take  much  more   place,   not   least,   as   inclusion  patterns  seem  to  emerge  in  that  context.  This  dilemma  is  not  new  and  it  probably  has  to  be  handled  over  and  over  again,  or  as  Vince  Lombardi,  a  famous  American  football  coach  said:    ”People  who  work  together  will  win,  whether  it  be  against  complex  football  defenses,  or  

the  problems  of  modern  society”.  (Vince  Lombardi,  football  coach,  Benson,  2008).    For   parenthood   and   gender   the   results   clearly   showed   that   “the   child   issue”  

triggers  a  lot  of  discussion  and  use  of  stereotypes  in  recruitment  situations.  In  fact  it  seemed  to  be  more  important  than  both  information  of  work-­‐‑life  experience  and  formal   education.   As   previously   mentioned   gender   was   not   introduced   to   the  discussions,  perhaps  the  answer  is  that  young  people  in  Sweden  today  knows  that  gender  shouldn’t  be  an  issue  in  recruitment  situations.    

Results   from   the   thesis   also   tell   us   something   about   how   to   create   a   good  decision-­‐‑making   group.   Contexts   seem   to   be   important   for   how   information   is  interpreted.  Moreover,  elaboration  of  arguments  seemed  to  be  most  constructive  as  it  displayed  how   stereotypes   could   change,   even  within   in   the   same   session   and  within  the  same  individual.  These  results  could  all  be  used  in  order  to  enhance  the  quality   of   decision-­‐‑making   in   groups.   By   setting   up   counter-­‐‑stereotypical  contextual   information,   as   well   as   having   discussion   rules   asking   for   counter-­‐‑arguments   more   elaborate   decision   patterns   may   emerge.   More   complex  conversation   patterns   are   not   only   important   for   satisfaction   it   also   prevents   the  use  of  stereotypes.    

There  has  been  a  big  change  in  the  workforce  in  recent  decades.  More  and  more  men  work  in  typically  female-­‐‑dominated  occupations,  and  more  and  more  women  have   taken   place   in   traditionally  male-­‐‑dominated  work   areas.  However,   there   is  still   large   disparities   between   different   areas   in   the   work   place,   i.e.   men   still  dominate  the  better-­‐‑paid  jobs  in  industry  and  there  are  more  men  than  women  in  the  boardrooms.  Women  on   the  other  hand  dominate   the   less  paid   jobs   in   social  service,  health  and  education  (World  Economic  Forum,  2015).  The  political  debate  about  gender  and  equality  in  work  life  is  often  related  to  the  representation  in  the  boardrooms.   Still   in   2016,   in   all   societies   around   the   globe,   men   are   to   a   much  higher  degree   represented   in   the  boardrooms   (International  Labour  Office,  2016).  All   in   all,  men   in   important   –   financial   –   sectors   are  more   often   in   charge   of   the  decision-­‐‑making  and  have  the  majority  of  the  power  positions  (Smith,  2002).    Some  countries   are   considering   political   action   such   as   affirmative   action   to   change  inequality.  For  example,  Norway,  Iceland,  Israel  and  Spain  have  applied  laws  that  allocate   a  percentage  of  women   to   the   boardrooms   (Brandth,  &  Bjørkhaug,   2015;  Sheridan,  Ross-­‐‑Smith  &  Lord,   2015).   But   counting  heads   is   perhaps   not   the   only  

Page 57: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

45  

solution,   although   the   present   research   indicates   that   gender   composition   is   not  trivial.  But  the  research  also  shows  that  gender  is  a  complex  phenomenon,  and  that  a  greater   repertoire  of   actions  need   to  be  used.    Affirmative  action  might   lead   to  more  gender  balance  in  power  positions,  but  more  actions  are  needed  if  we  really  want  a  change.    

6.5 Future research A  number  of  research  ideas  have  emerged  during  the  work  with  this  thesis.  In  

the   present   research   (paper   III)   the   conversation   patterns   within   the   parent  condition  was  analyzed,  and  it  was  found  that  all  groups  discussed  “the  child  issue”  a  lot.  But  how  do  these  groups  differ  from  those  where  the  parenthood  status  was  not  explicitly  mentioned?  Thus  there  is  a  need  to  analyze  also  how  groups  without  a   parent   target   discussed   the   different   candidates.   Do   more   gender   related  stereotypes   emerge   in   these   groups?   This   could   be   done   with   the   use   of   the  conversational   argument   coding   scheme   and/or   an   analysis   of   the   stereotype  contents.    

The  “parenthood”  design  would  also  work  very  well  with  other  discrimination  bases.  First,  ethnicity  could  easily  be  manipulated  by  using  a  non-­‐‑normative  name  for   the   target  person.  A  previous   study  conducted  on   real  persons  working  with  recruitment  revealed  that  a  person  with  a  non-­‐‑normative  name  needs   to  be  rated  higher   on   both   competence   and   warmth   compared   to   his   competitors   to   be  selected   for   an   interview,  while   a  man  with   a   normative   name   only   needs   to   be  rated  higher  in  one  of  the  two  dimensions  (Agerström,  et  al.,  2012).  

Another   aspect   that   might   have   an   impact   on   the   stereotype   dimensions  warmth   and   competence   is   sexual   orientation.   Previous   research   reveals   that  sexuality   is   strongly   related   to   stereotyping,   prejudice   and   discrimination   (e.g.  Fraïssé,  &   Barrientos,   2016;  Glick  &   Fiske,   2001;   Peplau  &   Fingerhut;   2004).     But  there  is  a  need  for  experiments  in  the  area  to  explore  how  people  communicate  and  negotiate  stereotypes  contents  related  to  sexual  orientation  especially  in  relation  to  power  positions.    

Finally,  the  use  of  different  goal  setting,  here  related  to  a  sports  scenario  would  also   be   of   interest   to   study   within   other   situations,   for   example   to   investigate  different  power  mechanisms   in  decision-­‐‑making  groups.  For  example   to   find  out  how  different  context  (goal  setting)  affects  democratically  related  components  in  a  political  decision-­‐‑making  group.    

Page 58: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

46  

6.6 The Bottom Line We  know  from  a  theoretical  perspective  that  people  use  stereotypes  in  daily  life  

to  process  information  about  other  people  (Fiske,  et  al,  2002).  The  stereotype  bias  is  formed   from   structures   in   society   and   some   groups   are   viewed   as  more   or   less  competent  and  some  as  more  or  less  warm  (Fiske,  2015).    

Previous   research  has   revealed   that   stereotypes   function  as  a   tool   to  maintain  structures   in   society   (Dovidio,   et   al.,   2010).     With   the   knowledge   that   attitudes  should  be  seen  as  constructs  (Howe  &  Krosnick,  2017)  and  that  the  strength  of  an  attitude  is  critical  for  the  possibility  to  change  it.  It  was  interesting  to  study  which  effect  negotiation  and  communication  had  on   the   stereotype  dimensions  warmth  and   competence.   The   way   a   decision-­‐‑making   group   solves   a   problem,   handles  difficulties  or  develops  manifests  stereotypes  about  others,  plays  an  important  role  in   society.   Often,   rational   choice   or   prisoners’   dilemmas   are   chosen   in   the  experimental  design  when  group  decision-­‐‑making  is  studied.    The  experiments  in  this  thesis  were  instead  designed  to  frame  either  the  context  or  the  object  without  competitive  achievement  goals.  The  aim  was  to  study  the  role  that  stereotypes  had  in  decision-­‐‑making   in  groups.  The  methodology   chosen   to   study   communication  and   the   communication   patterns   was   mainly   qualitative.   Both   previously   used  methods   such   as   interaction   process   analyses   and   conversational   arguments  coding   scheme   and   two   invented   methods   Stereotype   Content   Analysis   and  Communication  Pattern  Analysis  were  used.  The  main  reason  to  apply  new  methods  was  to  get  a  better  understanding  of  how  differences  such  as  gender  composition  and   framing   in   different   decision-­‐‑making   contexts   might   affect   the   use   of  stereotypes.   This   thesis   has   explored   how   stereotypes  may   be   constructed  when  people  communicate,  negotiate  and  make  decisions  together.    

All  in  all,  the  results  from  this  thesis  indicate  both  challenges  from  a  democratic  perspective  and  from  a  gender  perspective.    

This   thesis,   along   with   previous   research,   reveals   that   stereotypes   have   an  important  role  in  decision-­‐‑making  situations.  How  stereotypes  are  negotiated  and  communicated  is  affected  by  contextual  factors.  Finally,  people  can  simultaneously  be  seen  as  social  animals  (Aronson,  1984),  social  beings  (Fiske,  2014)  and  relational  beings  (Gergen,  2011).    

        ”Keep  on  rockin’  in  a  free  world”,  Neil  Young,  1989    

Page 59: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

47  

7. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS  

Without   support   from  many  people  had   this   thesis   been   impossible   to   finish.  First  of  all  do  I  have  to  bring  many  thanks  to  my  supervisor  and  colleague  Ingrid  Zakrisson  for  years  of  discussions  with  so  much  help  and  when  in  need  a  push  to  come  forward.  I  also  want  to  thank  my  two  co-­‐‑supervisors  Richard  Ahlström  and  Patric  Millet  for  framing  my  scientific  mind  and  for  suggestions.  

 I  also  want  to  thank  all  colleagues  from  the  department  of  psychology  at  Mid  Sweden   University,   for   advices   and   ideas   from   seminars,   meetings,   and   coffee  room  discussions.  I  also  want  to  bring  thanks  to  colleagues  from  other  departments  at  Mid  Sweden  for  good  companionship.  

 A   special   thanks   to   one   of   my   first   university   teachers   Sten   Antilla   who  probably   don’t   remember,   but  who  with   a   small   question   probably   changed   the  way   I   framed  myself.   It  was   after   a   course   in  methodology   that   I   talked   to   Sten  about  options   for   the   future.   I  was  at   the  moment  a  young  student  who  was   the  first   from   the   family   to  attend  university   studies.  When  he   suggested  me   to  be  a  teacher  was  my  answer  “I  don’t  think  I  want  work  with  education  and  small  children”.  Sten  responded,  “I  meant  a  university  teacher”,  that  certainly  changed  something  for  me.      

I   also   want   to   bring   thanks   to   colleagues   from   Universities   abroad   at   John  Moores,   Bremen,   Trier,   Stenden   and   Coe   College   where   the   ideas   of   the  parenthood  experiment   took   form.  Mostly   thanks   to   the  whole  bunch  of  political  psychologist  from  all  around  the  globe  that  I  had  the  opportunity  to  spend  a  great  time  together  with  at  Stanford  last  summer.  I  also  want  to  thank  all  students  that  I  have  met  during  my  process;  you  are  a  source  that  always  forces  me  to  bring  out  the  best  for  my  lectures.    

There   have   also   been   a   lot   of   persons   that   in   different   degrees   has   been  involved  in  my  experiments.    Henrik,  Emma,  Katarina  and  Kibebe,  thank  you  for  the  help  with  different  parts  with  the  experiments.  I  would  also  like  to  thank  all  the  participants   in   my   experiments,   without   whom   not   much   would   have   been  achieved.    

 Another   special   thanks   to  Ulrika  Danielsson  and  Kerstin  Weimer   for   reading  my  manuscript   for   the   half-­‐‑time   seminar   and   also   to   Eva  Hammar  Chiriac   from  Linköping  for  important  suggestions  from  this  seminar.  Another  special  thanks  to  Lina   Eriksson   and   Francisco   Esteves   for   reading   my   manuscript   for   my   final  seminar  and  to  Michael  Rosander  for  important  suggestions  at  the  final  seminar.      

Page 60: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

48  

Since  a  lot  of  my  ideas  comes  from  life  outside  the  laboratory  do  I  also  want  to  thank   colleagues   from  my   political   life   and   a   special   thanks   to   everyone   that   is  evolved  in  the  sports  association  Ope  IF.    

I   also   want   to   thank   my   ”brother   from   the   academy”,  Ulrik   Terp   for   years   of  inspiration,  discussions  and  fun  together.    Thanks  to  mom  and  dad  and  my  brother  and  his  family  for  always  being  there.  

   Finally,  my  most  important  acknowledgement  goes  to  my  wife,  Monica  and  my  

girls  Minna  &  Sofia  who  continuously  has  supported  me  with  love  and  enthusiasm.                                                              

Page 61: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

49  

8. REFERENCES  Agerström,  J.,  Björklund,  F.,  Carlsson,  R.,  &  Rooth,  D.  (2012).  Warm  and  competent  

Hassan   =   cold   and   incompetent   Eric:   A   harsh   equation   of   real-­‐‑life   hiring  discrimination.  Basic  and  Applied  Social  Psychology,  34(4),  359-­‐‑366.  

Ahn,   T.   K.,   Ostrom,   E.,   Schmidt,   D.,   &   Walker,   J.   (2003).   Trust   in   two-­‐‑person  games:  Game  structures  and  linkages.  In  E.  Ostrom  &  J.  Walker  (Eds.),  Trust  and  reciprocity  (pp.  323-­‐‑351).  New  York,  NY:  Russell  Sage  Foundation.  

Allport,   F.   H.   (1920).   The   influence   of   the   group   upon   association   and   thought.  Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology,  3(3),  159-­‐‑182.  

Allport,  G.W.  (1954).  The  nature  of  prejudice.  Reading,  MA:  Addison-­‐‑Wesley.    Ambady,   N.,   &   Rosenthal,   R.   (1992).   Thin   slices   of   expressive   behavior   as  

predictors   of   interpersonal   consequences:   A   meta-­‐‑analysis.   Psychological  Bulletin,  111(2),  256-­‐‑274.  

Apesteguia,  J.,  Azmat,  G.,  &  Iriberri,  N.  (2012).  The  impact  of  gender  composition  on   team   performance   and   decision   making:   Evidence   from   the   field.  Management  Science,  58  (1),  78-­‐‑93.  

Aronson,  E.  (1984).  The  social  animal.  New  York:  W.H.  Freeman.  Asch,  S.  (1956).  Studies  of  independence  and  conformity:  A  minority  of  one  against  

a  unanimous  majority.  Psychological  Monographs,  70  (Whole  No.416).  Baird,   K.,   &  Wang,   H.   (2010).   Employee   empowerment:   Extent   of   adoption   and  

influential  factors.  Personnel  Review,  39(5),  574-­‐‑599.  Bales,   R.F.   (1950).  A   set   of   categories   for   the   analysis   of   small   group   interaction.  

Americal  Sociological  Review,  15,  257-­‐‑263.    Bales,  R.F.  (1999).  Social  interaction  systems.    New  Brunswick,  NJ:  Transaction  Books.    

Barr,   S.,   &   Gold,   J.   M.   (2014).   Redundant   visual   information   enhances   group  decisions.   Journal   of   Experimental   Psychology:   Human   Perception   and  Performance,  40(6),  2124-­‐‑2130.  

Barreto,  M.,  Ryan,  M.K.,  &  Schmitt,  M.T.   (2009).   Introduction:   Is   the  glass   ceiling  still  relevant  in  the  21st  century?  In  M.  Barreto,  M.K.  Ryan,  &  M.T.  Schmitt  (Eds.),   The   glass   ceiling   in   the   21st   century:   Understanding   barriers   to   gender  equality  (pp.3-­‐‑18).  Washington,  DC:  American  Psychological  Association.    

Beck,   S.J.,   Gronewold,   K.L.,   &  Western,   K.J.   (2012).   Intergroup   argumentation   in  city   government   decision-­‐‑making:   The   wal-­‐‑mart   dilemma.   Small   Group  Research,  43(5),  587-­‐‑612.  

Page 62: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

50  

Behfar,  K.J.,  Mannix,  E.A.,  Peterson,  R.S.,  &  Trochim,  W.M.  (2011).  Conflict  in  small  groups:   The   meaning   and   consequences   of   process   conflict.   Small   Group  Research,  42,  127-­‐‑176.    

Bem,  D.  J.,  Wallach,  M.  A.,  &  Kogan,  N.  (1965).  Group  decision  making  under  risk  of   aversive   consequences.   Journal   of   Personality   and   Social   Psychology,   1(5),  453-­‐‑460).  

Bergh,   R.,   Akrami,  N.,   Sidanius,   J.,   &   Sibley,   C.  G.   (2016).   Is   group  membership  necessary  for  understanding  generalized  prejudice?  A  re-­‐‑evaluation  of  why  prejudices  are  interrelated.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  111(3),  367-­‐‑395.  

Benson,  M.  (2008)  Winning  words,  classic  quotes  from  the  world  of  sports.  Lanham,  MD:  Taylor  Trade  Publishing.    

Billig,  M.  (1997).  Discursive,  rhetorical,  and  ideological  messages.  In  C.  McGarty,  &  S.  A.  Haslam   (Eds.),  The  message   of   social   psychology:  Perspectives   on  mind   in  society;  the  message  of  social  psychology:  (pp.  36-­‐‑53),  Malden,  NY:  Blackwell.    

Billig,   M.   (2015).   Kurt   Lewin’s   leadership   studies   and   his   legacy   to   social  psychology:   Is   there   nothing   as   practical   as   a   good   theory.   Journal   for   the  Theory  of  Social  Behaviour,  45(4),  440-­‐‑460.  

Binggeli,   S.,   Krings,   F.,   &   Sczesny,   S.   (2014).   Stereotype   content   associated   with  immigrant  groups  in  switzerland.  Swiss  Journal  of  Psychology,  73(3),  123-­‐‑133.    

Bobbitt-­‐‑Zeher,   D.   (2011).   Gender   discrimination   at   work:   Connecting   gender  stereotypes,   institutional   policies,   and   gender   composition   of   workplace.  Gender  &  Society,  25(6),  764-­‐‑786.  

Bodenhausen,  G.  V.   (2005).  The   role  of   stereotypes   in  decision-­‐‑making  processes.  Medical  Decision  Making,  25(1),  112-­‐‑118.  

Bodenhausen,  G.  V.,  &  Macrae,  C.  N.  (1998).  Stereotype  activation  and  inhibition.  In  R.  S.  Wyer  Jr.  (Ed.),  Stereotype  activation  and  inhibition;  stereotype  activation  and   inhibition,   (pp.   1-­‐‑52),   Mahwah,   NJ:   Lawrence   Erlbaum   Associates  Publishers.  

Bodenhausen,  G.  V.,  Mussweiler,  T.,  Gabriel,  S.,  &  Moreno,  K.  N.  (2001).  Affective  influences   on   stereotyping   and   intergroup   relations.   In   J.   P.   Forgas   (Ed.),  Handbook  of  affect  and  social  cognition;  handbook  of  affect  and  social  cognition,  (pp.  319-­‐‑343).  Mahwah,  NJ:  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates  Publishers.  

 Brandth,   B.,   &   Bjørkhaug,   H.   (2015).   Gender   quotas   for   agricultural   boards:  Changing  constructions  of  gender?  Gender,  Work  and  Organization,  22(6),  614-­‐‑628.  

Brauer,   M.,   Judd,   C.   M.,   &   Jacquelin,   V.   (2001).   The   communication   of   social  stereotypes:  The  effects  of  group  discussion  and  information  distribution  on  

Page 63: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

51  

stereotypic  appraisals.   Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  81(3),  463-­‐‑475.    

Brewer,  M.  B.,  &  Kramer,  R.  M.  (1986).  Choice  behavior  in  social  dilemmas:  Effects  of  social  identity,  group  size,  and  decision  framing.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  50(3),  543-­‐‑549.  

Brown,   R.,   (1999)  Group   processes:   Dynamics   within   and   between   groups.   Oxford:  WileyBlackwell.  

Bygren,   M.,   Erlandsson,   A.,   &   Gähler,   M.   (2017).   Do   employers   prefer   fathers?  Evidence   from   a   field   experiment   testing   the   gender   by   parenthood  interaction  effect  on  callbacks  to  job  applications.  European  Sociological  Review,  33(3),  337-­‐‑348.  

Byrnes,  D.  A.,  &  Kiger,  G.  (1990),  The  effect  of  a  prejudice-­‐‑reduction  simulation  on  attitude  Change.  Journal  of  Applied  Social  Psychology,  20,    341–356.  

Canary,   D.J.,   &   Seibold,   D.R.   (2010).   Origins   and   development   of   the  Conversational   Argument   Coding   Scheme   Communication   Methods   and  Measures,  4,(1-­‐‑2),  7-­‐‑26.  

Coates,  J  (2004).  Women,  men  and  language.  Edinburgh,  UK,  Pearson  Longman.    

Cone,   J.,   &   Rand,   D.   G.   (2014).   Time   pressure   increases   cooperation   in  competitively  framed  social  dilemmas.  PLoS  ONE,  9(12),  13.  

Creswell,   J.  W.,   &   Clark,   V.   L.   P.   (2007).  Designing   and   conducting  mixed  methods  research.  Thousand  Oaks,  CA:  Sage  Publications,  Inc.    

Cuddy,  A.   J.  C.,  &   Fiske,   S.   T.   (2002).  Doddering   but   dear:   Process,   content,   and  function   in   stereotyping   of   older   persons.   In   T.   D.   Nelson   (Ed.),   Ageism:  Stereotyping   and   prejudice   against   older   persons;   ageism:   Stereotyping   and  prejudice  against  older  persons  (pp.  3-­‐‑26).  Cambridge,  MA:  The  MIT  Press.    

Cuddy,   A.   J.   C.,   Fiske,   S.   T.,   &   Glick,   P.   (2004).   When   professionals   become  mothers,  warmth  doesn'ʹt  cut  the  ice.  Journal  of  Social  Issues,  60(4),  701-­‐‑718.    

Cuddy,   A.   J.   C.,   Fiske,   S.   T.,   &   Glick,   P.   (2008).   Warmth   and   competence   as  universal  dimensions  of  social  perception:  The  stereotype  content  model  and  the  BIAS  map.  In  M.  P.  Zanna  (Ed.),  Advances  in  experimental  social  psychology,  vol   40;   advances   in   experimental   social   psychology,   vol   40  (pp.   61-­‐‑149).   San  Diego,  CA:  Elsevier  Academic  Press,.    

Cuddy,  A.  J.  C.,  Fiske,  S.  T.,  Kwan,  V.  S.  Y.,  Glick,  P.,  Demoulin,  S.,  Leyens,  J.,  .  .  .  Ziegler,   R.   (2009).   Stereotype   content   model   across   cultures:   Towards  universal  similarities  and  some  differences.  British  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  48(1),  1-­‐‑33.  

Page 64: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

52  

Curseu,  P.,  &  Schruijer,  S.  (2012).  Normative  interventions,  emergent  cognition  and  decision   rationality   in   ad   hoc   and   established   groups.  Management  Decision,  50(6),  1062-­‐‑1075.    

Curseu,  P.  L.,  Schruijer,  S.  G.  L.,  &  Fodor,  O.  C.  (2016).  Decision  rules,  escalation  of  commitment   and   sensitivity   to   framing   in   group   decision-­‐‑making:   An  experimental  investigation.  Management  Decision,  54(7),  1649-­‐‑1668.  

DeStephen,   R.S.,   &   Hirokawa,   R.Y.   (1998).   Small   group   consensus:   Stability   of  group  support  of   the  decision,   task  process,  and  group  relationships.  Small  Group  Behavior,  19,  227-­‐‑239.  

De  Dreu,  C.K.W.,  &  Weingart,  L.R.   (2003).  Task  versus  relationship  conflict,   team  performance   and   team   satisfaction:   A   meta-­‐‑analysis.   Journal   of   Applied  Psychology,  88,  741-­‐‑749.    

De   Dreu,   Carsten   K.   W.,   &   West,   M.   A.   (2001).   Minority   dissent   and   team  innovation:   The   importance   of   participation   in   decision  making.   Journal   of  Applied  Psychology,  86(6),  1191-­‐‑1201.  

Diekman,  A.  B.,  &  Eagly,  A.  H.  (2000).  Stereotypes  as  dynamic  constructs:  Women  and  men   of   the   past,   present,   and   future.   Personality   and   Social   Psychology  Bulletin,  26(10),  1171-­‐‑1188.  

Dovidio,   J.,   Hewstone,   M.,   Glick,   P.   &   Esses,   V.   (2010).   The   SAGE   Handbook   of  Prejudice,  Stereotyping  and  Discrimination.  London:  Sage  Publications.  

Duncan,  S,   Jr.,  &  Fiske,  D.W.   (1977).    Face-­‐‑to-­‐‑face   interaction:  Research,  methods,  and  theory.  Hilisdale,  NJ:  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates.  

Dubrovsky,  V.,  Kolla,  S.,  &  Sethna,  B.N.  (1991).  Effects  of  status  on  group  decision  making:   Ad   hoc   versus   real   groups.   Proceedings   of   the   Human   Factors   and  Ergonomics  Society  Annual  Meeting,    35,  (13),  p.  969  –  973.  

Durante,  F.,  Tablante,  C.  B.,  &  Fiske,  S.  T.  (2017).  Poor  but  warm,  rich  but  cold  (and  competent):   Social   classes   in   the   stereotype   content  model.  Journal   of   Social  Issues,  73(1),  138-­‐‑157.  

Eagly,  A.  H.,  &  Kite,  M.  E.  (1987).  Are  stereotypes  of  nationalities  applied  to  both  women  and  men?  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  53(3),  451-­‐‑462.  

Eagly,   A.  H.,   &   Sczesny,   S.   (2009).   Stereotypes   about  women,  men,   and   leaders:  Have  times  changed?  In  M.  Barreto,  M.  K.  Ryan  &  M.  T.  Schmitt  (Eds.),  The  glass   ceiling   in   the   21st   century:  Understanding   barriers   to   gender   equality;  (pp.  21-­‐‑47),  American  Psychological  Association.    

Eagly,  A.  H.,  Wood,  W.,  &  Johannesen-­‐‑Schmidt,  M.  (2004).  Social  role  theory  of  sex  differences   and   similarities:   Implications   for   the   partner   preferences   of  women  and  men.  In  A.  H.  Eagly,  A.  E.  Beall  &  R.  J.  Sternberg  (Eds.),  2nd  ed.;  

Page 65: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

53  

the  psychology  of   gender   (2nd   ed.)   (2nd   ed.   ed.,   pp.   269-­‐‑295),  New  York,  NY,  Guilford  Press.    

Edwards,  D.,  &  Potter,  J.  (1992).  Discursive  psychology.  London:  Sage.    

Erber,   M.W.,   Hodges,   S.D.,   &   Wilson,   T.D.   (1995).   Attitude   strength,   attitude  stability,  and  the  effects  of  analyzing  reasons.   In  R.E.  Petty  &  J.A.  Krosnick  (Eds).,  Attitude  strentgh:  Antecedents  and  consequences  (pp433-­‐‑454).  Mahwa,  NJ:  Erlbaum.    

Everly,  B.  A.,  Unzueta,  M.  M.,  &  Shih,  M.  J.  (2016).  Can  being  gay  provide  a  boost  in   the   hiring   process?  Maybe   if   the   boss   is   female.   Journal   of   Business   and  Psychology,  31(2),  293-­‐‑306.  

Fenwick,   G.D.,   &   Neal,   D.J.,   (2001).   Effect   of   gender   composition   on   group  performance.  Gender,  Work  and  Organization,  8  (2),  205-­‐‑225.  

Festinger,  L.  (1962).  Cognitive  dissonance.  Scientific  American,  207(4),  93-­‐‑106.  

Fingerhut,  A.  W.,  &  Peplau,  L.  A.  (2006).  The  impact  of  social  roles  on  stereotypes  of  gay  men.  Sex  Roles,  55(3-­‐‑4),  273-­‐‑278.  

Fiske,   S.   T.   (2015).   Intergroup   Biases:   A   focus   on   stereotype   content.  Current  Opinion  in  Behavioral  Sciences,  3,  45–50.    

Fiske,  A.P.,  Haslam,  N.,  &  Fiske,  S.T.   (1991).  Confusing  one  person  with  another:  What  errors  reveal  about  the  elementary  forms  of  social  relations.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  60,  656-­‐‑674.    

Fiske,   S.   T.,  Cuddy,  A.   J.  C.,  Glick,   P.,  &  Xu,   J.   (2002).  A  model   of   (often  mixed)  stereotype   content:   Competence   and   warmth   respectively   follow   from  perceived   status   and   competition.  Journal   of   Personality   and   Social  Psychology,  82(6),  878-­‐‑902.  

Fiske,   S.   T.,   Cuddy,   A.   J.   C.,   &   Glick,   P.   (2007).   Universal   dimensions   of   social  cognition:  Warmth  and  competence.  Trends  in  Cognitive  Sciences,  11(2),  77-­‐‑83.  

Fiske,   S.   T.,   Xu,   J.,   Cuddy,   A.   C.,   &   Glick,   P.   (1999).   (Dis)respecting   versus  (dis)liking:   Status   and   interdependence   predict   ambivalent   stereotypes   of  competence  and  warmth.  Journal  of  Social  Issues,  55(3),  473-­‐‑489.    

Fiske,  S.  T.  (2014).  Social  beings:  Core  motives  in  social  psychology.  Hoboken,  NJ,  John  Wiley  &  Sons.  

Fiske,  S.  T.  (2012).  The  continuum  model  and  the  stereotype  content  model.  In  P.  A.  M.  Van  Lange,  A.  W.  Kruglanski  &  E.  T.  Higgins  (Eds.),  Handbook  of  theories  of  social  psychology  (vol.  1);  (pp.  267-­‐‑288),  London,  Sage.  

Ford,   D.   L.,   Nemiroff,   P.   M.,   &   Pasmore,   W.   A.   (1977).   Group   decision-­‐‑making  performance   as   influenced   by   group   tradition.  Small   Group   Behavior,  8(2),  223-­‐‑228.  

Page 66: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

54  

Forsyth,   D.R.   (2006)  Group   Dynamics  4e   [International   Student   Edition].   Belmont  CA.:  Thomson  Wadsworth  Publishing.  

Fraïssé,   C.,   &   Barrientos,   J.   (2016).   The   concept   of   homophobia:   A   psychosocial  perspective.  Sexologies:  European  Journal  of  Sexology  and  Sexual  Health  /  Revue  Européenne  De  Sexologie  Et  De  Santé  Sexuelle,  25(4),  e65-­‐‑e69.    

Gavac,  S.,  Murrar,  S.,  &  Brauer,  M.  (2017).  Group  perception  and  social  norms.  In  R.  W.   Summers   (Ed.),  Social   psychology:  How   other   people   influence   our   thoughts  and  actions  (vol.  1)  (pp.  333-­‐‑359),  Santa  Barbara,  CA:  Greenwood  Press/ABC-­‐‑CLIO.    

Gelfand,   M.   J.,   Major,   V.   S.,   Raver,   J.   L.,   Nishii,   L.   H.,   &   O’Brien,   K.   (2006).  Negotiating  relationally:  The  dynamics  of  the  relational  self  in  negotiations.  Academy  of  Management  Review,  31,  427-­‐‑451.    

Gergen,  K.  J.  (2011).  Relational  being:  A  brief  introduction.  Journal  of  Constructivist  Psychology,  24(4),  280-­‐‑282.  

Gergen,   K.   J.   (2015).  An   invitation   to   social   construction   (3rd   ed.).  Thousand  Oaks,  CA:  Sage  Publishing  Ltd.  

Glick,   P.,   &   Fiske,   S.   T.   (2001).   An   ambivalent   alliance:   Hostile   and   benevolent  sexism   as   complementary   justifications   for   gender   inequality.   American  Psychologist,  56(2),  109-­‐‑118.  

Glick,  P.,  &  Fiske,  S.  T.  (2007).  Sex  discrimination:  The  psychological  approach.  In  F.   J.   Crosby,  M.   S.   Stockdale   &   S.   A.   Ropp   (Eds.),  Sex   discrimination   in   the  workplace:  Multidisciplinary  perspectives;  (pp.  155-­‐‑187),  Malden,  MA,  Blackwell  Publishing.  

Gorard,  S.  (2006).  Towards  a  judgement-­‐‑based  statistical  analysis.  British  Journal  of  Sociology  of  Education  27  (1):  67-­‐‑80.    

Gorard,  S.  (2007).  Mixing  methods  is  wrong:  An  everyday  approach  to  educational  justice.   Paper   presented   at   British   Educational   Research   Association.   5-­‐‑8  september,  2007,  London.    

Gouran,  D.S.  (1982).  Making  decisions  in  groups.  Glenview,  IL:  Scott,  Foresman  

Gouran,  D.  S.,  &  Hirokawa,  R.  Y.  (1996).  Functional  theory  and  communication  in  decision-­‐‑making  and  problem-­‐‑solving  groups:  An  expanded  view.   In  R.  Y.  Hirokawa  &  M.  S.  Poole,  Communication  and  group  decision  making   (2nd  ed.,  pp.  55–80).  Beverly  Hills,  CA:  Sage.  

Graziano,   W.   G.,   Jensen-­‐‑Campbell,   L.   A.,   &   Hair,   E.   C.   (1996).   Perceiving  interpersonal  conflict  and  reacting  to  it:  The  case  for  agreeableness.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  70,  820-­‐‑835.    

Page 67: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

55  

Gächter,   S.,   Orzen,   H.,   Renner,   E.,   &   Starmer,   C.   (2009).   Are   experimental  economists  prone   to   framing   effects?  A  natural   field   experiment.   Journal   of  Economic  Behavior  &  Organization,  70,  443-­‐‑446.  

Hackman   J.R   &   Vidmar   N.J.   (1970).   Effects   of   size   and   task   type   on   group  performance  and  member  reaction.  Sociometry,  33,  p.37-­‐‑54.  

Haslam,  S.  A.,  Turner,  J.  C.,  Oakes,  P.  J.,  Reynolds,  K.  J.,  &  Doosje,  B.  (2002).  From  personal   pictures   in   the   head   to   collective   tools   in   the  world:  How   shared  stereotypes   allow   groups   to   represent   and   change   social   reality.   In   C.  McGarty,   V.   Y.   Yzerbyt   &   R.   Spears   (Eds.),  Stereotypes   as   explanations:   The  formation  of  meaningful  beliefs  about  social  groups;  stereotypes  as  explanations:  The  formation   of   meaningful   beliefs   about   social   groups  (pp.   157-­‐‑185),   New   York,  NY,    Cambridge  University  Press.    

Hall,  J.,  &  Williams,  M.  S.  (1966).  A  comparison  of  decision-­‐‑making  performances  in   established   and   ad   hoc   groups.  Journal   of   Personality   and   Social  Psychology,  3(2),  214-­‐‑222.  

Hannagan,  R.J.  (2006).  Perceptual  asymmetry  in  gendered  group  decision  making..  ETD  collection  for  University  of  Nebraska  -­‐‑  Lincoln.  

Hannagan,   R.J.   &   Larimer,   C.W.   (2010).   Does   gender   composition   affect   group  decision  outcomes?  Evidence  from  a  laboratory  experiment.  Political  Behavior,  32,  51-­‐‑67.  

Hawkins,   K   &   Power,   S,   (1999).   Gender   differences   in   questions   asked   during  small   decision-­‐‑making   group   discussions   Small   Group   Research,30   (2),   235-­‐‑256.  

Heilman,  M.  E.,  &  Okimoto,  T.  G.  (2008).  Motherhood:  A  potential  source  of  bias  in  employment  decisions.  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  93(1),  189-­‐‑198.    

Hermann,  K.  (2015).  Field  theory  and  working  with  group  dynamics  in  debriefing.  Simulation  &  Gaming,  46(2),  209-­‐‑220.    

Hinsz,   V.B.,   &  Nickell,   G.S.   (2004).   Positive   reactions   to  working   in   groups   in   a  study  of  group  and  individual  goal  decision-­‐‑making.  Group  Dynamics  8,  253–264.    

Hirokawa,  R.Y.  (1988).  Group  communication  research:  Considerations  for  the  use  of  interaction  analysis.  In  C.H.  Tardy  (Ed.),  A  handbook  for  the  study  of  human  communication:  Methods  and  insruments  for  observing,  measureing,  and  assessing  communication  processes  (pp.229-­‐‑245).  Norwood,  NJ:  Ablex.    

Hogg,  M.  A.,  Sherman,  D.  K.,  Dierselhuis,   J.,  Maitner,  A.  T.,  &  Moffitt,  G.   (2007).  Uncertainty,   entitativity,   and   group   identification.  Journal   of   Experimental  Social  Psychology,  43(1),  135-­‐‑142.    

Page 68: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

56  

Hong,  L.,  &  Page,  S.  E.  (2004).  Groups  of  diverse  problem  solvers  can  outperform  groups  of  high-­‐‑ability  problem  solvers.  Proceedings  of  the  National  Academy  of  Sciences  of  the  United  States  of  America,  101(46),  16385-­‐‑16389.  

Howe,  L.  C.,  &  Krosnick,  J.  A.  (2017).  Attitude  strength.  Annual  Review  of  Psychology,  68,  327-­‐‑351.  

International   Labour   Office.   (2016).   Women   at   work:   Trends   2016,  http://www.ilo.org/gender/Informationresources/Publications/WCMS_457317/lang-­‐‑-­‐‑en/index.htm.  

Janis,  I.L.  (1972),  Groupthink:  Psychological  studies  of  policy  decisions  and  fiascos  (2nd  edn).  Boston,  MA,  Houghton  Mifflin.  

Jehn,  K.  A.   (1995).  A  multimethod  examination  of   the  benefits   and  detriments  of  intragroup  conflict.  Administrative  Science  Quarterly,  40(2),  256-­‐‑282.    

Jonsson,  G.,   Roempke,   S.  &   Zakrisson,   I.   (2003).   Att   undvika   oligarki   i   teori   och  praktik.   I   Kooperation,   ideellt   arebte   &   lokal   ekonomisk   utveckling.   Stockholm:  Fören.  Kooperativa  studier  (Kooperativ  årsbok).  S.  43-­‐‑72.  

Jost,   J.T.,   &   Major,   B.   (Eds.)   (2001).   The   psychology   of   legitimacy.   New   York:  Cambridge  University  Press.    

Kahai,  S.  S.,  Huang,  R.,  &   Jestice,  R.   J.   (2012).   Interaction  effect  of   leadership  and  communication   media   on   feedback   positivity   in   virtual   teams.  Group   &  Organization  Management,  37(6),  716-­‐‑751.    

Kameda,   T.,   Tsukasaki,   T.,   Hastie,   R.,   &   Berg,   N.   (2011).   Democracy   under  uncertainty:   The   wisdom   of   crowds   and   the   free-­‐‑rider   problem   in   group  decision  making.  Psychological  Review,  118(1),  76-­‐‑96.  

Kite,   M.   E.,   &   Whitley,   B.   E.,   Jr.   (2012).   Ethnic   and   nationality   stereotypes   in  everyday  language.  Teaching  of  Psychology,  39(1),  54-­‐‑56.  

Kong,  D.  T.,  Konczak,  L.   J.,  &  Bottom,  W.  P.   (2015).  Team  performance  as  a   joint  function   of   team   member   satisfaction   and   agreeableness.   Small   Group  Research,  46(2),  160-­‐‑178.  

Keyton,  J.,  &  Beck,  S.J.  (2009).  The  influential  role  of  relational  messages  in  group  interaction.  Group  Dynamics:  Theory,  Research  and  Practice,  13,  14-­‐‑30.  

Kolbe,   M.,     &   Boos,   M.   (2009).   Facilitating   Group   decision-­‐‑making:   Facilitator´s  subjective   theories  on  group  coordination.  Forum  Qualitative  Socialforschung  10,  (1).  Art.28.  

Kray,  L.  J.,  &  Thompson,  L.  (2005).  Gender  stereotypes  and  negotiation  performance:  An  examination  of  theory  and  research.  Research  in  organizational  behavior:  An  annual  series  of  analytical  essays  and  critical  reviews,  26,  103-­‐‑182.  

Page 69: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

57  

Knippendorff,  K.  (2005).    The  social  construction  of  public  opinion.  Kommunikation  über  kommunikation.  Theorie,  Methoden  und  Praxis.  Festschrift  für  Klaus  Merten,  129-­‐‑149.    

Kugelberg,  C.   (2006).  Constructing   the  deviant  other:  Mothering  and   fathering  at  the  workplace.  Gender,  Work  and  Organization,  13(2),  152-­‐‑173.  

Kurz,  T.,  &  Lyons,  A.   (2009).   Intergroup   influences  on   the   stereotype  consistency  bias   in   communication:   Does   it   matter   who   we   are   communicating   about  and  to  whom  we  are  communicating?  Social  Cognition,  27(6),  893-­‐‑904.  

Kühberger,   A.,   &   Tanner,   C.   (2010).   Risky   choice   framing:   Task   versions   and   a  comparison  of   prospect   theory   and   fuzzy-­‐‑trace   theory.   Journal   of  Behavioral  Decision  Making,  23(3),  314-­‐‑329.  

Lau,  R.  R.  (2003).  Models  of  decision-­‐‑making.  In  D.  O.  Sears,  L.  Huddy  &  R.  Jervis  (Eds.),  Oxford   handbook   of   political   psychology;   oxford   handbook   of   political  psychology,  (pp.  19-­‐‑59),  New  York,  Oxford  University  Press.  

Larson,  J.  R.,   Jr.,  Christensen,  C.,  Franz,  T.  M.,  &  Abbott,  A.  S.   (1998).  Diagnosing  groups:  The  pooling,  management,  and  impact  of  shared  and  unshared  case  information   in   team-­‐‑based   medical   decision   making.   Journal   of   Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  75(1),  93-­‐‑108.  

Latu,   l.,   &   Schmid   Mast,   M.   (2016).   The   effects   of   stereotypes   of   women'ʹs  performance   in   male-­‐‑dominated   hierarchies:   Stereotype   threat   activation  and   reduction   through   role   models.   In   K.   Faniko,   F.   Lorenzi-­‐‑Cioldi,   O.  Sarrasin   &   E.   Mayor   (Eds.),   Gender   and   social   hierarchies:   Perspectives   from  social  psychology;  (pp.  75-­‐‑87).  New  York:  Routledge/Taylor  &  Francis  Group.    

Lawson,   T.   J.,  McDonough,   T.   A.,   &   Bodle,   J.   H.   (2010).   Confronting   prejudiced  comments:   Effectiveness   of   a   role-­‐‑playing   exercise.  Teaching   of  Psychology,  37(4),  257-­‐‑261.      

Lee,  T.  L.,  &  Fiske,  S.  T.  (2006).  Not  an  outgroup,  not  yet  an  ingroup:  Immigrants  in  the   stereotype   content  model.   International   Journal   of   Intercultural   Relations,  30(6),  751-­‐‑768.  

Lennéer-­‐‑Axelson,   B.   &   Thylefors,   I.   (2018).  Arbetsgruppens   psykologi.   (5.,   [omarb.]  utg.)  Stockholm:  Natur  och  kultur.  

Leong,  L.  M.,  McKenzie,  C.  R.  M.,  Sher,  S.,  &  Müller-­‐‑Trede,   J.   (2017).  The   role  of  inference  in  attribute  framing  effects.  Journal  of  Behavioral  Decision  Making.  30,  1147-­‐‑1156.  

Levin,   I.   P.,   Johnson,   R.   D.,   &   Davis,   M.   L.   (1987).   How   information   frame  influences   risky   decisions:   Between-­‐‑subjects   and   within-­‐‑subject  comparisons.  Journal  of  Economic  Psychology,  8,  43–54.  

Lewin,  K.,  &  Lippit,  R.  (1938).  An  experimental  approach  to  the  study  of  autocracy  and  democracy:  a  preliminary  study.  Sociometry,  1,  292–300.  

Page 70: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

58  

Lewin,   K.,   Lippit,   R.   &   White,   R.K.   (1939).   Patterns   of   aggressive   behavior   in  experimentally   created   social   climates.   Journal   of   Social   Psychology,   10,   271-­‐‑301.  

Lewin,   K.   (1951).   Field   theory   in   social   science:   Selected   theoretical   papers   (edited   by  Dowrin  Cartwright),  Oxford,    Harper  &  Row.    

Lewin,   K.   (1947).   Frontiers   in   group   dynamics;   concept,   method   and   reality   in  social  science;  Social  equilibria  and  Social  change.  Human  Relations,  1,  5-­‐‑41.  

Lindsey,   E.   W.   (2012).   In   McGeown   S.   P.   (Ed.),  Girl'ʹs   and   boy'ʹs   play   form  preferences   and   gender   segregation   in   early   childhood,   Hauppauge,   NY,  Nova  Science  Publishers.  

Lippit,   R.   (1940).   An   experimental   study   of   the   effect   of   democratic   and  authoritarian  group  atmospheres.  University  of  Iowa  Studies   in  Child  Welfare,  16,  43–195.  

Lira  E.  M.,  Ripoll  P.,  Peiró  J.  M.,  Zornoza  A.  M.  (2008).  The  role  of  information  and  communication  technologies  in  the  relationship  between  group  effectiveness  and  group  potency.  Small  Group  Research,  39,  728-­‐‑74.  

Lovaglia,   M.,   Mannix,   E.A.,   Samuelson,   C.D.,   Sell,   J.,     &   Wilsson,   R.K.   (2005)  Conflict,   power   and   status   in   groups.   In  M.S.   Poole,   &  A.B.  Hollnigshead  (eds)   Theories   of   small   groups:   interdisciplinary   perspectives.   Thousand   Oaks,  CA,  Sage.  

Lyons,   A.,   &   Kashima,   Y.   (2003).   How   are   stereotypes   maintained   through  communication?  the  influence  of  stereotype  sharedness.  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology:  Attitudes  and  Social  Cognition,  85(6),  989-­‐‑1005.    

Maio,   G.,   &   Haddock,   G.   (2014).   The   psychology   of   attitudes   and   attitude   change.  London,  Sage.  

Mason,   C.   M.,   &   Griffin,   M.A.   (2002).   Group   task   satisfaction:   Applying   the  construct  of  job  satisfaction  to  groups.  Small  Group  Research,  22,  271-­‐‑312.  

Mason.  C.M.,&    Griffin,  M.A.   (2003).   Identifying  group   task   satisfaction   at  work.  Small  Group  Research  34,  413-­‐‑435.  

Macrae,  C.  N.,  Hewstone,  M.,  &  Griffiths,  R.  J.  (1993).  Processing  load  and  memory  for  stereotype-­‐‑based  information.  European  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  23(1),  77-­‐‑87.  

McGregor,   J.   (1993).   Effectiveness   of   role   playing   and   antiracist   teaching   in  reducing   student   prejudice.  The   Journal   of   Educational   Research,  86(4),   215-­‐‑226.    

Page 71: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

59  

McNeill,   A.,   &   Burton,   A.   M.   (2002).   The   locus   of   semantic   priming   effects   in  person  recognition.  The  Quarterly  Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology  A:  Human  Experimental  Psychology,  55a(4),  1141-­‐‑1156.  

Meyer,  M.,  &  Gelman,   S.  A.   (2016).  Gender   essentialism   in   children   and  parents:  Implications  for  the  development  of  gender  stereotyping  and  gender-­‐‑typed  preferences.  Sex  Roles,  75(9-­‐‑10),  409-­‐‑421.  

Meyers,   R.A.,   Brashers,   D.E.,   Winston,   L   &   Grob,   L   (1997)   Sex   differences   and  group   argument:   A   theoretical   framework   and   empirical   investigation.  Communication  Studies,  48,  19-­‐‑41.  

Meyers,   R.A.,   &   Brashers,   D.E.   (2010).   Extending   the   conversational   argument  coding   scheme:   Argument   categories,   units   and   coding   procedures.  Communication  Methods  and  Measures,  4,  27-­‐‑45.  

Meyers,   R.A.,   Brashers,   D.E.,   &   Hanner,   J.   (2000).   Majority-­‐‑minority   influence:  Identifying  argumentative  patterns  and  predicting  argument-­‐‑outcome  links.  Journal  of  Communication,  50(4),  3-­‐‑30.  

Myers,  D.G.;  H.  Lamm  (1976).  The  group  polarization  phenomenon.  Psychological  Bulletin,  83,  602–627.  

Michie,  S.  &  Williams,  S.  (2003).  Reducing  work  related  psychological  ill  health  and  sickness   absence:   a   systematic   literature   review.   Occupational   &    Environmental  Medicine.  60(1),  3-­‐‑9.    

Michels  (1911/1983).  Organisationer  och  demokrati:  en  sociologisk  studie  av  de  oligarkiska  tendenserna  i  vår  tid.  (Political  parties.  A  sociological  study  of  the  oligarchical  tendencies  of  modern  democracy).  Stockholm:  Ratio.    

Milgram,  S.   (1974).  Obedience  to  authority:  An  experimental  view.  New  York:  Harper  &  Row.  

Moyer,  K.  H.,  &  Gross,  A.   (2011).  Group  designs.   In   J.  C.   Thomas,  &  M.  Hersen  (Eds.),  Understanding  research  in  clinical  and  counseling  psychology  (2nd  ed.)  (pp.  155-­‐‑179),  New  York,    Routledge/Taylor  &  Francis  Group.  

Nam,   C.   S.,   Lyons,   J.   B.,   Hwang,   H.,   &   Kim,   S.   (2009).   The   process   of   team  communication   in  multi-­‐‑cultural   contexts:  An   empirical   study  using   bales’  interaction   process   analysis   (IPA).   International   Journal   of   Industrial  Ergonomics,  39(5),  771-­‐‑782.  

Nawata,  K.,  &  Yamaguchi,  H.   (2011).   The   escalation  of  personal   altercations   into  intergroup   conflict:   Initiating   intergroup  vicarious   retribution   in   an  ad  hoc  laboratory   experiment   group.  Japanese   Journal   of   Experimental   Social  Psychology,  51(1),  52-­‐‑63.    

Page 72: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

60  

Nelson,  N.,  Bronstein,   I.,  Shacham,  R.,  &  Ben-­‐‑Ari,  R.   (2015).  The  power   to  oblige:  Power,   gender,   negotiation   behaviors,   and   their   consequences.   Negotiation  and  Conflict  Management  Research,  8(1),  1-­‐‑24.  

Nemeth,  C.  J.,  &  Goncalo,  J.  A.  (2005).  Influence  and  persuasion  in  small  groups.  In  T.   C.   Brock,   &   M.   C.   Green   (Eds.),  Persuasion:   Psychological   insights   and  perspectives   (2nd   ed.)  (pp.   171-­‐‑194),   Thousand  Oaks,   CA,   Sage   Publications,  Inc.  

Nemeth,  C.   J.,  &  Nemeth-­‐‑Brown,  B.   (2003).  Better   than   individuals?   the  potential  benefits  of  dissent  and  diversity  for  group  creativity.  In  P.  B.  Paulus,  &  B.  A.  Nijstad   (Eds.),   Group   creativity:   Innovation   through   collaboration;   group  creativity:   Innovation   through   collaboration   (pp.   63-­‐‑84),   New   York:   Oxford  University  Press.  

Okimoto,   T.   G.,   &   Heilman,   M.   E.   (2012).   The   “bad   parent”   assumption:   How  gender   stereotypes   affect   reactions   to   working   mothers.   Journal   of   Social  Issues,  68(4),  704-­‐‑724.  

Ostrom,   E.   (2003).   Toward   a   behavioral   theory   linking   trust,   reciprocity,   and  reputation.   IN   E.Osterom   &   J.Walker   (Eds.),   Trust   and   reciprocity:  Interdisciplinary   lessons   from   experimental   research.   (pp.   19-­‐‑79).   New   York,    Russell  Sage  Foundation.  

Peplau,  L.  A.,  &  Fingerhut,  A.  (2004).  The  paradox  of  the  lesbian  worker.  Journal  of  Social  Issues,  60(4),  719-­‐‑735.  

Peters,  W.   (1987).   A   Class   Divided:   Then   and   now:   1st   Edition.   Yale   University  Press.  

Petersson,   S.,   Catásus,   H.,   &   Danielsson,   E.   (2016).   Vem   håller   i   klubban?   Om  demokrati   och   delaktighet   i   idrottsföreningar,   Stockholm,   Centrum   för  idrottsforskning,  1,    1-­‐‑52.  

Peterson,  R.,  Owens,  P.,  Tetlock,  P.E.,  Fan,  E.  &  Martorana.  (1998).  Group  dynamics  in  top  management  teams:  Groupthink,  vigilance,  and  alternative  models  of  failure   and   success   in   organizations.   Organizational   Behavior   and   Human  Decision  Processes,  73,  77-­‐‑99.  

Poole,  M.  S.,  &  Folger,  J.  P.  (1981).  A  method  for  establishing  the  representational  validity   of   interaction   coding   systems:   Do   we   see   what   they   see.  Human  Communication  Research,  8,  26-­‐‑42.  

Potter,   J.   (1988).   Discursive   social   psychology:   From   attitudes   to   evaluative  practices.   In   W.   Stroebe   &   M.   Hewstone   (Eds.),   European   review   of   social  psychology  (Vol.9,  pp.  233-­‐‑266).  Chichester,  UK:  Wiley.  

Page 73: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

61  

Potter,  J.,  &  Edwards,  D.  (2001).  Discursive  social  psychology.  In  W.  P.  Robinson  &  H.  Giles   (Eds.),  The  new  handbook   of   language   and   social   psychology   (pp.   103–  118).  London:  John  Wiley  &  Sons  Ltd.  

Potter,  J.,  &  Wetherell,  M.  (1987).  Discourse  and  social  psychology:  Beyond  attitudes  and  behaviour.  London:  Sage.  

Pratto  &  Walker.  (2004).  The  bases  of  gendered  power.    In  A.H.  Eagly,  A.E.  Beall  &  R.J.   Sternberg   (Eds.),   2nd   ed,   the   psychology   of   gender   (2nd   ed).   (pp.   242-­‐‑268).  New  York:  Guilford.)  

Pratto,   F.,   Sidanius,   J.,   &   Levin,   S.   (2006).   Social   dominance   theory   and   the  dynamics   of   intergroup   relations:   Taking   stock   and   looking   forward.  European  Review  of  Social  Psychology,  17,  271-­‐‑320.  

Raghubir,   P.   &   Valenzuela,   A.   (2010).   Male-­‐‑female   dynamics   in   groups:   A   field  study  of  The  Weakest  Link.  Small  Group  Research,  41  (1),  41-­‐‑70.  

Ramsay,  J.  E.,  &  Pang,  J.  S.   (2017).  Anti-­‐‑immigrant  prejudice  in  rising  east  asia:  A  stereotype   content   and   integrated   threat   analysis.  Political  Psychology,  38(2),  227-­‐‑244.    

Rees,  D.  M.   (2014).  Framing   group   decisions:  How   emphasizing   deadline   vs.   teamwork  influences  group  decision  making.  (Doctoral  dissertation,  The  Chicago  School  of  Professional  psychology,  Ann  Arbor,  MI.    

Ridgeway,  C.  L.  (2011).  Framed  by  gender:  How  gender  inequality  persists  in  the  modern  world.  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press.  

Rijnbout,   J.   S.,   &  McKimmie,   B.   M.   (2012).   Deviance   in   group   decision   making:  Group-­‐‑member   centrality   alleviates   negative   consequences   for   the   group.  European  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  42(7),  915-­‐‑923.  

Riksidrottsförbundet.  (2011).  Svenska  folkets  idrotts-­‐‑  och  motionsvanor.  Stockholm:    http://www.rf.se/globalassets/riksidrottsforbundet/dokument/motionsidrott/svenska-­‐‑folkets-­‐‑motionsvanor-­‐‑2011.pdf.  

Rode,   J.   (2010).  Truth  and  trust   in  communication:  Experiments  on   the  effect  of  a  competitive  context.  Games  and  Economic  Behavior,  68(1),  325-­‐‑338.  

Russell,  A.  M.  T.,  &  Fiske,  S.  T.  (2008).  It'ʹs  all  relative:  Competition  and  status  drive  interpersonal   perception.  European   Journal   of   Social   Psychology,  38(7),   1193-­‐‑1201.  

Russell,  N.,  &  Picard,   J.   (2013).  Review  of  behind   the   shock  machine:  The  untold  story   of   the   notorious   Milgram   psychology   experiments.  Journal   of   the  History  of  the  Behavioral  Sciences,  49(2),  221-­‐‑223.    

Schegloff,   E.  A.   (2000).  Overlapping   talk   and   the   organization   of   turn-­‐‑taking   for  conversation.  Language  in  Society,  29(1),  1-­‐‑63.  

Page 74: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

62  

Schultz,  J.M.,  Sjøvold,  E.,  &  Andre,  B.,  (2017)  Can  group  climate  explain  innovative  readiness   for   change?   Journal   of   Organizational   Change   Management.     30(3),  440-­‐‑452.  

Schmid   Mast,   M.   (2001).   Gender   differences   and   similarities   in   dominance  hierarchies   in   same-­‐‑gender   groups   based   on   speaking   time.   Sex   Roles,   44,  537–556.    

Schmid  Mast,  M.  (2004).  Men  are  hierarchical,  women  are  egalitarian:  An  implicit  gender  stereotype.  Swiss  Journal  of  Psychology,  63,  107–111.    

Schweiger,   D.M.,   Sandberg,   W.R.,   &   Ragan,   J.W.   (1986).   Group   approaches   for  improving   strategic  decision  making:  A   comparative   analysis   of  dialectical  inquiry,  devil´s  advocacy,  and  consensus.  Academy  of  Management  Journal,  32,  745-­‐‑772.    

Seibold,  D.R.,  &  Meyers,  R.A.,  (2007)  Group  argument:  A  Structuration  perspective  and  research  program.  Small  Group  Research,  38,  312-­‐‑336.  

Sheridan,   A.,   Ross-­‐‑Smith,   A.,   &   Lord,   L.   (2015).  Women   on   boards   in   Australia:  Achieving  real   change  or  more  of   the  same?   In  A.  M.  Broadbridge,  &  S.  L.  Fielden   (Eds.),  Handbook  of  gendered  careers   in  management:  Getting   in,  getting  on,  getting  out,  (pp.  322-­‐‑340)  Northampton,  MA:  Edward  Elgar  Publishing.  

Sherif,  M.  (1936),  The  psychology  of  social  norms.  New  York:  Harper.  Sidanius,   J.,   &   Pratto,   F.   (1999).   Social   dominance:   An   intergroup   theory   of   social  

hierarchy  and  oppression.  New  York:  Cambridge  University  Press.  Simons   T,   Pelled   L,   Smith  K.   (1999)  Making   use   of   difference:  Diversity,   debate,  

and   decision   comprehensiveness   in   top   management   teams.   Academy   of  Management  Journal.  42,  662-­‐‑673.  

Sinaceur,  M.,   Thomas-­‐‑Hunt,  M.,  Neale,  M.  A.,  O'ʹNeill,  O.  A.,  &  Haag,  C.   (2010).  Accuracy   and   perceived   expert   status   in   group   decisions:   When   minority  members  make  majority  members  more   accurate   privately.   Personality   and  Social  Psychology  Bulletin,  36(3),  423-­‐‑437.  

Slotegraaf,  R.J.,  &  Atuahene-­‐‑Gima,  K.   (2011).  Product  development   team  stability  and  new  product  advantage:  The  role  of  decision-­‐‑making  processes.  Journal  of  Marketing,  75,  96-­‐‑108.    

Smith,   R.A.   (2002).     Race,   gender,   and   authority   in   the   workplace:   theory   and  tesearch.”  Annual  Review  of  Sociology  28(1),  509-­‐‑542.    

Spector,   P.   E.   (1988),   Development   of   the   work   locus   of   control   scale.   Journal   of  Occupational  Psychology,  61:  335–340.    

Statistiska   centralbyrån   (2016).  På   tal   om   kvinnor   och  män:   lathund   om   jämställdhet.  Örebro:  SCB.  

Page 75: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

63  

Statistiska   centralbyrån.   (2017).   Undersökningarna   av   barns   levnadsförhållanden.  Stockholm:  https://www.scb.se/.  

Summers,  R.W.,  (2017)  Social  psychology:  How  other  people  influence  our  thoughts  and  actions  (2017).  Santa  Barbara,  CA:  Greenwood  Press/ABC-­‐‑CLIO.  

Tasa,  K.,  &  Whyte,  G.   (2005).  Collective   efficacy   and  vigilant   problem   solving   in  group   decision   making:   A   non-­‐‑linear   model.   Organizational   Behavior   and  Human  Decision  Processes,  96,  119-­‐‑129.  

Tajfel,   H.   (1981).  Human   groups   and   social   categories.   Cambridge:   Cambridge  University  Press.  

Thompson,   M.   S.,   Judd,   C.   M.,   &   Park,   B.   (2000).   The   consequences   of  communicating   social   stereotypes.   Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology,  36(6),  567-­‐‑599.  

Toma,  C.,  Bry,  C.,  &  Butera,  F.   (2013).  Because  I’m  worth  it!   (more  than  others...):  Cooperation,   competition,   and   ownership   bias   in   group   decision-­‐‑making.  Social  Psychology,  44(4),  248-­‐‑255.  

Toma,   C.,   &   Butera,   F.   (2015).   Cooperation   versus   competition   effects   on  information  sharing  and  use  in  group  decision-­‐‑making.  Social  and  Personality  Psychology  Compass,  9(9),  455-­‐‑467.  

Toma,   C.,   Vasiljevic,   D.,   Oberlé,   D.,   &   Butera,   F.   (2013).   Assigned   experts   with  competitive   goals   withhold   information   in   group   decision   making.   British  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  52(1),  161-­‐‑172.  

Triplett,   N.   (1898).   The   dynamogenic   factors   in   pacemaking   and  competition.  American  Journal  of  Psychology,  9,  507-­‐‑533.  

Tubbs,  S.L.  (2007).  A  systems  Approach  to  small  Group  interaction,  (9th  ed.).  New  York:  McGraw-­‐‑Hill.  

Tubbs,  S.,  Moss,  S.  &  Papastefanou,  N.  (2011).  Human  Communication  Principles  and  Contexts.  Berkshire,  UK:  McGraw-­‐‑Hill  Education.  

Tversky,  A.  &  Kahneman,  D.  (1981).  The  framing  of  decisions  and  the  psychology  of  choice.  Science,  211,  453–458.  

van  Knippenberg,   B.,   van  Knippenberg,  D.,  &  Wilke,  H.  A.   (2001).   Power  use   in  cooperative   and   competitive   settings.   Basic   and   Applied   Social   Psychology,  23(4),  291-­‐‑300.  

Van   Vugt,   M.,   De   Cremer,   D.,   &   Janssen,   D.   P.   (2007).   Gender   differences   in  cooperation   and   competition:   The   male-­‐‑warrior   hypothesis.  Psychological  Science,  18(1),  19-­‐‑23.  

Page 76: COMMUNICATING, NEGOTIATING, AND STEREOTYPING: THE …1183806/... · 2018. 2. 20. · SMALL GROUP DECISION-MAKING Pär Löfstrand Main supervisor: Ingrid Zakrisson Co-supervisors:

64  

Vogel,   J.,   Amnå.,   E.,   Munk,   I.,   &   Häll,   L.   (2003).   Associational   life   in   Sweden:  General   welfare   production,   social   capital,   training   in   democracy,   living  condition  (Report  No.  98).  Stockholm,  Sweden:  SCB  (Statistics  Sweden).  

von   Hippel,   C.,  Wiryakusuma,   C.,   Bowden,   J.,   &   Shochet,   M.   (2011).   Stereotype  threat   and   female   communication   styles.   Personality   and   Social   Psychology  Bulletin,  37(10),  1312-­‐‑1324.  

Van  Swol,  L.M.  (2009).  Extreme  members  and  group  polarization.  Social  Influence,  4  (3),  185–199.  

van   Woerkom,   M.,   &   Sanders,   K.   (2010).   The   romance   of   learning   from  disagreement.   The   effect   of   cohesiveness   and   disagreement   on   knowledge  sharing   behavior   and   individual   performance   within   teams.   Journal   of  Business  and  Psychology,  25(1),  139-­‐‑149.  

Wheelan,   S.A.   (2009).  Group   size,   group   development,   and   group   productivity  Small  Group  Research,    40,  2,    247  –  262.    

Wheelan,   S.   A.,   Murphy,   D.,   Tsumura,   E.,   &   Fried-­‐‑Kline,   S.   (1998).   Member  perceptions   of   internal   group   dynamics   and   productivity.   Small   Group  Research,  29,  371-­‐‑  393.    

Wheelan,   S.A.  &  Davidson,   B  &  Tilin,   F   (2003).  Group  development   across   time.  reality  or  illusion?  Small  Group  Research,  34,  2,  223-­‐‑245.    

Wheelan,  S.A.  (2005).  Creating  Effective  Teams.  A  guide  for  members  and  leaders.  Thousand  Oaks,  CA:  Sage  Publications.    

Wolgast,   S.   (2017).  How   does   the   job   applicants’   ethnicity   affect   the   selection   process?  Norms,  Preferred  competencies  and  expected  fit,  (doctoral  thesis),  Lund.  

Yi,   R.   (2003).   A   modified   tit-­‐‑for-­‐‑tat   strategy   in   a   5-­‐‑person   prisoner'ʹs   dilemma,  Unpublished  doctoral  dissertation,  New  York  State  University.    

Zakrisson,   I.   (2008).   Gender   differences   in   social   dominance   orientation:   Gender  invariance  may  be  situation  invariance.  Sex  Roles:  A  Journal  of  Research,  59(3-­‐‑4),  254-­‐‑263.    

Zimbardo,   P.   G.   (1973).   On   the   ethics   of   intervention   in   human   psychological  research:   With   special   reference   to   the   stanford   prison  experiment.  Cognition,  2(2),  243-­‐‑256.    

Zimbardo,  Philip  (2007).  The  Lucifer  Effect.  New  York:  The  Random  House.    World  Economic  Forum.  (2015).  Ten  years  of  the  global  gender  gap.  Geneva:  Report    Highlights  (2015).  http://reports.weforum.org/global-­‐‑gap-­‐‑report-­‐‑2015/.