Clarifying the NAP effect and the role of dispositional ...

64
Clarifying the NAP effect and the role of dispositional factors: Behavioural and electrophysiological investigations by Regard Martijn Booy B.A. Hons., Simon Fraser University, 2013 Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts in the Department of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences Regard Martijn Booy 2016 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY Summer 2016

Transcript of Clarifying the NAP effect and the role of dispositional ...

Clarifying the NAP effect and the role of

dispositional factors: Behavioural and

electrophysiological investigations

by

Regard Martijn Booy

B.A. Hons., Simon Fraser University, 2013

Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

Master of Arts

in the

Department of Psychology

Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences

Regard Martijn Booy 2016

SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY

Summer 2016

ii

Approval

Name: Regard Martijn Booy

Degree: Master of Arts

Title: Clarifying the NAP effect and the role of dispositional factors: Behavioural and electrophysiological investigations.

Examining Committee:

Mario Liotti Senior Supervisor Professor

Thomas Spalek Supervisor Associate Professor

Rebecca Todd External Examiner Assistant Professor Department of Psychology University of British Columbia

Date Defended/Approved: May 05, 2016

iii

Ethics Statement

iv

Abstract

Researchers using the negative affective priming (NAP) paradigm favour a dispositional

argument for the negative ruminatory cycle in depression despite some evidence

supporting an affect-based explanation. Additionally, the cognitive mechanisms

underlying the task are poorly understood. To examine these questions directly, a modified

version of the NAP task, that dissociates the priming effects of positive and negative

words, was implemented in two experiments. The first experiment tests the contribution of

disposition (as measured by the NEO-PI-R depression subscale) versus symptom severity

(as measured by the BDI-II) to NAP scores. Compared to low BDI-II scorers, high BDI-II

scorers showed a larger NAP effect for negative words, and a smaller NAP effect for

positive words. However, there were no significant differences between high and low

scorers on the NEO-PI-R depression subscale. This suggests that current severity of

symptoms and not dispositional factors influence the NAP effect more strongly. The

second experiment used event related potentials (ERPs) to determine the cognitive

processes involved in the NAP task prior to a participant’s response. The results showed

that prior presentation of irrelevant emotion words significantly affected the brain response

to subsequent task-relevant emotion words. Thus, on ignored repetition (IgnRep) trials

positive voltage modulations were observed both early, over frontal scalp (between 190-

260ms) and late, primarily over posterior scalp (between 500-700ms). Interestingly,

IgnRep trials for negative words were associated with the early frontal effects, suggesting

an early, implicit attentional bias to negative material, while IgnRep trials for positive words

were associated with the later positivity, indicating more conscious processing of positive

material. Results are discussed in terms of current theories of the processes involved in

the NAP effect.

Keywords: Negative Affective Priming; Dysphoria; Event Related Potentials; BDI-II; Late Positive Potential; Early Anterior Positivity

v

Dedication

To my parents who taught me the value of critical

thinking and hard work. To my wife who embodies

the virtues of compassion and sincerity. And to my

brother who inspires loyalty and creativity.

vi

Acknowledgements

So many people have contributed to my educational progress that it would be

impossible to list them all. But, anyone who reads this has had a hand in bringing me this

far. Please accept my heartfelt thank you!

Specifically, I’d like to thank the member of the Laboratory for Affective and

Developmental Neuroscience who have supported me in innumerable ways. To Patrick

Carolan and Killian Kleffner-Canucci who have done everything from teaching me

software, to helping me interpret results. Your guidance and expertise is very much

appreciated. On a similar note, I’d like to thank Adam Baker, whose innovative ideas and

hard-working attitude inspired me into some of the most rewarding directions of this thesis.

But these projects could not have been completed without the tedious legwork of the

research assistants who worked diligently and conscientiously to collect quality data. So

to all the RAs, thank you!

Lastly, I’d like to thank my supervisors. Dr. Mario Liotti, whose continued

supervision over the last several years has enabled me to grow as a researcher, but who

trusted me enough to pursue my own avenues of interest. And Dr. Tom Spalek, who has

patiently explained concepts, written innumerable reference letters, and whose

suggestions has guided my research into new directions. I’d like to thank you both for all

the time you’ve invested in me over the years. But more importantly, thank you for always

being there to support and guide me when I’m in over my head.

vii

Table of Contents

Approval .......................................................................................................................... ii Ethics Statement ............................................................................................................ iii Abstract .......................................................................................................................... iv Dedication ....................................................................................................................... v Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................ vi Table of Contents .......................................................................................................... vii List of Tables .................................................................................................................. ix List of Figures................................................................................................................. ix List of Acronyms & Abbreviations .................................................................................... x

Chapter 1. General Introduction ............................................................................... 1 1.1. Cognitive Bias in Depression .................................................................................. 1 1.2. Rumination in Depression ....................................................................................... 3

1.2.1. Dispositional Argument: Inhibition and WM ................................................ 4 1.2.2. Emotion Regulation Argument: Sad Mood and Depression ....................... 5

1.3. The current study .................................................................................................... 8

Chapter 2. Experiment 1 .......................................................................................... 10 2.1. Introduction ........................................................................................................... 10 2.2. Methods ............................................................................................................... 12

2.2.1. Participants .............................................................................................. 12 2.2.2. Procedure ................................................................................................ 12 2.2.3. Design ..................................................................................................... 13 2.2.4. Variables ................................................................................................. 15

NEO-PI-R ............................................................................................................... 15 BDI-II ...................................................................................................................... 15

2.2.5. Statistical Analysis ................................................................................... 16 2.3. Results ................................................................................................................. 18

2.3.1. Accuracy.................................................................................................. 18 2.3.2. Overall NAP effects ................................................................................. 18 2.3.3. Effects of ‘state’ depression on NAP effect .............................................. 19 2.3.4. Effects of ‘’trait’ depression on NAP effect ............................................... 20

2.4. Discussion ............................................................................................................ 21

Chapter 3. Experiment 2 .......................................................................................... 23 3.1. Introduction ........................................................................................................... 23 3.2. Methods ............................................................................................................... 26

3.2.1. Participants .............................................................................................. 26 3.2.2. Procedure and Design ............................................................................. 26 3.2.3. Electrophysiological Recording ................................................................ 27 3.2.4. Statistical Analysis ................................................................................... 28

Behavioural ............................................................................................................ 28 ERP ........................................................................................................................ 29

3.3. Results ................................................................................................................. 30

viii

3.3.1. Behavioural ............................................................................................. 30 Accuracy ................................................................................................................ 30 NAP ........................................................................................................................ 30

3.3.2. ERP ......................................................................................................... 31 EAP (190-260ms) ................................................................................................... 31 LPP (500-700ms) ................................................................................................... 32

3.4. Discussion ............................................................................................................ 36 3.4.1. EAP ......................................................................................................... 37 3.4.2. LPP ......................................................................................................... 38 3.4.3. Behavioural ............................................................................................. 39

Chapter 4. General Discussion ............................................................................... 41 4.1. Implications .......................................................................................................... 41 4.2. Limitations and Future Research .......................................................................... 43

References 46 Appendix A. Questionnaires................................................................................... 50

Medical Questionnaire .......................................................................................... 50 NEO-PI-R ............................................................................................................. 51

Appendix B. Word Lists .......................................................................................... 53

ix

List of Tables

Table 1: The modified NAP task. .......................................................................... 13

Table 2: Demographics ........................................................................................ 16

Table 3: Accuracy ................................................................................................ 18

Table 4: Mean # of Trials Retained ...................................................................... 22

List of Figures

Figure 1: NAP Effect ............................................................................................. 20

Figure 2: ERPs to Word Valences ......................................................................... 33

Figure 3: ERPs to Negative Words ........................................................................ 34

Figure 4: ERPs to Positive Words ......................................................................... 35

Figure 5: Topographic Maps ................................................................................. 36

x

List of Acronyms & Abbreviations

ACC Accuracy

ANEW Affective Norms for English Words

ANOVA Analysis of Variance

AttRep Attended Repetition

BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory (second edition)

CES-D Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale

DIA Distractor Inhibition Account

EAP Early Anterior Positivity

EEG Electroencephalogram

EPN Early Posterior Negativity

ERA Episodic Retrieval Account

ERP Event-related Potential

HBDI High BDI

HD High Depression

IgnRep Ignored Repetition

ISI Inter-stimulus Interval

LBDI Low BDI

LD Low Depression

LPC Late Positive Complex

LPP Late Positive Potential

NAP Negative Affective Priming

NEO-PI-R NEO Personality Inventory Revised

NEO-PI-R E6 NEO Personality Inventory Revised Extraversion 6

NEO-PI-R N3 NEO Personality Inventory Revised Neuroticism 3

NFC Need for Cognition

NP Negative Priming

PD Prime Distractor

PP Positive Priming

RSI Response Stimulus Interval

RT Reaction Time

WM Working Memory

1

Chapter 1.

General Introduction

Whether depression revolves primarily around negative affect resulting in

cognitive changes, or a negative thought patterns leading to a negative mood state

remains undetermined. Depression is often described as a cognitive disorder

characterized by negative schemas (Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan, & De Raedt,

2011) which causes the altered patterns in both memory and attention (Gotlib &

Joormann, 2010). Under this conception, negative thought patterns, contribute to the

characteristic sad mood of the disorder. For example, when receiving a poor grade on

an assignment a person might think “I am terrible at this” causing them to feel

negatively, especially about themselves. Continued repetition of this pattern leads to a

pervasive sad mood, as well as a negative cognitive bias. However, a plausible

alternative considers ineffective or insufficient emotion regulation strategies to lie at the

heart of the disorder (Joormann & Gotlib, 2010; Bertram & Dickhauser, 2012). In this

case, receiving a poor grade on the assignment results in negative emotions such as

disappointment. If an individual does not employ the necessary compensatory

mechanisms, or the emotions overwhelm the individual’s compensatory abilities, then

they are unable to overcome the negative affect which contributes to negative thought

patterns.

1.1. Cognitive Bias in Depression

Certainly, depressed individuals show a negative cognitive bias. Most obviously,

they tend to focus on negative material, and remember negative content more readily (Dai,

Feng & Koster, 2011; Gotlib & Joormann, 2010). However, a more general pattern of

2

altered cognition is also evident in these populations. This is best captured by the fact that

they do not show the same need for cognition (NFC; Bertram & Dickhauser, 2012) relative

to non-depressed individuals. NFC is the tendency to seek out and enjoy cognitive exertion

(e.g. preferring demanding strategy games). Because higher NFC is related to seeking

out effortful cognition, it is related to deeper processing of information and better

performance on cognitive tasks. Therefore, decreased NFC in depressed individuals might

explain the cognitive profile of depression. They have difficulties concentrating (Joormann,

Yoon & Zetsche, 2007) which likely contributes to the over-general memories that lack

detail and specificity (Rawal & Rice, 2012). And they show impairments on working

memory (WM) and executive function related tasks (Gotlib & Joormann, 2010; Nixon,

Liddle, Nixon & Liotti, 2013). Manipulating negative material in WM and removing negative

material from WM seems to be especially difficult for depressed individuals (Gotlib &

Joormann, 2010; Joormann & Gotlib, 2008). This would crowd WM capacity with negative

material, and could manifest as a generalized negative world view.

Interestingly, this distinctive cognitive profile of depression is most obvious in tasks

that 1) do not constrain attention 2) require more cognitive effort, and 3) where personal

emotions are salient (Joormann, Yoon & Zetsche, 2007; Gotlib & Joormann, 2010; Pelosi,

Slade, Blumhardt, & Sharma, 2000). For example, performance is poorer on free-recall

tasks relative to recognition tasks (Gotlib & Joormann 2010) and following a period

wherein rumination was possible. Thus, when dysphoric students had to wait before

receiving instructions, performance was decreased (Hertel, 1998). Most likely, depressed

individuals revert back to the dominant negative schema under these circumstances,

allowing them to brood on the salient negative information.

3

1.2. Rumination in Depression

This negative bias could be explained by the negative ruminatory cycle. In the case

of depression, rumination consists of recurring, intrusive thoughts fixated on the negative

mood state and its associated consequences (Joormann, Yoon & Zetche, 2007; Nolen &

Hoeksema, 1987). It is possible that dwelling on negative material results in a negative

mood and reinforces the negative schema which originally made the negative material

salient (Joormann, Yoon & Zetsche, 2007; Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan & De Raedt,

2011). In support of this postulation, such ruminatory thought patterns are associated with

more severe depressive symptomology in previously depressed individuals (Cooney,

Joormann, Eugène & Gotlib 2010; Joormann & Gotlib, 2010), and indeed reduces

inhibition for emotional material as measured by the negative affective priming task

(Zetsche & Joormann, 2011) irrespective of depressive symptoms (Joormann, 2006). This

suggests that people who are more prone to such ruminatory thought patterns, are more

likely to develop a negative schema, and are potentially at higher risk of developing

depression (Joormann, Yoon & Zetche, 2007).

However, while this description nicely captures the cognitive processes involved

in beginning and perpetuating a depressed episode, it does not account for the origins of

the ruminatory cycle. Why only certain individuals develop this thought pattern needs to

be addressed (Joormann, Yoon, & Zetche, 2007). This requires one of two solutions:

Firstly, one could assume that, while all aspects of the ruminatory process (negative

schema, inhibitory processes, and working memory content) are continually interacting,

one of the three is affected by trait factors and represents an innate vulnerability towards

developing depression. For example, a person may have lower activation thresholds for

negative material allowing more negative material to reach conscious awareness, and

4

thus building the negative schema. Alternatively, an additional factor may act on one of

the components, thereby initiating the ruminatory cycle. For instance, a pervasive negative

mood resulting from situational elements (e.g. losing one’s job or being bullied at school)

may lead to the negative schema, which precipitates the ruminatory cycle.

1.2.1. Dispositional Argument: Inhibition and WM

Research using the Negative Affective Priming (NAP) task often claim that

depression originates with a predisposition in the form of reduced inhibition for negative

material. During the task, participants are presented with two objects (either two words or

two faces), and are asked to ignore one while indicating the valence (positive, neutral or

negative) of the other (Wentura, 1999). The ignored word is considered the distractor,

while the attended word is the target (typically identified by font colour or a coloured square

on each face). Reaction time (RT) to the target is analyzed based on the valence of the

previous distractor. If the valence of the previous distractor is congruent with the valence

of the current target, RTs are typically slower relative to trials where the previous distractor

and current target valence are incongruent (Control Trials). This difference is best

quantified as a NAP effect, which is calculated by subtracting the RT on control trials from

the corresponding Ignored Repetition (IgnRep) trials for each word type.

Depressed individuals do not show the expected cost on IgnRep trials for negative

words. That is, they are not slower when responding to a negative word after previously

ignoring a negative word (Joormann & Gotlib, 2010; Joormann, 2006; Zetsche &

Joormann, 2011; Dai, Feng & Koster, 2011), a robust pattern that has been replicated for

sad faces (Dai & Feng, 2011; Goeleven, De Raedt, Baert & Koster, 2006; Zetsche &

Joormann, 2011). Even individuals experiencing dysphoria (typically measured through

BDI scores) show a similar pattern of results (Joormann, 2004; Frings, Wentura & Holtz,

2007). This reduced NAP effect for negative material is often interpreted as an inability to

5

inhibit negative material effectively. And it is this that is thought to drive the negative

ruminatory cycle (Gotlib & Joormann, 2010). The argument generally revolves around a

difficulty disengaging attention from the negative material (Owens, Koster & Derakshan,

2012; Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan & De Readt, 2011; Eugène, Joormann, Cooney

Atlas, & Gotlib, 2010). Essentially, a fixation on negative material and subsequent failure

to exercise attentional control over the content that enters WM (Joormann, Yoon & Zetche,

2007; Davis & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000) results in abnormal amounts of negative material

in WM. This primes other negative material and prevents positive material from being

processed, resulting in the negative schema, the negative ruminatory thought patterns and

pervasive negative affect. Hence, the reduced inhibition for negative material may

constitute a dispositional vulnerability (Dai & Feng, 2011; Zetsche & Joormann, 2011) that,

when aggravated by stress (Carter & Garber, 2011), leads to a depressed episode.

1.2.2. Emotion Regulation Argument: Sad Mood and Depression

However, given that the presence of negative cognitions may not predict the

severity of depressive symptoms (LaGrange et al. 2011), there must be more to the story.

Specifically, some evidence indicates that rumination is caused by ineffective emotion

regulation strategies (Joormann & Gotlib, 2010; Bertram & Dickhauser, 2012; Gotlib &

Joormann, 2010) suggesting a depressed episode might be triggered by a negative mood

state rather than negative thought patterns. In other words, it is possible that the inability

to overcome a negative mood state, prevents the sad mood from dissipating, which

prompts the negative schema. Importantly, this failure to dispel the sad mood may be

either an innate aspect of the individual (e.g. poor emotion regulation strategies), or

environmental (e.g. extremely negative circumstances). Thus, the current mood state may

simply be too severe or continuous to fend off, or emotion regulation strategies such as

compensatory mechanisms, might be insufficient. Consequently, this sad mood begins to

6

pervade the individual’s life resulting in the negative schema and thought patterns that

characterize the ruminatory cycle. If this is the case, then negative affect is the cause

rather than the consequence of the negative rumination.

Only a few studies have pursued this line of inquiry. However, evidence does

indicate that emotional state influences performance on the NAP task. Most importantly,

a negative mood induction has been shown to increase inhibition for negative words and

decrease inhibition for positive words; a pattern opposite that found in the neutral mood

induction condition (Goeleven, De Raedt & Koster, 2007) and opposite the pattern typically

observed in depressed (e.g. Joormann & Gotlib 2010) and dysphoric groups (e.g. Frings,

Wentura & Holtz, 2007). This increased inhibition for negative words has also been

observed in previously depressed (remitted) samples (Joormann & Gotlib, 2010;

Joormann, 2004), and was explained as a subconscious over-reaction to counteract the

priming bias for negative material (Joormann, Yoon, & Zetsche, 2007; Joormann, 2004).

Such an explanation is in line with the general avoidance of negative emotions shown by

most individuals (Grybowski, Wyczesany & Kaiser, 2014; Koster, De Lissnyder, Derashan,

& De Readt, 2011), and indicates that people naturally employ compensatory mechanisms

to prevent descending into a sad mood state. Likely, the sad mood and corresponding

negative thoughts conflict with a person’s tendency towards a positive self-view (Koster,

De Lissnyder, Derashan, & De Readt, 2011). The resulting conflict signal initiates some

compensatory mechanism that breaks the negative ruminatory cycle. The fact that this

does not happen in depressed individuals could indicate that frequent and excessive

negative emotions become part of a person's self-concept and thus no conflict occurs.

Consequently, the reduced inhibition for negative words in high depressed trait

groups so frequently reported, might represent an already broken system, rather than the

origins of the problem. Essentially, these subjects are already past appropriate

7

compensatory behaviours. They will have incorporated negative emotions into their self-

concept, so no conflict signal was engaged and rumination on negative material is

possible. Consistent with this logic, depressed individuals are less able to differentiate

positive and negative feedback (Foti & Hajcak, 2009), a pattern that appears to be driven

by current levels of sadness (Foti & Hajcak, 2010). Thus, prolonged exposure to a sad

mood state might reduce the impact of positive reinforcers, thereby contributing to a

negative world view. This clearly suggests that mood state lies at the heart of the cognitive

profile of depression.

If this line of reasoning is correct, then an emotional state that is congruent with a

person’s self-concept should result in rumination. If material that is incongruent with an

individual’s self-concept triggers a conflict signal, then no such signal would be engaged

for material that is congruent with the person’s self-concept. Without this conflict signal,

resources are not deployed to remove the congruent material from WM making rumination

possible. Because of the innate bias towards a positive self-image (Koster, De Lissnyder,

Derashan, & De Readt, 2011), a positive mood state would be most congruent with a low-

depressed trait person’s self-concept. However, the more negative self-concept of

individuals at greater risk for developing depression would be matched more closely by a

negative mood state. Thus, comparable amounts of rumination should be observed for

positive material in low-depressed trait participants during a positive mood state, and for

negative material in high-depressed trait individuals while in a negative mood state. Some

evidence suggests this speculation is correct. When in a negative mood state, high-

depressed trait individuals showed longer RT on the NAP task irrespective of the word

valence. Similarly, the low depressed trait group was slowest following an induced positive

mood (Booy & Liotti, 2015). Thus, when the induced mood matches a pre-existing internal

8

state (e.g. experiencing depressed symptoms) rumination occurs suggesting mood state

plays an important role in the negative ruminatory cycle.

1.3. The current study

With relatively little research examining the role of mood state on the NAP effect,

it is difficult to judge the validity of an emotion regulation account presented above. This

is compounded by insufficient knowledge on the cognitive mechanisms involved in the

task. Thus, to determine if factors beyond the negative ruminatory cycle influences the

NAP effect, two questions in particular need to be addressed: Firstly, to what extent is the

NAP effect attributable to dispositional factors? And secondly, how is the task processed

by the brain?

In order to address these, a modified version of the NAP task was created allowing

us to examine negative priming effects for each word valence independently. In the

literature, control trials are typically defined as trials where the probe target valence is

opposite to the prime distractor valence. In other words, on a control trial, if the probe

target was negative, the prime distractor was necessarily positive, whereas if the probe

target was positive the prime distractor was inevitably negative. This design leads to two

problems: First, because emotional material are part and parcel with the affective context

wherein they are perceived (Grzybowski, Wyczesany, & Kaiser, 2014), in designs that

contrast positive and negative content directly, there are no priming free trials, and so, no

true control trials. For example, a positive distractor might prime a positive mental set

which may be more difficult to disengage from if the current target is negative resulting in

a deceivingly inflated NAP effect. Second, it is unclear if the lack of a NAP effect can be

attributed to the effect of negative words. Instead, it may be driven by altered processing

of positive words, or changes in the cognitive treatment of both positive and negative

9

words. The modified version of the NAP task proposed here, seeks to dissociate the

effects of negative and positive words by implementing true control trials. Therefore, on

control trials in this modified task, the preceding distractor was a neutral word ensuring no

negative priming effect was present. This enabled us to isolate differences in negative

priming for each word. Specifically, we expected that the NAP effect for positive words

would be smallest, and the NAP effect for negative would be largest.

The modified NAP task was implemented in two studies. The first experiment

sought to determine whether the severity of depressed symptoms or dispositional factors

contributed more to the NAP effect. The second experiment aimed to determine the nature

of the NAP effect using event related potentials (ERPs). Given that previous studies were

unable to truly dissociate positive and negative material, it is necessary to determine if

positive and negative material are processed differently. The temporal resolution of EEG

allows us to examine the cognitive processes prior to making a response, and thereby

gain insight into the cognitive mechanisms underlying the NAP task.

10

Chapter 2.

Experiment 1

2.1. Introduction

Much of the research using the NAP paradigm has used clinical populations,

focussing on currently depressed, relative to previously depressed, and never depressed

individuals. If the task does measure an innate vulnerability towards depression, then a

sub-clinical sample (individuals who show a propensity towards depression but who are

not currently depressed) should show the same reduced NAP effect for negative words.

Two studies have examined the NAP effect in non-clinical populations. In the first

study, participants were divided into dysphoric and non-dysphoric groups based on their

Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) scores. As predicted, the

dysphoric sample showed a significantly reduced NAP effect for negative words

(Joormann, 2004). In the second study, subjects were divided into high and low depressed

trait groups using a median split based on their Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores.

Again, the expected reduced NAP effect for negative words was found (Frings, Wentura

& Holtz, 2007).

However, these studies could not distinguish vulnerability towards depression due

to dispositional/personality factors (‘trait’ effects, likely innate), from the actual presence

and severity of symptoms of negative affect (‘state’ effects precipitated by environmental

factors). This is because, both the BDI and CES-D require respondents to indicate how

they have been feeling in the recent past. This particular phrasing of the questionnaires

asks participants to think about their emotional state over the previous two weeks which

is congruent with the diagnostic criteria for depression. Thus, they measure the current

11

severity of depressive symptoms; symptoms that include negative affect, feelings of

worthlessness and sadness. A measure of trait depression should focus on a person’s

typical emotional response in general and not only in a specified time period. Hence, the

conclusion that a predisposing towards depression in the form of reduced inhibition for

negative words results in negative affect may not be warranted. It is equally possible that

severe or sustained negative affect results in the reduced NAP for negative words resulting

in greater risk for developing depression. These explanations indistinguishable using the

BDI or CES-D as a measure of depressed trait.

The present study sought to expand this line of research by dissociating the effects

of ‘trait’ and ‘state’ depression. To do this, respondents completed the NEO-PI-R N3

subscale in addition to the BDI-II. While the NEO-PI-R asks respondents to consider how

they typically think and act, the BDI-II asks them to consider how they have felt recently.

Thus, the former can be considered a measure of ‘trait’ depression; a more general,

possibly innate, predisposition to experience negative emotions when confronted with

environmental challenges. The latter however, measures ‘state’ depression; the presence

of depressed symptoms due to environmental factors. This allowed us to address the first

research question: are dispositional factors primarily responsible for the NAP effect?

Although the focus is typically on negative material, previous studies do show

numeric if not statistical differences in the NAP effect for positive words as well. Given that

the modified NAP task was designed to dissociate the effects of positive and negative

words it was predicted that larger differences would be observed for both positive and

negative words. Specifically, because of the general positive self-bias exhibited by most

individuals, the NAP effect for positive words was expected to be reduced, while the NAP

effect for negative words was expected to be increased compared to neutral words.

12

Because of the emphasis the BDI-II instructions places on emotional state we

suspected that previous findings might represent an effect of mood state. Therefore, it was

predicted that symptom severity rather than trait depression would be the primary force

behind altered processing of valenced material. Lastly, no group differences would be

present as a function of ‘trait’ depression.

2.2. Methods

2.2.1. Participants

184 female undergraduate students (MAge=19.13 SD=1.74 Min=17 Max=26) with

normal or corrected to normal vision, and no reported history of depression or anxiety,

were recruited via the online Research Participation System at Simon Fraser University

receiving course credit for their participation. Students admitting to a history of affective

disorder were excluded to avoid unnecessary biases in the results. Since mood disorders

and vulnerability to depression are more common among women, (Kornstein, Schatzberg,

Thase, et al, 2000) and women tend to show more symptoms, higher symptom severity,

and report more subjective distress compared to depressed men (Seney & Sibille, 2014)

we restricted the sample to females. Participants with accuracy below 50% for any word

type were discarded, yielding a final sample of 137 individuals (MAge=19.17 SD=1.77

Min=17 Max=27).

2.2.2. Procedure

After obtaining informed consent, participants completed a medical history

questionnaire containing some demographic questions, the NEO-PI-R N3 subscale and

the BDI-II. They then completed the modified NAP task.

13

2.2.3. Design

During the NAP task, a central fixation cross was displayed for 500ms, followed by

a response slide. The response slide contained two words presented one above the other,

in the centre of the screen and remained on screen for up to 2.5sec. The words were

separated by a 1cm edge-to-edge gap which contained a white fixation cross. The words

were coloured in yellow and pink, indicating which word was to be ignored and which was

to be attended to. The attended colour was counterbalanced such that half of the

participants attended to the yellow word and half attended to the pink word. The font colour

was independent of valence.

Table 1: The modified NAP task.

positive word (+) negative word (-) neutral word (N)

Because each slide served as the prime for the next slide as well as the probe for the previous slide the probe target and prime distractor varied. This allowed for a more trials within a shorter time period in order to avoid fatigue effects. Additionally, systematic effects of the prime target and probe distractor would average out, ensuring any effects is due to the prime distractor, probe target combination. The NAP effect for neutral words is not of particular interest in the present study. Future studies should focus on validating this task. In particular, the differential priming effects of negative versus positive words can be identified within the control condition for neutral words.

A slide could contain a positive and a negative word, a neutral and a positive word,

or a negative and a neutral word, but never two words of the same valence. Nor could any

target be preceded by a target of the same valence (see table 1) and only IgnRep and

corresponding control trials were present in the experiment. These criteria were chosen to

maximize the amount of trails in the experiment while limiting the time required for

Positive Word Negative Words Neutral Words

IgnRep Control IgnRep Control IgnRep Control

Prime - Distractor + N - N N +/-

Probe - Target + + - - N N

14

completing the task. This reduced the risk of fatigue effects and the possibility of

systematic mood effects resulting from boredom of frustration.

Six trial lists were generated according to the following criteria: First, each of the

six conditions (IgnRep and Control for each word valence) had to appear five times in each

list. Second, none of the words could be repeated within the same list, though the same

words may appear in multiple lists. Third, the order of the conditions could not be the same

across lists. These constraints determined the order of the six conditions in each mini-

block. Participants completed two repetitions of each of the six trail lists (consisting of 30

trials each) for a total of 360 experimental trials. Additionally, subjects completed 15

practice trials. Any trials on which subjects made an incorrect response, or with RT below

300ms were excluded from the analysis. No response was recorded after 2500ms.

Words were selected from the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW)

database. The ANEW database provides the average valence, arousal and dominance

ratings for commonly used English words (Bradley & Lang, 1999). Ratings range from 1-

10 with higher values indicating higher levels of the construct. 64 positive, 64 negative,

and 50 neutral words were selected based on valence rating, and controlling for length

and arousal ratings. Words with valence ratings above 6 were considered for the positive

list, between 4 and 6 for the neutral list, and below 4 for the negative list. Because of their

highly arousing properties to a sub-portion of people, any words that are commonly

associated with fear (such as snake, serpent, spider, etc.) were excluded from

consideration to control extraneous factors as much as possible.

15

2.2.4. Variables

NEO-PI-R

The depression (N3) subscale from the NEO Personality Inventory Revised (NEO-

PI-R) was used to measure a subject’s propensity towards depression. It includes eight

statements and participants indicate how well a statement describes them on a 5-point

scale from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Theoretically, scores range from 0-

32 and ranged from 0-29 in the present study. This measure has good internal consistency

(N3 coefficient α=.83) and inter-rater reliability (N3 cross-observer r=.51; McCrae, Martin

& Costa, 2005).

BDI-II

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) is a 21-item questionnaire used to

measure the severity of depressive symptoms during the preceding two weeks including

today. Responses corresponding to no symptoms are assigned a score of 0, while severe

symptoms are scored 3. Theoretically, scores range from 0 – 63 and ranged from 0-34 in

the present study. The BDI – II is well established as a reliable and valid measure of

depressed symptoms (outpatient coefficient α =.92, n=500; correlation with Hamilton

Psychiatric Rating Scale for Depression r=.71, n=87; Beck, Brown & Steer, 1989).

16

Table 2: Demographics

Mean and standard deviation for each group on important variables. The HBDI and LBDI groups differed significantly in terms of BDI scores while the HD and LD groups differed significantly in terms of NEO-PI-R N3 scores.

2.2.5. Statistical Analysis

T-tests comparing the accuracy (ACC) for each word valence were conducted to

determine if performance across the three word valences were comparable. Tests were

run at the α’=.033 significance level capping family-wise error rate at α=.1.

To address the research question of whether there are differences in the priming

effect for different word valences, a single factor, within-subject analysis of variance

(ANOVA) to examine the differences in the NAP effect for each word valence (Positive,

Neutral and Negative). The NAP effect is defined as the difference between RTs to IgnRep

and the corresponding control trials for each word valence. Follow-up t-tests were

conducted to compare specific conditions relevant to the a-priori hypotheses. Family-wise

error rate was capped at α=.1, using a Bonferroni correction which kept the significance

level for individual test at α’=.025.

To determine if dysphoria is characterized by alterations in processing of negative

material only, subjects where split into high BDI (HBDI) and low BDI (LBDI) groups.

Consistent with the cut-offs used by Frings, Wentura and Holtz (2007) subjects who scored

AGE BDI-II NEO-PI-R N3

Experiment 1

Overall 19.17 ± 1.77 8.99 ± 6.61 13.84 ± 5.11

HBDI 19.06 ± 1.68 12.65 ± 5.71 15.52 ± 4.71

LBDI 19.35 ± 1.91 3.00 ± 1.98 11.10 ± 4.56

HD 19.09 ± 1.54 11.88 ± 7.41 18.36 ± 3.32

LD 19.23 ± 1.93 6.86 ± 5.03 10.52 ± 3.34

Experiment 2 Overall 18.74 ± 1.62 9.05 ± 6.84 13.66 ± 4.91

17

7 or greater on the BDI-II were assigned to the HBDI group (n=85) while subjects scoring 6

or below were assigned to the LBDI group (n=52). An independent samples t-test comparing

BDI scores in the HBDI (M=12.65, SD=5.71) and LBDI (M=3.00, SD=1.98) groups confirmed

that the groups were significantly different; t113.07=11.74, p<.001. The mean BDI score for

each group in the current study was similar to the mean BDI score of the corresponding

group in the Frings, Wentura & Holtz (2007) study. A 2 (BDI group) x 3 (WordValence)

ANOVA was then conducted on the NAP scores for each word valence. This was followed

up by paired samples t-test comparing the HBDI and LBDI NAP scores for each word

valence. All tests were run at the α’=.05 significance level capping the family-wise error

rate at α=.2. Due to the unequal sample sizes, sphericity and equality of variance was not

assumed and corrected significant values are reported.

To determine if any word valence effect can be attributed to depressed ‘trait’,

participants were then divided into a high-depressed trait (HD) and low depressed trait

(LD) group based on their depression scores from the NEO-PI-R N3 subscale. An

independent samples t-test comparing N3 scores in the HD (M=18.36, SD=3.318) and LD

(M=10.52, SD=3.34) groups confirmed that the groups were significantly different;

t123.50=13.61, p<.001.

The standardized mean for college age women on this subscale is 14.

Consequently, subjects scoring 14 or below were included in the LD group (n=79) while

those scoring 15 or greater constituted the HD group (n=58). A 2 (N3 group) x 3

(WordValence) ANOVA was then conducted on the NAP scores for each word valence.

This was followed up by paired samples t-test comparing the HD and LD NAP scores for

each word valence. All tests were run at the α’=.05 significance level capping the family-

wise error rate at α=.2. Due to the unequal sample sizes, sphericity and equality of

variance was not assumed and corrected significant values was reported.

18

2.3. Results

2.3.1. Accuracy

ACC for positive words was significantly higher than both positive and negative

words, t(136)=8.21, p<.001; t(136)=2.95, p=.004, respectively. However, no differences were

observed between positive and negative words, t(136)=-1.76, p=.08. This indicates that

positive words were significantly easier to identify correctly than both negative and neutral

words. It is possible that this had a systematic effect on RTs to positive words, however

numerical differences between ACC for the word valences were small, as was the

variability in ACC to all word types. Therefore, the influence of these differences was not

anticipated to be a large problem for the present study.

Table 3: Accuracy

Mean ACC and standard deviation for each word valence. In Experiment 1, ACC to positive words was significantly higher than ACC to other word valences. This lead to more stringent criteria for including specific word in the list. Consequently, in Experiment 2, ACC to neutral words was significantly higher than ACC to other word valences.

2.3.2. Overall NAP effects

The t-tests on the NAP effect for each word valence indicated significant

differences between the three word valences. Positive words showed a facilitation effect

while negative and neutral words both showed a delay (see table 2). The NAP effect for

positive words (PosWordNAP) was significantly lower than the NAP effect for both

negative (NegWordNAP) and neutral (NeuWordNAP) words; t136=-9.67, p<.001 and t136=-

4.56, p<.001 respectively. The smaller difference between NegWordNAP and

Positive Words Negative Words Neutral Words

Experiment 1 .79±.05 .75±.06 .77±.08

Experiment 2 .86±.06 .85±.07 .91±.04

19

NeuWordNAP was also significant; t136=4.79, p<.001. These results suggest that the

traditional NAP effect is composed of two sub-processes, a facilitation effect for positive

words, and an impeding effect for negative words.

2.3.3. Effects of ‘state’ depression on NAP effect

A 2 between (BDI group: High vs. Low) x 3 within (Word valence: Negative,

Neutral, or Positive) subject mixed-factors ANOVA revealed a main effect for word

valence; F(1.98,269.54)=36.99, MSE=2570.82, p<.001, consistent with the word valence

differences described above. More critically, the two-way interaction between BDI group

and word valence was significant, F(2,269.54)=4.235, MSE=2570.82, p=.016. Independent

samples t-tests revealed that the HBDI group had a significantly reduced NAP score

compared to the LBDI group for positive words (on average 18.79 ms); t115.62=-2.133,

p=.016. Additionally, the HBDI group was had an increased NAP score compared to the LBDI

group for negative words (on average 17.55ms). However, this difference approached but

did not reach significance, t119.09=1.82, p=.063 (see figure 1).

20

Figure 1: NAP Effect

The NAP effect to each word valence. The HBDI group showed an increased NAP to negative words and a facilitation effect for positive words relative to the LBDI group. No differences were observed between the HD and LD groups. This suggests that symptom severity and not dispositional factors are behind altered processing of valenced material in dysphoric individuals, and that that the effect is not limited to negative material. Importantly, the same pattern of results is observed in all conditions.

2.3.4. Effects of ‘’trait’ depression on NAP effect

The One-Between-One-Within ANOVA (N3 group x Word Valence) again revealed

a main effect for word valence; F(2,270.00)=44.24, p<.001. This again emphasizes the word

valence pattern described above. Critically however, the two-way interaction N3 group X

21

word valence was not significant; F(2,270.00)=.18, p=.84. Consistently, HD and LD groups

were similar on PosWordNAP and NegWordNAP scores; t129.01=-.267, p=.79 and

t128.07=.51, p=.612 respectively (see figure 1).

2.4. Discussion

The first novel finding of this study is that in the modified NAP task, designed to

dissociate the influence of a prior positive or negative word, two subcomponents of the

NAP effect were identified; a facilitation effect for positive words, and an impeding effect

for negative words. This overall pattern supports the first prediction, and suggests that the

sample of female college students displayed a general orientation towards positive

material, with avoidance of negative material. This pattern held true irrespective of current

dysphoria or depressed trait, which may be an artifact of the subclinical sample. Following

inhibitory accounts of negative priming, this would suggest that subjects were less able to

ignore positive words, and more able to ignore negative words. Thus, while a positive

distractor might prime a positive mental set, inhibition for negative material following a

negative distractor needs to be overcome before a response can be made.

The second prediction, that participants would exhibit differences in processing

both positive and negative material as a function of current depression symptoms (BDI

status) was also supported. However, the direction of the effects was unexpected. The

HBDI group showed a larger NAP effect for negative words and a smaller NAP for positive

words than the LBDI group. Thus, it seems that HBDI subjects were more responsive to

positive words and less responsive to negative words compared to normal controls. One

possible interpretation of these results is that our sample of healthy participants with

subclinical dysphoria were employing compensatory mechanisms to counteract the higher

levels of depressed symptoms they were experiencing. This explanation is consistent with

22

findings suggesting previously depressed individuals in clinical remission are biased

towards positive material (Joormann & Gotlib, 2010) as well as the positive bias found in

the general population (Koster, De Lissnyder, Derashan, & De Readt, 2011).

The third prediction, that ‘trait’ depression would have less influence on NAP

effects, was also upheld. When subjects were divided into the HD and LD groups based

on the N3 subscale, no significant group differences were found. This suggests that more

stable, dispositional traits may not be responsible for the apparent difference in the NAP

results seen between the BDI groups. Rather it appears that the presence of depressed

symptoms is responsible for differences in responses to negative and positive material.

In general, results suggest greater depressed symptoms are responsible for

altered processing of emotional material, but that these differences are not due to innate

factors. Rather they appear to be compensatory mechanisms used to counteract higher

levels of negative affect.

Table 4: Mean # of Trials Retained

The average number of retained trials for Experiment 2 after discarding trials contained artifacts (blinks, etc.) and incorrect responses. Consistent with the accuracy results, more trials were retained for neutral words.

Positive Words Negative Words Neutral Words

IgnRep 42.08 (9.39) 43.70 (8.13) 45.97 (6.62)

Control 42.38 (8.29) 42.65 (7.56) 46.24 (7.41)

23

Chapter 3.

Experiment 2

3.1. Introduction

The general pattern of results from experiment 1 suggests that positive and

negative words are processed differently regardless of currently mood. However, it is not

clear if this depends on bottom-up or top-down effects (Grzybowski, Wyczesany, & Kaiser,

2014).

A large body of research investigating the electrophysiology of emotion suggest

that both early and late stages of processing are affected. A robust finding is that relative

to neutral stimuli, emotion enhances the amplitude of the late positive potential (LPP), a

sustained wave with broad distribution over posterior scalp, peaking between 400-700ms,

thought to reflect conscious (top-down) evaluation of a motivationally salient stimulus (e.g.,

Moser, Hajcak, Bukay, & Simons, 2006; Hajcak, MacNamara, & Olvet, 2010). Shorter

latency ERP effects in the P2 family (180-300ms), have also been reported, with both

posterior (Early posterior negativity or EPN, Schupp, Flaisch, Stockburger, & Junghöfer,

2006), and anterior scalp distribution (Early anterior positivity or EAP: Taake, Jaspers-

Fayer, & Liotti, 2009; Williams, Palmer, Liddell, Song, & Gordon, 2006), thought to reflect

more automatic or even pre-attentive (bottom-up) aspects of attentional capture. This

suggests that both top-down and bottom-up mechanisms are involved in the processing

of emotional material. Therefore, the differences in RT to positive and negative words,

might reflect variations at different stages of processing.

Yao, Liu, Liu, Hu, Yi & Huang (2010) addressed this question by examining the

ERP waveforms associated with the NAP task in depressed patients and normal controls.

24

They found reduced amplitude of the centro-parietal P2 and latency of the late positive

complex (LPC) components within the patients for negative words. On the other hand, the

control group showed increased amplitude of P2 component over left-central scalp and a

longer LPC latency to negative targets. This suggests that both early and late stages of

processing are affected by depression. However, because the primary concern of this

study was to compare patients to normal controls, it did not attempt to isolate the effects

of state versus trait depression. Specifically, it could not be determined whether altered

patterns in early or late processing of negative material reflects the presence of negative

affect or a predisposition towards depression. More importantly, the study employed a

design similar to Joormann (2004), in which positive and negative words are directly

contrasted with one another. Thus, on IgnRep trials for negative words, the prime slide

contained a negative distractor and a positive target, while the probe slide contained a

negative target and a positive distractor. On control trials for negative words, both the

distractor and target was positive while the probe slide contained a negative target and a

positive distractor. This prevents the priming effects of positive and negative words from

being fully dissociated from one another.

One other study examined the NAP effect in the context of ERPs. Using a facial

variant of the NAP task, researchers compared never depressed, previously depressed

and currently depressed individuals (Dai, Feng & Koster, 2011). The task presented

subjects with happy, sad or neutral faces. Two pictures appeared simultaneously, one

above the other, and subjects were asked to indicate if the face in colour (as opposed to

black and white) was happy, sad or neutral. Currently depressed participants showed

greater P1 and P3 amplitude for sad faces, indicating deficient inhibition for negative

material. Unfortunately, given that written words hold symbolic meaning only (Grzybowski,

Wyczesany, & Kaiser, 2014), and are processed differently from faces, it is unlikely that

25

the cognitive processes underlying the facial and verbal variants of the NAP task are

directly comparable. However, again changes in both early and late processing were

evident. The particular design of this task contrasted happy and sad faces in a manner

similar to previous studies described. Thus, it is still not clear whether the NAP effect is

driven by altered processing of negative words only.

Using the modified version of the word NAP task, the present study set out to

dissociate the priming effect of positive and negative words and determine which stage of

processing drives the effect for each word valence. Participants completed the task while

continuous EEG was recorded. Distinct ERP waveforms were then extracted for IgnRep

and Control trials for each of the three word valences. Behaviourally a pattern of results

similar to that found in experiment one was expected since the tasks are virtually identical.

Given that this variant of the NAP task used words, modulations within the P2 and

LPP time ranges were expected consistent with findings by Yao et. al. (2010). With regards

to the P2, it was predicted that IgnRep trials would elicit a larger positivity than control

trials due to increased salience of the target. This difference would be best characterized

as an early anterior positivity (EAP) because the EAP is thought to index the self-relevance

or emotional salience of valenced material (Taake, Jasper-Fayer & Liotti, 2009). However,

it was predicted that the EAP would be larger for negative words relative to positive and

neutral words due to a possible association with threat detection systems (Grzybowski,

Wyczesany, & Kaiser, 2014). For example, someone expressing anger might indicate an

imminent physical attack. Similarly, disappointment expressed by group members might

indicate that a relationship is in jeopardy. In both cases early detection of the negative

emotion is essential in order to act appropriately and mitigate the perceived threat.

The LPP is defined as a late positive deflection in response to emotional, relative

to neutral, stimuli and is thought to indicate rumination or maintenance of relevant

26

information in WM. Therefore, a positivity within this time window was expected for

negative and positive words only. The amplitude of the LPP was predicted to be larger for

IgnRep trials since more conscious processing might be required in order to overcome

inhibition of the previous distractor and make a correct response. However, due to the

positive bias shown by most individuals (Koster, De Lissnyder, Derashan, & De Readt,

2011), it was predicted that this difference would be greatest for positive words.

3.2. Methods

3.2.1. Participants

46 female undergraduate students (MAge=18.67 SD=1.32 Min=17 Max=24) were

recruited via the online Research Participation System at Simon Fraser University

receiving course credit for their participation. The same exclusion criteria as experiment 1

was applied. Thus, only individuals with normal or corrected to normal vision, and no

reported history of depression or anxiety were included in the sample. Additionally,

because any trials containing incorrect responses or artifacts such as blinks are discarded,

subjects with fewer than 30% of trials retained for any condition were removed from

analysis. Thus, the final sample consisted of 37 individuals (MAge=18.67 SD=1.36 Min=17

Max=24).

3.2.2. Procedure and Design

Very little was altered between the two experiments in order to maintain

consistency and comparability. Thus, after providing informed consent, participants

completed the medical questionnaire, the NEO-PI-R N3 subscale, and the BDI-II while the

EEG cap was being prepared. This allowed us to compare the current sample to

Experiment 1 on some key variables (see table 2). Scores on the BDI-II ranged from 0-27

while scores on the NEO-PI-R N3 scale ranged from 5-25 in the present study. Following

27

this, they completed the modified version of the NAP task while continuous EEG was

recorded.

The NAP task was almost identical to the one used in experiment 1 with a few

minor adjustments to ensure compatibility with ERP methodology and to address the

concern of systematic ACC differences present in Experiment 1. Primarily this required a

jittered inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 300-800ms during which a fixation cross is presented.

Additionally, a pilot study was conducted to select which words were to be included in the

study. Words between 4 and 6 letter long were selected from the Affective Norms for

English Words (ANEW) database. Words with valence ratings above 6 were considered

for the positive list, between 4 and 6 for the neutral list, and below 4 for the negative list,

while controlling for word length and arousal ratings. Any words that may be associated

with fear (such as snake, serpent, spider, etc.) were excluded from consideration. This

yielded 80 positive words, 91 negative words, and 71 neutral words. Pilot study

participants then completed a simple task where they simply responded to each word,

indicating if it’s positive, neutral or negative. Words with the lowest probability of being

correctly identified (ACC<70%) were excluded from the list. Following this, to ensure the

same number of words for each word valence was utilized in the study, words with extreme

RTs (highest or lowest RT within each word valence) were trimmed until 62 words from

each lists with the highest accuracy and RT closest to the list average remained. This was

done to maximize the strength of the manipulation, and reduce variability in RT and

accuracy. Trial lists were then generated according to the same rules as in experiment 1

(see table 1).

3.2.3. Electrophysiological Recording

Continuous EEG was recorded using 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes mounted in an elastic

cap (Electrocap Inc.) arranged in a standard 10-20 montage. Additional electrodes were

28

placed on the left and right mastoids, approximately 1cm lateral to the external canthi

(measuring horizontal eye movements) and approximately 2cm below each eye

(measuring vertical eye movements and blinks). Voltage at each site was determined

against a common mode sense (CMS) electrode and recorded at a sampling rate of

512Hz.

EEG for each subject was digitally filtered (0.01Hz high-pass, 30Hz low-pass, zero

phase, 12dB/octave slope) and re-referenced to the average mastoid (BESA 5.3). Visual

inspection as well as semiautomatic artifact rejection were performed to eliminate trials

containing blinks and eye movements. Distinct ERP averages were obtained for each of

the 6 conditions (IgnRep and Control for each word type) time-locked to word onset

(200ms pre-stimulus baseline and 800ms post-stimulus). Subjects with fewer than 30%

usable trials in any condition were discarded from the analysis.

3.2.4. Statistical Analysis

Behavioural

T-tests comparing the accuracy (ACC) for each word valence were conducted to

determine if performance across the three word valences were comparable. Tests were

run at the α’=.033 significance level capping family-wise error rate at α=.1.

This was followed by a single factor, within-subject analysis of variance (ANOVA)

to examine the differences in the NAP effect for each word valence (Positive, Neutral and

Negative). The NAP effect is calculated as the difference between IgnRep and the

corresponding control trials for each word valence, providing a measure of the cost

associated with previously ignoring a word of the same valence as the current target.

Follow-up t-tests were conducted to compare specific conditions relevant to the a-priori

29

hypotheses. Family-wise error rate was capped at α=.1, using a Bonferonni correction

which kept the significance level for individual test at α’=.025.

ERP

Through visual inspection of the grand average waveforms for each condition, 2

different ERP time windows where IgnRep and control waveforms diverged were

identified. The first effect was a positive polarity enhancement spanning roughly 200-

300ms primarily over left-lateral frontal scalp, closely resembling the Early Anterior

Positivity (EAP). The second effect was a later positive polarity modulation over posterior

scalp, spanning 500-700ms, resembling the late posterior potential (LPP).

EAP mean amplitudes were calculated between 190-260ms over a left anterior

(F1, F3, F5) region of interest (ROI). A 3x2 within-subjects repeated-measures ANOVA

was conducted comparing the effects of WordValence (PosWord vs. NegWord vs.

NeuWord) and TrailType (IgnRep vs. Control). This was followed by 3 t-test comparisons

examining differences between the IgnRep and Control trials within each word valence.

Visual inspection of the ERP waveforms suggested amplitude of the EAP for both

TrailType to positive words was comparable to the EAP amplitude to negative words on

IgnRep trials. T-test comparisons were conducted to test this impression. In total, 6

statistical tests were conducted at the α’=.04 significance level capping family-wise error

rate at α=.24.

LPP mean amplitudes were calculated between 500-700ms post stimulus over a

posterior region of interest (ROI; P3, P1, Pz, P2, P4). A 3x2 (WordValence x TrailType)

within-subjects repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted, after which t-test

comparisons were run. These examined the differences between the overall mean

amplitudes for each word valence as well as the difference in IgnRep and Control trials for

30

each word valence. A total of 8 statistical tests were conducted at the α’=.03 significance

level capping family-wise error rate at α=.24.

3.3. Results

3.3.1. Behavioural

Accuracy

No differences were observed between positive and negative words, t(36)=.46,

p=.65. However, ACC for neutral words was significantly higher than both positive and

negative words, t(36)=-4.39, p<.001; t(36)=-5.04, p<.001, respectively (see table 3). This

suggests that neutral words were much easier to identify than both positive and negative

words. However, this is not deemed problematic for the experiment since neutral words

are compared to themselves in the IgnRep and Control conditions, just as is done for the

positive and negative words. Thus, although neutral words might in general be easier to

identify, this would be the case in both the IgnRep and Control conditions. Moreover the

primary purpose of the neutral words were to dissociate the effects of positive and negative

words and no differences between positive and negative words were observed. Thus,

none of the variance in RT on positive and negative words can be attributed to differences

in difficulty.

NAP

A visual inspection of the results indicates that the overall pattern in NAP scores

from Experiment 1 was also present here in Experiment 2 (see figure 1). Specifically,

previously ignoring a positive word improved performance for a positive target, while

previously ignoring a negative word resulted in slowed RT for a negative target. The within-

subjects ANOVA revealed no significant differences between the NAP effect for each word

valences, F(2,72)=1.88, MSE=2263.26, p=.16. It is possible that the study suffered from a

31

lack of power due to the large variability in RTs as well as the small sample size.

Consistent with expectations, the difference between NAP effects for positive and negative

words was large, but not significant t(36)=-2.01, p=.05. This could indicate that an effect is

present, but that the present study is unable to detect it. However, no other effects

approached significance. Given that the overall pattern is similar to that found in

experiment 1 (see figure 1), the tasks must be operating on the same underlying

processes and are thus comparable.

3.3.2. ERP

EAP (190-260ms)

The 3x2 within-subjects ANOVA revealed a significant main effect for Trail Type,

F(1,36)=4.82, MSE=3.03, p=.04. However neither the main effect for WordValence nor the

2-way interaction was significant, F(2,72)=2.89, MSE=3.23, p=.41, F(2,72)=2.04, MSE=2.19,

p=.93. Visual inspection of the ERPs suggest that mean amplitude for IgnRep was more

positive than mean amplitude for Control trials, and that the effect was largest for negative

words (see figure 3). This is in line with the a-priori hypothesis and thus, follow-up t-tests

were conducted to examine the differences between IgnRep and Control trials within each

word valence. Consistent with the hypothesis, the difference in mean amplitude between

IgnRep and Control trials was significant for negative words, t36=2.41, p=.02. However, no

differences were observed for positive and neutral words, t36=1.35, p=.18, t36=.48, p=.64,

respectively. Thus, IgnRep trials elicited a larger positivity for negative words, relative to

control trials. Interestingly, the amplitude of the EAP to negative words on IgnRep trails

appeared visually similar to the EAP amplitude to positive words regardless of trial type.

Consistent with this suspicion, no differences were observed between positive word

32

IgnRep trials and negative word IgnRep trials, nor between positive word control trials and

negative word IgnRep trials, t36=.65, p=.52, t36=-.78, p=.44, respectively.

LPP (500-700ms)

The 3x2 within-subjects ANOVA yielded significant main effects for both TrialType

and WordValence, F(1,36)=50.03, MSE=6.00, p<.001; F(2,72)=21.22, MSE=4.80, p<.001,

respectively. Follow-up t-tests indicate that the main effect of word valence was driven by

large differences between positive and neutral words as well as negative and neutral

words regardless of TrialType, t36=8.82, p<.001, t36=8.87, p<.001, respectively (see figure

2). However, the 2-way interaction was not significant, F(2,72)=2.71, MSE=4.26, p=.07.

Given that the a-priori hypothesis predicted that IgnRep and Control trial effects would

manifest differently across the three word valences, follow-up t-tests were conducted to

examine these differences more directly.

These revealed that the difference in mean amplitude between IgnRep and Control

trials was significant for positive and neutral words, t36=3.98, p<.001, t36=2.20, p=.03,

respectively. However, no differences were observed for negative words, t36=1.87, p=.07.

Thus, IgnRep trials elicited a larger positivity for positive and neutral words, relative to their

respective control trials (see figure 4). However, the lack of a significant difference

between IgnRep and Control trials for negative words, seems to be driven by a larger LPP

for control trials. Accordingly, neither the mean amplitude on IgnRep nor Control trials for

negative words, differed significantly from the mean amplitude on IgnRep trials for positive

words, t36=-1.41, p=.17, t36=-.03, p=.98, respectively. Thus, a negative target elicited a

large LPP regardless of the valence of the previous distractor (see figure 3).

33

Figure 2: ERPs to Word Valences

Average mastoid referenced ERP waveforms to IgnRep and Control trials for each word valence regardless of TrialType. As expected, a large LPP was observed for both positive and negative targets.

34

Figure 3: ERPs to Negative Words

Average mastoid referenced ERP waveforms to IgnRep and Control trials for negative words. In stark contrast to positive words (figure 4), IgnRep trials for negative words elicited a larger EAP which is most evident at F3. This indicates that previously ignoring a negative word, increases the salience of a negative target at early stages of processing. Also of note is the large positivity to both IgnRep and control trials, suggesting that salience of a negative target at later stages of processing was not affected by TrialType.

35

Figure 4: ERPs to Positive Words

Average mastoid referenced ERP waveforms to IgnRep and Control trials for positive words. Clearly evident is a large LPP to IgnRep trials across the entire scalp, although the difference between IgnRep and control trials is largest over parietal regions. This indicates that previously ignoring a positive word, increased the amount of resources devoted to conscious processing of a positive target.

36

Figure 5: Topographic Maps

Topographic maps based on the average referenced differences waves between IgnRep and Control trials for each word valence (positive and negative words). Importantly, the primary difference for negative words lies over left anterior scalp in both epochs. In contrast, a large positive deflection to IgnRep trails was observed for positive words within the LPP time window.

3.4. Discussion

In the present study, the modified version of the NAP task enabled us to completely

dissociate the priming effects of positive and negative words. Consistent with

expectations, modulations in both an early (EAP) and late (LPP) component were

identified. These components conform to those found by Yao et. al. (2010) suggesting that

similar cognitive processes underlie both variants of the task, and consequently inspiring

37

confidence in the validity of the modified NAP task used in the present study. The presence

of these components suggest that both early and late stages of processing are affected

by the presentation of emotional material.

3.4.1. EAP

The prediction that IgnRep trials would elicit a larger positivity than control trials

within the P2 time window was supported. The EAP is a positive deflection within the P2

family over anterior scalp and is thought to index bottom-up attentional capture of

emotional material (Schupp, Flaisch, Stockburger, & Junghöfer, 2006; Taake, Jasper-

Fayer, & Liotti, 2009; Asmaro, Carolan & Liotti, 2014). Consequently, it likely reflects early

and implicit capture by motivationally salient emotional stimuli (Taake, Jasper-Fayer &

Lioti, 2009). Consistent with this explanation, marijuana users show an enhanced EAP for

drug related stimuli (Asmaro, Carolan, & Liotti, 2014). Drug related stimuli are presumably

more relevant to drug users in much the same way as when one notices a particular brand

of car more often after purchasing one. Thus, the enhanced EAP to IgnRep trials in the

present study suggests that previously seeing a distractor of the same valence as the

current target, increased the salience of that stimulus type, and thus the self-relevance of

the target.

When separated by word valence, the prediction that the EAP difference (IgnRep

vs. Control) would be larger for negative words relative to positive and neutral words was

supported. The enhanced EAP on IgnRep trials for negative words indicates that

previously presenting an irrelevant negative word, increased the emotional salience of the

subsequent negative target. Similar results have been found in a study examining

electrophysiological responses to threat-related stimuli in high and low anxiety individuals.

In this case the EAP manifested as a larger positive modulation with an anterior scalp

distribution to threat-related relative to neutral words regardless of anxiety status (Taake,

38

Jasper-Fayer, & Liotti, 2009). This would suggest that negative material captures attention

more readily, which might represent the need to orient to negative feedback. Thus, in the

present study, prior presentation of task-irrelevant negative material might serve as an

implicit warning that a negative stimulus is coming.

3.4.2. LPP

The prediction that an LPP for positive and negative relative to neutral words would

be observed was supported. The LPP is a component in the P3 family (Grzybowski,

Wyczesany, & Kaiser, 2014), and appears as a positive deflection over centro-parietal

scalp to both pleasant and unpleasant stimuli beginning approximately 300ms post-

stimulus (Hajcak, Weinberg, MacNamara and Foti, 2012). The LPP is thought to originate

when WM resources are allocated to the increased processing of motivationally relevant

material (Schupp, Flaisch, Stockburger, & Junghöfer, 2006), for example, culturally salient

or task relevant stimuli (Asmaro, Carolan & Liotti, 2014). There is also evidence that top-

down regulatory processes can modulate the LPP (Hajcak, MacNamara & Olvet, 2010).

Therefore, it likely reflects more conscious processing of stimuli (Asmaro, Carolan & Liotti,

2014) and especially its maintenance in WM.

Furthermore, as predicted the amplitude of the LPP was larger for IgnRep than

control trials. This might indicate that more conscious processing was required in order to

overcome inhibition of the previous distractor and make a correct response. Alternatively,

it might also indicate that previously ignoring a word of the same valence as the current

target simply increased the motivation to ruminate on the target. The latter explanation is

most consistent with the EAP differences observed in the present study since RTs to

positive words showed a facilitation effect. However, these explanations cannot be teased

apart with in the present study design.

39

Although not statistically significant, a surprising interaction between WordValence

and TrialType was observed. IgnRep trials for positive words resulted in a larger LPP

compared to control trials indicating that prior presentation of a positive word resulted in

positive words being more prominent at later, more conscious stages of processing. No

difference between IgnRep and Control trials was observed for negative words, possibly

due to a lack of power. However most interestingly, the lack of a difference between

IgnRep and Control trials for negative words with regards to mean LPP amplitude was due

to a large LPP to Control trials for negative words. However, mean amplitude for negative

word (for both IgnRep and Control trials) within this time window was comparable to mean

amplitude on IgnRep trials for positive words. This indicates that more working memory

resources was deployed to negative material regardless of the previous distractor valence.

Therefore, while it is possible that the present study was simply not able to detect the

difference between IgnRep and Control trials, overall negative words require more

processing at later stages compared to positive and neutral words.

3.4.3. Behavioural

The ERP results are difficult to reconcile with the observed pattern of RTs.

Logically, increased salience of negative material should result in a faster RT on IgnRep

trials. However, no such effect is evident in the behavioural results for negative words.

Instead, RT is delayed on IgnRep trials relative to Control trials. In contrast, RTs on IgnRep

trials were much faster than RTs on Control trials suggesting the presence of a positive

distractor during the prime facilitated performance on the probe slide. However, no EAP

differences were observed between TrialType suggesting the positive distractor does not

affect the salience of a positive target during the probe slide at early stages of processing.

The lack of association between EAP amplitude and behavioural response measures in

not unusual, with previous studies indicating a similar pattern (see Taake, Jaspers-Fayer,

40

& Liotti, 2009). Logically, it is not entirely unsurprising given that the EAP represent early

stages of processing. Later stages of processing might be more influential in the response

selection process and thus, could override whatever effect the EAP may have had.

Therefore, the RT results might be best explained by the differences in the LPP to

positive and negative words. Given that the LPP is thought to index top-down processing

of emotional material it likely represents a combination of processes rather than a single,

discrete process. This is congruent with the temporal overlap between the LPP and P3

components (Hajcak, Weinberg, MacNamara, & Foti, 2012). Thus, the presence of the

LPP might require different explanations for positive and negative words. For positive

words, the larger amplitude to IgnRep trials might indicate increased resources allocated

to the processing of the previous distractor. This would assist the entry and maintenance

of positive material in WM and thus, facilitate a response. On the other hand, for negative

words, the enhanced LPP for IgnRep and Control trials might indicate increased resources

allocated to the suppression of the distractor, which would need to be overcome in order

for a response to be made. The increased effort would cause a delay and consequently

the increased NAP effect. These two explanations are consistent in that the amplitude of

the LPP represents the amount of WM resources allocated to the current target. However,

the exact processes these resources are allocated to, cannot be determined under the

current study design. This unfortunately means the above explanations are purely

speculative.

41

Chapter 4.

General Discussion

4.1. Implications

Based on the results from these studies it would appear that an affect based

explanation of the negative ruminatory cycle and depression is most accurate. Experiment

1 demonstrated that current symptom severity and not more stable, dispositional response

patterns account for group differences on the NAP task. This indicates that mood state

over the previous few weeks, is associated with altered processing of emotional material.

Somewhat surprisingly, more severe depressed symptoms were associated with a smaller

NAP for positive words and a larger NAP for negative words. This was interpreted as

evidence for compensatory mechanisms being engaged to counteract the negative mood

state. Thus, the experience of a negative mood in the recent past, predicted altered

performance on the NAP task.

Findings are consistent with an emotion regulation account of depression. Within

the sample of female university students without any history of depression or anxiety,

regulation of negative affect in particular was evident. Experiment 2 indicated increased

self-relevance of positive words (indicated by the larger EAP to both TrialTypes) which is

congruent with a general positive bias and avoidance of negative material (Koster, De

Lissnyder, Derakshan, & De Raedt, 2011). Further more, the lack of differences between

trial types were observed in later stages of processing (indexed by the LPP), suggesting

both conditions involved an equal amount of WM resources. It is possible that these

resources were engaged to remove or suppress negative material that is in WM because

it conflicts with an existent positive bias. The consequent suppression would make a

42

response more difficult and resulting in the slowed RT on IgnRep trials observed in both

experiments. In contrast, when the positive target was preceded by a positive distractor,

it elicited a larger LPP, indicating more WM resources were engaged. This could suggest

that positive material is more salient at later stages, and thus, more likely to reach

conscious awareness and therefore facilitate RT. With positive material more strongly

represented in WM, it is easier to make a response to a positive target resulting in the

faster RT on IgnRep trials. This would account for the behavioural pattern of results

observed in both experiments. Thus, the NAP effect and the ERP correlates observed in

the present studies, might reflect emotion regulation strategies to preserve a positive self-

image.

These results suggest that previous conclusions based on the NAP task may be

inaccurately focussing on altered processing of negative words in depression. In order to

distribute the effects of word valence across all trials, these studies directly contrasted

positive and negative words in their NAP calculations. Thus, on control trials, positive

words were always preceded by a negative distractor, and negative words by a positive

distractor. Following the logic used to explain the NAP effect, viewing an emotional item

produces a temporary mental set for the treatment of that type of item. For example, a

positive distractor would prime a mental set to ignore positive material which affects a

response to a subsequent positive target. Under The design described above, the NAP

effect is calculated as the difference between RTs to trials that require inhibition of the

same word valence and trials that require inhibition of the opposite word valence.

Therefore, the NAP effect measures a combination of inhibition for positive words, and

inhibition for negative words, rather than inhibition for any single word valence. The current

modified NAP task enabled the effect of inhibition for positive and negative words to be

dissociated more directly. Results from the experiment 1 suggests dysphoria is

43

characterized by changes in processing of both positive and negative material. Hence,

conclusions that depression relies only on reduced inhibition for negative words may need

to be revised. However, further research needs to be conducted in particular focussing on

the influence of negative affect on this variant of the NAP task, as well as the performance

of currently depressed patients.

4.2. Limitations and Future Research

It should be noted that the validity of the NAP calculations used in the present

study has not been determined. Indeed, there exists a confound of word valence since

negative targets are never preceded by a positive distractor and positive targets are never

preceded by a negative distracter. However, this design was chosen in order to fully

dissociate the effects of positive and negative material. To improve on this design, follow-

up studies should follow the lead of Frings, Wentura & Holtz (2007) by including a prime

distractor (PD) effect in the calculations. In other words, including RTs on trials where the

probe target is neutral and the prime distractor was positive in the NAP calculations for

negative words, and RTs on trials where the probe target was neutral and the prime

distractor was negative in the NAP calculations for positive words. This would account for

possible differences priming effects of each word valence. Additionally, a within-subjects

study should be conducted examining the effect of the different NAP calculations to

determine if indeed this might account for the differences between studies.

Underlying the discussion of the results thus far is the contentious assumption that

NP is caused by inhibitory processes. It is thought that the distractor and its attributes on

the prime slide is suppressed. The continuation of this suppression results in slowed

responses if a similar item is the target on the probe slide (Neill, 1977). However, this

Distractor Inhibition Account (DIA) of NP that still pervades the literature on the NAP effect

44

has largely been abandoned in more theoretical areas (Neill, Valdes, Terry & Gorfein,

1992) since experimental results are not consistent with theories of selective attention.

Primarily, NP magnitude diminishes as the interval between response to the prime and

presentation of the probe increases (Neill, Valdes, Terry & Gorfein, 1992). Also, when both

items on the prime need to be attended to in order to make a response, NP is still observed

(MacDonald, Joordens & Seergobin, 1999). Together this suggests that inhibition of the

distractor is not involved in NP. Additionally, NP only occurred when perceptual features

differed between the distractor and target, while positive priming (PP) occurred when the

perceptual features were the same (MacDonald & Joordens, 2000) which is inconsistent

with DIA. Consequently, several other interpretations have been proposed to account for

a wider range of situations where in NP is observed.

The most robust of these, the Episodic Retrieval Account (ERA) argues that the

delayed response results not from an attentional bias induced by the prime, but from the

comparison of the probe slide to the memory trace of its prime (Mayr & Buchner, 2007;

Milliken, Joordens, Merikle & Seiffert, 1998; Neill, Valdes, Terry & Gorfein, 1992). With the

episodic memory of the prime comes a host of other information points, including the

response that was made. Attended repetition (AttRep) trials contain a memory trace where

a similar target was previously responded to (MacDonald & Joordens, 2000). The current

response is simply a repeat of the last response, resulting in a significantly reduced

reaction time. On the other hand, on IgnRep trials the most salient memory trace (episodic

memory of the prime) is of an item where the current target was ignored and thus an

inappropriate response was encoded. This conflicts with the need to make a response on

the current trial and so, in order to resolve the conflict, the stimulus must be processed

anew. This takes time, and thus causes the NP effect (Mayr & Buchner, 2007). The fact

that the NP effect is strongest immediately following a response to the prime (response

45

stimulus interval (RSI) = 20-520ms) and decays over a period of 2s (Neill, Valdes, Terry

& Gorfein, 1992) supports such an explanation since the strength of the memory trace

would fade over time, thus reducing the conflict on IgnRep trials.

From an ERA perspective, the fact that most individuals tend to avoid negative

emotions (Koster, De Lissnyder, Derashan, & De Readt, 2011), might indicate that the

ignore tag is stronger for negative words and weaker for positive words. This might

account for the facilitation effect observed for positive words in the present study. It might

be the case that previous studies using the NAP effect were unable to pick up on this

difference due to the mutually depended nature of negative and positive words in their

NAP calculations. Such explanations are purely speculative at this time and further

research needs to be conducted before more precise conclusions can be reached. In

particular attempts should be made to clarify which explanation (inhibition or retrieval) best

accounts for the NAP effect.

46

References

Bertrams, A., & Dickhäuser, O. (2012). Passionate thinkers feel better: Self-control capacity as mediator of the relationship between need for cognition and affective adjustment. Journal of Individual Differences, 33(2), 69-75. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000081

Booy, R. M., & Liotti, M., (2015). State and trait influences on inhibition in pre-clinical depression. Simon Fraser University - Undergraduate journal of psychology, 2,1-14.

Bradley, M.M., & Lang, P.J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

Carter, J., & Garber, J. (2011). Predictors of the first onset of a major depressive episode and changes in depressive symptoms across adolescence: Stress and negative cognitions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(4), 779-796. doi:10.1037/a0025441

Cooney, R. E., Joormann, J., Eugène, F., Dennis, E. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Neural correlates of rumination in depression. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(4), 470-478. doi:10.3758/CABN.10.4.470

Dai, Q., & Feng, Z. (2011). Dysfunctional distracter inhibition and facilitation for sad faces in depressed individuals. Psychiatry Research, 190(2-3), 206-211. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.007

Dai, Q., Feng, Z., & Koster, E. W. (2011). Deficient distracter inhibition and enhanced facilitation for emotional stimuli in depression: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 249-258. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.10.016

Davis, R.N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24,699-711.

Dunn, B. R., Dunn, D. A., Languis, M., & Andrew, D. (1998). The Relation of ERP Components to complex memory processing. Brain and Cognition, 36, 355-376.

Eugène, F., Joormann, J., Cooney, R. E., Atlas, L. Y., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Neural correlates of inhibitory deficits in depression. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 181(1), 30-35. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.07.010

Frings, C., Wentura, D., & Holtz, M. (2007). Dysphorics cannot ignore unpleasant information. Cognition and Emotion, 21(7), 1525-1534. doi:10.1080/02699930601054042

47

Foti, D., Hajcak, G., (2009). Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology 81,1–8.

Foti, D., & Hajcak, G., (2010). State sadness reduces neural sensitivity to nonrewards versus rewards. NeuroReport 21,143-147.

Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. W. (2007). The influence of induced mood on the inhibition of emotional information. Motivation and Emotion, 31(3), 208-218. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9064-y

Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., & Koster, E. W. (2006). Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93(1-3), 149-157. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.007

Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6285-312. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305

Grzybowski, S. J., Wyczesany, M., & Kaiser, J. (2014). The influence of context on the processing of emotional and neutral adjectives—An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 99, 137-149. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.01.002

Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., & Olvet, D. M. (2010). Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotional regulation: An integrative review. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 129–155. doi: 10.1080/ 87565640903526504

Hajcak, G., Weinberg, A., MacNamara, A., & Foti, D. (2012). ERPs and the study of emotion. In Luck, S. J., & Kappenman, E. S., (1st), The Oxford handbook of event related potential components (pp. 441-474). New York, NY: Oxforn university Press.

Hertel, P. T. (1998). Relation between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 166-172. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.107.1.166

Joormann, J,. Yoon, K. L., & Zetche, U., (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and preventive psychology, 12, 128-139. doi:10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.002

Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 18(1), 125-147. doi:10.1080/02699930244000480

Joormann, J. (2006). Differential Effects of Rumination and Dysphoria on the Inhibition of Irrelevant Emotional Material: Evidence from a Negative Priming Task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30(2), 149-160. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9035-8

48

Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Updating the contents of working memory in depression: Interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1), 182-192. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.182

Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 281-298. doi:10.1080/02699930903407948

Koster, E. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138-145. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005

Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31, 291-308.

MacDonald, P. A., & Joordens, S. (2000). Investigation a memory-based account of negative priming: Support for selection-feature mismatch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(4), 1478-1496. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.26.4.1478

MacDonald, P. A., Joordens, S., & Seergobin, K. N. (1999). Negative priming effects that are bigger than a breadbox: Attention to distractors does not eliminate negative priming, it enhances it. Memory & Cognition, 27(2), 197-207. doi:10.3758/BF03211405

Mayr, S., & Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years of negative priming research. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie/Journal Of Psychology, 215(1), 35-51. doi:10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.35

Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., & Seiffert, A. E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105(2), 203-229. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.203

Moser, J. S., Hajcak, G., Bukay, E., & Simons, R. F. (2006). Intentional modulation of emotional responding to unpleasant pictures: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 43(3), 292-296. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00402.x

Neill, W. T. (1977). Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(3), 444-450. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.3.3.444

Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, And Cognition, 18(5), 993-1000. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.993

49

Nixon, E., Liddle, P. F., Nixon, N. L., & Liotti, M. (2013). On the interaction between sad mood and cognitive control: The effect of induced sadness on electrophysiological modulations underlying Stroop conflict processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(3), 313-326. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.11.014

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological bulletin, 101, 259-282.

Owens, M., Koster, E. W., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Impaired filtering of irrelevant information in dysphoria: An ERP study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(7), 752-763. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr050

Rawal, A., & Rice, F. (2012). Examining overgeneral autobiographical memory as a risk factor for adolescent depression. Journal of The American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(5), 518-527. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.02.025

Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention: Event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 123–143. Retrieved from http://nbn-resolving.de/ urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-58199

Wentura, D. (1999). Activation and inhibition of affective information: Evidence for negative priming in the evaluation task. Cognition and Emotion, 13(1), 65-91. doi:10.1080/026999399379375

Williams, L. M., Palmer, D., Liddell, B.J., Song, Le., & Gordon, E. (2006). The ‘when’ and ‘where’ of perceiving signals of threat versus non-threat. NeuroImage, 31(1), 458-467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.009

Yao, S., Liu, M., Liu, J., Hu, Z., Yi, J., & Huang, R. (2010). Inhibition dysfunction in depression: Event-related potentials during negative affective priming. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 182(2), 172-179. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.010

Yuan, J., Xu, S., Yang, J., Liu, Q., Chen, A., Zhu, L., & ... Li, H. (2011). Pleasant mood intensifies brain processing of cognitive control: ERP correlates. Biological Psychology, 87(1), 17-24. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.01.004

Zetsche, U., & Joormann, J., (2011). Components of interference control predict depressive symptoms and rumination cross-sectionally and at a six month follow-up. Journal of Behavioural Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 65-73. Doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.06.001

50

Appendix A. Questionnaires

Medical Questionnaire

Booy – LADN Participant ID:________

Medical Questionnaire

Demographics

Age: _____ Gender: Male / Female Dominant Hand: L eft/ Right

Country of Birth: _________________ Mother Tongue (First Language): _______________

Major: _________________ Current Year Post-secondary Education: _____________

How many hours per night do you typically sleep? _____________

How many hours did you sleep last night? _____________

Are you colour blind? Yes No

Do you require glasses/contacts? Yes No

Are you currently wearing glasses/contacts? Yes No

Medical and Psychiatric History:

Please indicate if you’ve ever been diagnosed/treated for any of the following:

Learning Disability Yes No

Attention Deficit Disorder Yes No

Depression Yes No

Anxiety Yes No

Epilepsy Yes No

Sleep Disorder Yes No

Please indicate if you’ve ever had any of the following scans:

Computed Tomography (CT) Yes No

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) Yes No

Electroencephalography (EEG) Yes No

Please indicate if you’ve ever experienced any of the following:

Loss of Motor Function Yes No

Loss of Sensory Function Yes No

Loss of Consciousness Yes No

Head Concussion Yes No

Migraines Yes No

51

NEO-PI-R

52

53

Appendix B. Word Lists

Po

siti

ve W

ord

sN

ega

tive

Wo

rds

Ne

utr

al W

ord

s

Wo

rdR

TA

CC

Len

gth

Val

en

ceA

rou

sal

Wo

rdR

TA

CC

Len

gth

Val

en

ceA

rou

salW

ord

RT

AC

CLe

ngt

hV

ale

nce

Aro

usa

l

acce

pt

867.

560.

816

7.89

5.40

abu

se78

6.31

0.88

51.

806.

83an

kle

879.

810.

885

5.27

4.16

adm

ire

792.

500.

886

7.74

6.11

add

ict

856.

190.

756

2.48

5.66

arm

s84

9.38

0.94

45.

343.

59

ado

re72

9.50

0.88

57.

815.

12ag

on

y79

1.50

0.94

52.

436.

06b

ann

er

908.

630.

886

5.40

3.83

agre

e97

2.38

0.88

57.

085.

02an

gry

707.

630.

886

2.85

7.17

bar

rel

947.

130.

756

5.05

3.36

be

auty

797.

751.

006

7.60

6.17

be

tray

749.

190.

886

1.68

7.24

be

nch

834.

560.

945

4.61

4.09

ble

ss75

5.50

0.81

57.

194.

05b

loo

dy

839.

880.

886

2.90

6.41

bla

nk

890.

440.

885

bli

ss83

0.00

0.81

56.

954.

41b

ruta

l82

7.69

0.88

62.

806.

60b

oar

d91

4.19

0.94

54.

823.

36

bra

ve87

0.75

0.69

57.

156.

15b

urd

en

894.

560.

886

2.50

5.63

bo

dy

993.

060.

756

5.55

5.52

bri

ght

720.

880.

946

7.50

5.40

can

cer

848.

880.

946

1.50

6.42

bo

wls

728.

690.

885

5.33

3.47

care

801.

500.

884

7.54

4.17

cow

ard

836.

690.

886

2.74

4.07

bu

tte

r86

8.88

1.00

65.

333.

17

cham

p93

4.88

0.63

57.

186.

00cr

ime

713.

190.

885

2.89

5.41

chai

r85

2.75

0.94

55.

083.

15

char

m77

9.94

0.88

56.

775.

16cr

isis

783.

250.

886

2.74

5.44

circ

le87

0.50

0.94

65.

673.

86

che

er

807.

310.

885

8.10

6.12

cru

el

710.

810.

945

1.97

5.68

clo

ck76

7.94

0.81

55.

144.

02

com

ed

y84

0.44

0.94

68.

375.

85d

ange

r88

2.94

0.88

62.

957.

32co

rne

r84

7.56

0.88

64.

363.

91

cud

dle

816.

000.

756

7.72

4.40

de

ad73

8.38

1.00

41.

945.

73cu

sto

m87

6.19

0.94

65.

854.

66

cute

669.

630.

944

7.62

5.53

de

mo

n72

3.19

0.75

52.

116.

76d

oo

rs78

8.00

0.88

55.

133.

80

daz

zle

880.

440.

756

7.29

6.33

de

test

811.

810.

816

2.17

6.06

elb

ow

788.

750.

815

5.12

3.81

de

vote

776.

440.

816

7.41

5.23

de

vil

784.

941.

005

2.20

6.07

en

gin

e81

1.81

0.81

65.

203.

98

en

joy

765.

000.

945

7.80

5.20

dre

ad84

4.13

1.00

52.

265.

84fa

bri

c91

3.81

0.94

65.

304.

14

favo

r75

4.81

0.63

56.

464.

54d

row

n83

4.75

0.81

51.

926.

57fa

rm83

4.88

0.88

45.

533.

90

fre

e78

7.56

0.75

47.

585.

52fa

il85

1.44

0.94

41.

704.

95fo

ot

752.

310.

944

5.02

3.27

frie

nd

819.

191.

006

8.43

5.11

fear

938.

940.

944

2.25

6.33

fork

s97

8.25

0.88

55.

293.

96

gen

tle

954.

060.

816

7.31

3.21

fou

l85

0.94

0.75

42.

814.

93h

amm

er

829.

250.

696

4.88

4.58

glo

ry91

6.69

0.81

57.

556.

02gr

ee

d89

2.56

0.81

53.

514.

71h

and

s89

7.19

0.94

55.

954.

40

goo

d84

9.69

0.88

47.

475.

43gr

ief

698.

130.

815

1.69

4.78

ice

bo

x89

7.19

1.00

64.

954.

17

hap

py

674.

381.

005

8.21

6.49

guil

ty91

8.75

0.81

62.

636.

04ir

on

748.

440.

944

4.90

3.76

ho

ne

st76

8.81

0.81

67.

705.

32h

ate

839.

880.

944

1.98

6.66

kett

le84

9.94

0.94

65.

223.

22

ho

no

r80

9.69

0.75

57.

665.

90h

orr

or

764.

190.

946

2.76

7.21

kno

ts85

0.31

0.81

54.

644.

07

ho

pe

809.

000.

884

7.10

5.78

hu

rt88

6.00

0.94

41.

905.

85la

mp

733.

880.

814

5.41

3.80

hu

mo

r83

4.56

0.88

58.

565.

50in

sult

753.

130.

816

2.29

6.00

lock

er

798.

940.

886

5.19

3.38

joke

860.

750.

754

8.10

6.74

jail

815.

250.

814

1.95

5.49

mar

ket

838.

000.

816

5.66

4.12

54

Po

siti

ve W

ord

sN

ega

tive

Wo

rds

Ne

utr

al W

ord

s

Wo

rdR

TA

CC

Len

gth

Val

en

ceA

rou

sal

Wo

rdR

TA

CC

Len

gth

Val

en

ceA

rou

salW

ord

RT

AC

CLe

ngt

hV

ale

nce

Aro

usa

l

joll

y76

2.00

0.81

57.

415.

57ki

lle

r62

8.56

1.00

61.

897.

86m

eta

l98

8.31

0.81

54.

953.

79

joyf

ul

695.

311.

006

8.22

5.89

liar

827.

441.

004

2.79

5.96

mo

nth

1007

.56

0.81

55.

154.

03

kin

d71

3.88

0.81

47.

594.

46lo

ne

ly91

4.81

0.88

62.

174.

51n

ame

791.

250.

944

5.55

4.25

kiss

907.

000.

884

8.26

7.32

lose

r77

6.13

0.94

52.

254.

95n

ew

s90

7.31

0.75

45.

305.

17

lau

gh84

0.63

0.75

58.

456.

75m

ise

ry89

5.44

0.81

61.

935.

17p

ain

t92

8.63

0.88

55.

624.

10

live

ly81

0.88

0.81

67.

205.

53m

urd

er

743.

560.

886

1.53

7.47

pap

er

746.

690.

945

5.20

2.50

love

706.

751.

005

8.64

6.38

pai

n75

8.00

0.94

42.

136.

50p

en

cil

920.

750.

946

5.22

3.14

loya

l75

0.88

0.88

57.

555.

16p

ois

on

780.

380.

886

1.98

6.05

pla

nt

848.

380.

755

5.98

3.62

luck

y81

1.69

0.81

58.

176.

53p

riso

n81

5.00

1.00

62.

055.

70ro

cks

862.

190.

945

5.56

4.52

me

rry

851.

690.

885

7.90

5.90

rage

800.

690.

884

2.41

8.17

ship

s95

3.69

0.81

55.

554.

38

nic

e76

6.94

0.88

46.

554.

38ra

pe

805.

810.

944

1.25

6.81

sph

ere

827.

381.

006

5.33

3.88

par

ty83

6.63

0.81

57.

866.

69re

gre

t80

4.38

0.81

62.

285.

74sp

ray

879.

880.

885

5.45

4.50

pe

ace

821.

060.

885

7.72

2.95

reje

ct76

2.56

0.94

61.

506.

37sq

uar

e87

2.06

0.81

64.

743.

18

ple

ase

934.

000.

636

8.28

5.74

rott

en

879.

060.

816

2.26

4.53

stat

ue

755.

880.

816

5.17

3.46

po

we

r86

4.56

0.63

56.

546.

67ru

de

779.

750.

884

2.50

6.31

sto

ve92

8.00

0.75

54.

984.

51

pre

tty

759.

130.

886

7.75

6.03

scar

e79

5.94

0.94

52.

786.

82st

ree

t78

3.44

0.81

65.

223.

39

pro

ud

846.

500.

945

8.03

5.56

sham

e86

9.25

0.75

52.

504.

88ta

ble

941.

940.

945

5.22

2.92

rew

ard

930.

060.

946

7.53

4.95

sick

807.

380.

754

1.90

4.29

tan

k74

6.44

0.75

45.

164.

88

safe

859.

190.

814

7.07

3.86

sin

ful

947.

500.

946

2.93

6.29

taxi

884.

130.

884

5.00

3.41

secu

re94

1.88

0.63

67.

573.

14sl

ave

785.

690.

815

1.84

6.21

ten

ts84

6.63

0.81

5

sexy

817.

940.

814

8.02

7.36

stre

ss79

4.88

0.75

62.

097.

45th

eo

ry10

46.4

40.

886

5.30

4.62

soci

al90

1.63

0.75

66.

884.

98st

up

id88

4.06

0.81

62.

314.

72ti

me

1038

.63

0.69

45.

314.

64

stro

ng

797.

250.

946

7.11

5.92

thie

f80

1.38

0.88

52.

136.

89to

ol

910.

310.

944

5.19

4.33

tale

nt

870.

940.

756

7.56

6.27

toxi

c84

8.94

0.88

52.

106.

40to

we

r91

8.00

0.88

55.

463.

95

ten

de

r87

9.13

0.69

66.

934.

88tr

agic

779.

940.

946

1.78

6.24

tru

cks

821.

630.

946

5.09

4.18

than

k76

8.38

0.94

56.

894.

34tr

aum

a74

8.06

0.81

62.

106.

33u

nit

800.

440.

884

5.59

3.75

tru

st89

2.69

0.88

56.

685.

30u

gly

762.

190.

754

2.43

5.38

vio

lin

994.

940.

816

5.43

3.49

tru

th80

1.31

0.81

57.

805.

00va

nd

al82

3.56

0.81

62.

716.

40w

ago

n84

2.75

0.88

55.

373.

98

virt

ue

871.

440.

816

6.22

4.52

vom

it80

8.44

0.88

62.

065.

75w

atch

936.

940.

885

5.78

4.10

win

ne

r77

3.44

0.94

68.

387.

72w

ho

re77

6.69

0.94

52.

305.

85w

ind

859.

880.

814

5.60

3.74

wis

e83

3.00

0.81

47.

523.

91w

icke

d80

5.31

0.88

62.

966.

09w

ind

ow

794.

000.

816

5.91

3.97

Ave

rage

826.

380.

835.

197.

565.

47A

vera

ge80

6.80

0.87

5.32

2.19

6.07

Ave

rage

883.

370.

855.

035.

353.

95