chess_com (6)

download chess_com (6)

of 6

Transcript of chess_com (6)

  • 7/25/2019 chess_com (6)

    1/6

    Why Chess Players BlunderVik-Hansen |Feb 9, 2016 |2715 views |13 commentsWe have seen them, havent we? The howlers? Amateurs and

    professionals alike in the aftermath of a game, trying to

    explain their blunder, shaking their heads in disbelief,

    scratching their brows, sighing while trying to come up with a

    rational explanation.

    Born out of recent findings from the field of consciousness and

    mind, the article seeks to explain that chess playing is based

    upon a fine interplay between a mind subconsciously

    triggering moves, and a well-disciplined consciousness

    knowing what to keep and what to discard.

    Unlike other play-by-play-articles, this first part aspires to go behind the moves to unravel the

    conundrum underlying our blunders and the second proposes a methodfor fighting impulsiveness

    and blunder-tendencies.

    .

    Who is doing the thinking?Playing chess is often linked to thinking and Descartes Cogito Ergo Sum, I think, therefore I am,

    has long since cast its spell on us regarding our beliefs in our mental life.

    Concerning the nature of thinking, in the chapter The Stream of Thought( The Principles of

    Psychology, 1890), William James emphasized that consciousness always chooses; it is always

    more interested in one part of its object than another, and welcomes and rejects, or chooses, all

    the while it thinks (James quoted by Nrretranders, 1998: 176):.

    The mind is at every stage a theatre of simultaneous possibilities. Consciousness consists in the

    comparison of these with each other, the selection of some, and the suppression of the rest by the

    reinforcing and inhibiting agency of attention. The highest and most elaborated mental products are

    filtered from the data chosen by the faculty next beneath, out of the mass offered by the faculty

    below that, which mass in turn was sifted from a still larger amount of yet simpler material, and so

    on.

    The mind, in short, works on the data it receives very much as a sculptor works on his block of

    stone. In a sense the statue stood there from eternity. But there were a thousand different ones

    beside it, and the sculptor alone is to thank for having extricated this one from the rest.

    Just so the world of each of us, how so ever different our several views of it may be, all lay

    ^ Ads keep Chess.com free. Upgrade to remove ads! ^

    ^ Ads keep Chess.com free. Upgrade to remove ads! ^

    SearchSign Up - It's Free!Log In

    PLAY LEARN SHARE FORUMS MORE

    Most Recent Articles

    Why Chess Players Blunder

    by Vik-Hansen

    8 hours ago

    Not Used To It

    by JoseDiaz

    48 hours ago

    Who Loved These Knight Moves?

    by GMGserper

    2 days ago

    Castling Early And Piece Coordination

    by IMSilman

    5 days ago

    Surprise Mating Attacks

    by batgirl

    6 days ago

    https://www.chess.com/article/view/not-used-to-ithttps://www.chess.com/members/view/Vik-Hansenhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttp://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_2/membership.html?c=adshttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/batgirlhttps://www.chess.com/article/view/surprise-mating-attackhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/Silmanhttp://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_2/members/titled_playershttps://www.chess.com/article/view/castling-early-and-piece-coordinationhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/Gserperhttp://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_2/members/titled_playershttps://www.chess.com/article/view/mister-ng5-and-nb5https://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/JoseDiazhttps://www.chess.com/article/view/not-used-to-ithttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/Vik-Hansenhttp://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_2/membership.html?c=adshttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttp://localhost/var/www/apps/conversion/tmp/scratch_2/members/view/Vik-Hansenhttps://www.chess.com/sitemaphttps://www.chess.com/forum/https://www.chess.com/sitemap#sharehttps://www.chess.com/learnhttps://www.chess.com/play/https://www.chess.com/loginhttps://www.chess.com/registerhttps://www.chess.com/
  • 7/25/2019 chess_com (6)

    2/6

    embedded in the primordial chaos of sensations, which gave the mere matterto the thought of all

    of us indifferently. We may, if we like, by our reasonings unwind things back to that black and

    jointless continuity of space and moving clouds of swarming atoms which science calls the only real

    world.

    But all the while the world we feel and live in will be that which our ancestors and we, by slowly

    cumulative strokes of choice, have extricated out of this, like sculptors, by simply removing portions

    of the given stuff. Other sculptors, other statues from the same stone! Other minds, other worlds

    from the same monotonous and inexpressive chaos!

    My world is but one in a million alike embedded, alike real to those who may abstract them. How

    different must be the worlds in the consciousness of ant, cuttlefish, or crab!' (James, quoted by

    Nrretranders, 1998: 177).

    .

    In 1901 and 1905, K. Marbe and H.J. Watt respectively, confirmed that thinking and judging, the

    supposed hallmarks of consciousness (Jaynes: 2000: 36-41) are not conscious at all; i.e. the

    (conscious) I is not the thinking substance Descartes (and later Kant) took it to be.

    In other words, we do our thinking before we know what we are to think about (Jaynes, 2000:

    39), we do not know what we are thinking before were thinking it and [...] the actual process of

    thinking, so usually thought to be the very life of consciousness, is not conscious at all and that

    only its preparation, its materials, and its end result are consciously perceived (Jaynes, 2000:

    41).

    The preparation, materials and end results being the only things consciously perceived does notimply they areconscious, because if they were, apparently, we could control the quality of the

    thinking process by consciously choosing the best preparations and materials for the actual

    thought processes. In chess, the chessboard is your marble block; figuratively speaking, your mind

    carves out moves from and presents to your consciousness. Positions with several equal moves?

    Different sculptor, i.e. player, different move.

    Since the end of the 1950s it has been known that from all the information flooding through our

    sense organs (1,121,000 bits), only a fraction (1-16 bits) makes up a conscious experience

    (Nrretranders, 1998: 124-126) and in 1988 Hans H. Kornhuber, echoing James and Jaynes, states

    thinking as independent of consciousness and acts of volition and that most of the information

    passing through our central nervous system is subconscious, suggesting we cannot think what we

    want, but that we might be able to direct the focus of our attention (Nrretranders, 1998: 177).

    As it suggests we by acts of volition can focus or direct our attention towards what we shouldfocus on or direct it towards, focussing our attention is less straightforward than what may be

    assumed, since there would be no reasons not to focus on or direct our attention towards what we

    should focus on or direct it towards. Concretely, this means that the quality of our thoughts,

    feelings and actions depends of the quality of the information flowing through our sense organs

    into the brain and the brain's capacity and capability to analyse and process the information

    before sent out as thoughts, feelings and actions.

    However, the possibility of directing our attention might not be as straightforward as first

    thought: the phrase of having our attention or interest caught implies that something outside our

    consciousness does the catching, i.e. we do not decide what to be interested in, what to desire,

    urge or crave, nor as to what to direct our attention to. We cant turn our head until we are

    made aware of that we can turn it. We do not have to turn our head, but to be able at all to turn

    it; an impulse making us aware of the possibility must subconsciously be triggered or presented toour consciousness.

    .

  • 7/25/2019 chess_com (6)

    3/6

    Who is doing the playing?

    Until Benjamin Libets ground-breaking experiment (1983) showed that any apparent act of

    volition normally begins subconsciously with studies showing that the brain subconsciously prepare

    actions before consciousness is informed (Nrretranders, 1998: 213-256), it was assumed was that

    we ourselves, consciously, decide when to move and what to move (pieces, arms, fingers, legs).In the wake of his experiment, Libet suggested that consciousness despite not being able to

    initiate actions could veto, abort or stop impulses from running to action where chess playing

    does not differ from any other kinds or types of actions.This implies that, contrary to popular and

    traditional beliefs, consciousness cannot trigger or initiate actions but can, if disciplined, veto,

    or abort, impulses leading to unwanted, awkward, unacceptable, unfortunate, embarrassing or

    immoral actions.

    Actions starting subconsciously further suggest that the only thing we consciously can do

    deliberately or on purpose is to veto and thus reveals an inherent ambiguity in the phrase

    on purpose (deliberately). Since consciousness cannot trigger actions, we cannot

    inadvertently, unintentionally, un-purposefully or un-deliberately commit actions or play moves

    we didnt mean to.

    The brain does work in mysterious ways not needing any reason or justification for triggering this

    or that impulse and does it not see a purpose, it will not trigger the impulse and there no other

    way to act. In short: if consciousness triggered actions, slip-ups would never happen or occur.

    Thinking being subconscious, moves are triggered by the brain and consciousness working by the

    veto, chess playing (and human activities in general) is left in the hands of the fine-tuned

    interplay between conscious and subconscious processes (rather than a definite and isolated self,

    acting out from nowhere, so to speak); knowing what to keep and what to discard among all the

    suggestions, whims and ideas with which the mind comes up with.

    Consciousness more or less functions as blunder check, lightly monitoring our play, making sure

    no pieces are left hanging or put en prise. Most of the time when playing, consciousness is not

    involved at all; Romanovsky writing that A maneuvering game can also sometimes arise from

    the conscious efforts of one of the opponents (Romanovsky, 2013: 201) may serve as an example

    as to how entrenched the belief in the role of consciousness when playing chess is.

    Were we to choose our moves consciously, how do we explain or account for our blunders and

    mistakes? Were chess playing conscious, we would never make mistakes since nobody blunders

    (consciously) on purpose. Simply by acts of volition, we could decide to play the best moves as the

    chess board in front of us would yield access to full information; we would have the entire

    overview of what is going on since consciousness would be transparent, and the position on the

    board would be there for everyone to see.

    Traditionally, chess games are explained and moves attempted justified in the analyses after the

    game and this is usually the order of the day; first playthen explanation. If chess playing were

    conscious, logically, it should be the other way around; first we explain why certain moves

    are to be played and then the brain triggers the requested moves, right?

    If we could give perfectly viable and reasonable explanations for every move we make, why wouldour brain trigger a blunder or not produce or come up with moves best fitting the explanation

    thus making chess the rational game it is perceived to be?

    The Russian proverb -- We are all satisfied with our reason but not with our position -- nicely

    captures this apparent paradox. In blitz and rapid games, where consciousness is almost absent,

    these kinds of games are merely perception and intuition, this being even more apparent since

  • 7/25/2019 chess_com (6)

    4/6

    there is no time to ponder possible explanations before a move is to be triggered.

    .

    Who is blundering?

    Blunders might be perceived as some sort of spontaneously ill-conceived move-suggestions, i.e.

    impulses to moves which would be detrimental to ones position if not aborted. However, we are

    not talking about strategically weak moves on a general level, like misplacing a piece due to lack

    of general chess ability and understanding but moves literally occurring out of nowhere, moves

    there apparently are no sensible reasons to play. The key question is; if consciousness does not do

    the playing, then, who does the blundering? Someone or something must be responsible for

    players blundering, and who or what part of us might that be?

    As mentioned, a l ight consciousness monitors while playing, whereas full consciousness announces

    itself the moment a chess player blunders, which his/her body language just too well illustrates.

    Note the order; never do we come across players announcing their blunders in advance; we only

    hear about the ones that blundered first, and thenbecame aware of it.

    This time, only briefly can we touch upon the whys and hows of blunders but as a general

    pointer, we might say that blunders occur due to lack of interplay between brain and

    consciousness and seem to have only three possible explanations:

    1) We take in only parts of the position due to inadequate vision, focussing only on certain

    parts of the board.

    2) We take in the whole position but something happens while processing the material

    resulting in apparently spontaneous and inexplicable blunders.

    3) Even when seeing the whole board, our brain does not take it all in.

    The first explanation might be the most clear-cut, implying that inadequate focus results in lack

    of information and thus absence of interplay between the brain and mind resulting in being

    consciously unable to abort the impulse. Mistakes in this department might be caused both due to

    fatigue but also due to lack of general chess ability and experience. Differently put: GMs might

    fall victim to these kind of blunder due to fatigue rather than lack of proficiency, whereas

    amateurs might suffer from it both because of fatigue and lack of chess skills.

    Regarding the second explanation, blunders are something we try to avoid, so if blunders have

    anything to do with what we take in, why would the brain process the material in such a way that

    it leads to blunders? This seems to happen only if there is a problem with the wiring so to

    speak, which is conceivable if not too frequent. Having consciousness purposefully misinterpret

    the information seems to lead to a conflict of interests since the goal of playing chess is to mate

    your opponent and why would you want to mess things up for yourself?

    The plot thickens when arriving at the third explanation, valid for both amateurs and

    professionals, raising a timely dilemma: it would seem impossible to blunder when seeing the

    whole board with our own two eyes, right? Wrong! This reason for blundering is closely linked to

    our point about the order in which chess is played and explained and research shows that only a

    fraction of all information passing through our eyes is perceived by consciousness, implying that

    we might see the whole board and still notperceive it.This means there might be chunks ofinformation our brain does not take in or misses even when our eyes physically are seeing the

    board.

    Solving tactical puzzles or playing the guessing game where we try to guess a GMs moves, may

    illustrate the difference between seeing and perceiving. The whole board is in front of us we

    see (eyes directed towards) the position clearly all information for solving it is right in front of

  • 7/25/2019 chess_com (6)

    5/6

    Comments

    us and still, many a time, we end up face-palming ourselves. How could we miss that? However,

    when the solution is presented to us, its the most obvious thing in the world.

    Despite purists considering tactics the sophism of chess, how fast you find tactical solutions tells

    you how quickly your brain processes the information, i.e. the position. Perception being

    subconscious, there is no point in beating ourselves (or others) up for not seeing this tactical shot

    or missing that beautiful combination.

    Amateurs and professionals literallyperceivedifferent boards even when seeing the same one and

    the reason is that the professional mind is better trained to perceive more information than the

    amateur mind.This is so since perception is not based on acts of volition and the brain works

    independently of what we think it should perceive, think, understand, comprehend or interpretetc. Blunders happen because humans are fallible and ill-founded impulses prove stronger than

    our ability to abort them.

    Our next installment presents a method for combatting blunders....and more.

    .

    Bibliography

    Jaynes, J., The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind, Mariner Books,

    2000

    Nrretranders, T., The User Illusion, Penguin Books, 1999

    Romanovsky, P., Soviet Middlegame Technique, Quality Chess, 2013

    26 minutes agoprunebiscuit

    This is brilliant. Thank you

    88 minutes agoHaggard-CC

    100 minutes agokkmkr

    well thought out article!

    101 minutes agoenotSgnilloR

    Because we're human.

    2 hours agokajalbasak

    very nice! waiting for next part.

    2 hours agoklimski

    If you enjoyed this, as I did, you will enjoy the book I am currently reading which delves

    further into this matter: Move first, think later. By Willy Hendriks. Highly recommended

    and fun!

    3 hours agoNightCrawler2003

    Awesome!

    5 hours agoadelsonpr

    https://www.chess.com/members/view/adelsonprhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/NightCrawler2003https://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/klimskihttps://www.chess.com/members/view/kajalbasakhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/enotSgnilloRhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/kkmkrhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/Haggard-CChttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/prunebiscuit
  • 7/25/2019 chess_com (6)

    6/6

    Back to Top

    Post your reply:

    Excellent article. Are you a psychologist? It is unusual to know this type of information if

    you are not a PhD in Psychology or Neuroscience.

    Thank you very much for sharing and Im looking forward the second part.

    6 hours agoslowfooted23x

    After having thought so deep, a player may be a move late before coming to the present

    reality at the chessboard. Hence, en prise pieces are left hanging. Alexander Kotov

    talked about this in his book Think Like a Grandmaster.

    6 hours agoAtlas_T

    6 hours ago4Dchessonline

    Fantastic article! I was working on similar lines myself but this covers more than I have

    achieved so far. Thanks. Look forward to the next installment.

    7 hours agogargelfuh

    Wow - great stuff. Thank you

    7 hours agotondeaf

    holy cow. best article ever here.

    7 hours agovalen1949

    Great article, Thanks

    Log In or Join

    Home | Why Join | Chess Topics | About | FAQs | Help & Support | Site Map

    Privacy Policy | Legal | 2016 Chess.com Chess - English

    https://www.chess.com/legal#termsofservicehttps://www.chess.com/legal#privacypolicyhttps://www.chess.com/sitemaphttps://www.chess.com/helphttp://support.chess.com/https://www.chess.com/abouthttps://www.chess.com/topicshttps://www.chess.com/whyhttps://www.chess.com/https://play.google.com/store/apps/details?id=com.chesshttp://itunes.apple.com/app/chess-play-learn/id329218549?mt=8http://www.youtube.com/wwwChesscomhttp://twitter.com/chesscomhttps://www.facebook.com/chesshttps://www.chess.com/registerhttps://www.chess.com/login_and_go?returnUrl=https%3A%2F%2Fwww.chess.com%2Farticle%2Fview%2Fwhy-chess-players-blunderhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/valen1949https://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/tondeafhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/gargelfuhhttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/4Dchessonlinehttps://www.chess.com/membership?c=iconhttps://www.chess.com/members/view/Atlas_Thttps://www.chess.com/members/view/slowfooted23xhttp://-/?-