Bird song and singing behavior

24
1 Bird song and singing behavior Heather Williams Williams College http://www.williams.edu/Biology/Faculty_Staff/hwilliams/Finches/ The flowers appear on the earth; the time of the singing of birds is come, and the voice of the turtle dove is heard in our land. - Song of Songs Sumer is icumen in, Lhude sing cuccu! - Anon The ousel-cock so black of hue With orange-tawny bill, The throstle with his note so true, The wren with little quill; The finch, the sparrow, and the lark … - Shakespeare Bird song has never lacked for admirers drawn by its aesthetic qualities. But to the scientist, bird song is also of interest because it represents an evolutionary flowering of vocal learning. Among mammals, vocal learning - the imitation of sounds used in communication – is extremely rare, having been demonstrated in one primate species (humans), the cetaceans (whales and dolphins), and two bat species (there is also some evidence for vocal learning in pinnipeds). In contrast, almost 5,000 species of birds, distributed among three groups, learn their songs or calls: parrots (350+ species), hummingbirds (300+ species), and oscine songbirds. (The oscines, a suborder of the Passeriformes, have a worldwide distribution and demonstrate vocal learning. Taxonomically, the oscines are defined by the complex musculature of the vocal organ, the syrinx.) Since many of these bird species are relatively easy to maintain in the lab, sing readily, and are diurnal, the pre-eminence of bird song as a model system is understandable. Beginning in the early 1950s at Cambridge University, William Thorpe’s and Peter Marler’s work on the development of bird songs and alarm calls led the way to a systematic analysis of song behavior, yielding a bountiful harvest of insights into the stages of song learning, critical periods, the role of auditory models for song, and song dialects. As understanding of the nuances of birdsong deepened and grew richer, the next step was to begin use experimental approaches to ask questions about the neural bases and correlates of vocal learning and communication. Songbirds (and their cousins, the parrots and hummingbirds) offer several advantages in their attempt to understand the brain processes that underlie complex learned behavior. First is the song itself. Because adult song is a complex sound repeated in a stereotyped manner, it can be easily recorded, quantified, and analyzed. Since young birds imitate the songs of adults, it is easy to follow the learning process by recording the tutor’s song and successive versions of the young bird’s song. As a signal used in communication, song is affected by the conditions and social context the singer experiences – and, in turn, song affects the conspecific listeners’ subsequent behaviors; all of these factors can also be observed and quantified. The second major advantage is the ease with which songbirds can be managed. Several species have been domesticated and can readily be raised and studied in the laboratory; some may produce three

Transcript of Bird song and singing behavior

Page 1: Bird song and singing behavior

1

Bird song and singing behavior

Heather WilliamsWilliams Collegehttp://www.williams.edu/Biology/Faculty_Staff/hwilliams/Finches/

The flowers appear on the earth;the time of the singing of birds is come,and the voice of the turtle dove is heard in our land.- Song of Songs

Sumer is icumen in, Lhude sing cuccu!- Anon

The ousel-cock so black of hue With orange-tawny bill,The throstle with his note so true, The wren with little quill;The finch, the sparrow, and the lark …- Shakespeare

Bird song has never lacked for admirers drawn by its aestheticqualities. But to the scientist, bird song is also of interest becauseit represents an evolutionary flowering of vocal learning. Amongmammals, vocal learning - the imitation of sounds used incommunication – is extremely rare, having been demonstrated inone primate species (humans), the cetaceans (whales anddolphins), and two bat species (there is also some evidence forvocal learning in pinnipeds). In contrast, almost 5,000 species ofbirds, distributed among three groups, learn their songs or calls:

parrots (350+ species), hummingbirds (300+ species), and oscinesongbirds. (The oscines, a suborder of the Passeriformes, have aworldwide distribution and demonstrate vocal learning.Taxonomically, the oscines are defined by the complexmusculature of the vocal organ, the syrinx.) Since many of thesebird species are relatively easy to maintain in the lab, singreadily, and are diurnal, the pre-eminence of bird song as amodel system is understandable.

Beginning in the early 1950s at Cambridge University,William Thorpe’s and Peter Marler’s work on the development ofbird songs and alarm calls led the way to a systematic analysis ofsong behavior, yielding a bountiful harvest of insights into thestages of song learning, critical periods, the role of auditorymodels for song, and song dialects. As understanding of thenuances of birdsong deepened and grew richer, the next step wasto begin use experimental approaches to ask questions about theneural bases and correlates of vocal learning andcommunication.

Songbirds (and their cousins, the parrots and hummingbirds)offer several advantages in their attempt to understand the brainprocesses that underlie complex learned behavior. First is thesong itself. Because adult song is a complex sound repeated in astereotyped manner, it can be easily recorded, quantified, andanalyzed. Since young birds imitate the songs of adults, it is easyto follow the learning process by recording the tutor’s song andsuccessive versions of the young bird’s song. As a signal used incommunication, song is affected by the conditions and socialcontext the singer experiences – and, in turn, song affects theconspecific listeners’ subsequent behaviors; all of these factorscan also be observed and quantified. The second majoradvantage is the ease with which songbirds can be managed.Several species have been domesticated and can readily beraised and studied in the laboratory; some may produce three

Page 2: Bird song and singing behavior

2

generations each year. The third advantage is that the neuralcircuitry which mediates singing behavior is well described(Reiner et al., 2004), and there is a growing recognition that theavian brain is based on the same organizational scheme as that ofmammals (Ferries, 2004; see also Ferries and Perkel, this volume).Finally, methods and databases that harness the powerful tools ofmolecular biology for understanding genetic mechanisms are inplace for studying the acquisition and expression of singingbehavior, and the genome of one songbird will soon besequenced (Clayton, 2004; see Arnold and Clayton, this volume).Thus bird song is currently the best – or perhaps the only – modelsystem that allows the use of neural, genetic, and molecularanalyses to be brought to bear on the study of a complexcommunication system that is learned by imitating a model.

This chapter focuses upon the culmination of vocal learning,the songs and calls of adult birds. Even the simplest and moststereotyped of songs has levels of complexity that can potentiallymuddy the waters of interpretation. However, this verycomplexity, evolved by the animals in their natural environment,allows bird song to serve as a model for a variety of phenomenathat cannot be investigated using behavioral protocols designed(by humans) specifically for use in the lab.

The diversity of bird songs

Much of what we know about how bird song is related to thebird brain is based upon the study of a very small subset ofsongbirds (see Kaplan, this volume). One species (the zebrafinch, Taeniopygia guttata) accounts for approximately half of allstudies of the neural basis of vocal learning, and the second mostpopular subject, the canary (Serinus canaria) represents a further14% of such studies. Six additional species (European starlingsSturnus vulgaris, song sparrows Melospiza melodia, white-crowned sparrows Zonotrichia leucophrys, brown-headedcowbirds Molothrus ater, swamp sparrows Melospiza georgiana,and Bengalese finches, Lonchura domestica) account for another

19% of the studies of vocal learning in songbirds. More than two-thirds of all such studies use one of three domesticated species(zebra finches, canaries, and Bengalese finches) as subjects; thesespecies breed readily in captivity and are not stressed by humanpresence and the laboratory environment. While some wild-caught birds (e.g., European starlings, black-capped chickadeesParus atricapillus) can adapt to captivity, other species may notunless they are captured as nestlings and hand-reared (a difficult,time-consuming process). Hence the choice of the primaryspecies used for laboratory-based studies of vocal learning is,understandably, most often based upon criteria other than theproperties of the song.

For most songbird species, only the males sing. However,females may sing as much as males in duetting species such asthe bay wren (Thryothorus nigricapillus; Farabaugh, 1982).Female canaries do not sing during the spring breeding season,but may sing quietly during the fall (Pesch and Güttinger, 1985);female zebra finches do not sing, and differences in the songcircuitry of males and females are correlated with the differencesin singing behavior (Nottebohm and Arnold, 1976; see alsoBolhuis, this volume). Not surprisingly, male bird songs andbrains have been the primary focus of research on the basis ofsong learning, although comparative studies of females have andwill continue to provide insights about hormonal influences onand brain mechanisms for song behavior and learning.

A bias towards studying males of domesticated songbirdspecies may have affected our current view of vocal learning,since it is all too easy to generalize from male zebra finches (orcanaries) to “birds”. Fortuitously, the songs of the two mostcommonly used species – the canary and the zebra finch – havedifferent structures as well as different learning trajectories. Evenso, these two species’ songs cannot begin to span the full rangeof avian vocal learning. Neither zebra finches nor canaries aremimics; adults of species adept at mimicry (such as themockingbird Mimus polyglottus and mynah Gracula religiosa),

Page 3: Bird song and singing behavior

3

can produce accurate imitations after hearing a single example ofa sound. “Action-based learning”, the pruning of excess songsfrom the repertoire at the end of the song learning period (Marler,1997), is important for species such as song sparrows and swampsparrows, but does not appear to occur in zebra finches orcanaries. Including mimics and birds that exhibit action-basedlearning among the species used for studies of the neural basisvocal learning might yield both new insights and new questions.

The song learning process

The primary impetus for studying bird song is its value as amodel for vocal learning with parallels to human speech. Songlearning has several stages (Marler, 1981): first, a sensory learningperiod, during which the young male listens to and memorizesthe song of a socially salient adult model; second, a subsongstage, akin to the babbling of human infants, during which theyoung male produces sounds and listens to the results, calibratinghis vocal instrument; third, plastic song, during which the youngmale adjusts his song to approximate the memorized model;finally, crystallization, when the song becomes fixed in its adultform and the components of the song and the order in which theyare sung become stereotyped (see Hultsch and Todt, this volume).These stages may be separate and distinct during development, ormay overlap (particularly in the case of the first and secondstages). For seasonally breeding birds such as canaries, the stagesof song learning usually begin during the summer, soon afterfledging (when young birds leave the nest) and crystallizationoccurs the following spring, when young males are ready to breedfor the first time (Nottebohm et al., 1986). In contrast, birds thatare not seasonal breeders may complete all of the stages of songlearning during a more compressed period; zebra finches reachsexual maturity at about 90 days, and song crystallizes at thattime (Immelmann, 1969; Price, 1979). Species also differ inwhether song is fixed after it crystallizes during the first year (incritical-period learners, e.g. zebra finches and Savannah

sparrows, Passerculus sandwichiensis) or whether new songs maybe learned each year (in open-ended learners, e.g. canaries).Notwithstanding these species differences in the timing andrepetition of song learning, the four classic stages (sensorylearning, subsong, plastic song, and crystallization) are readilyapparent in the developmental trajectories of most songbirds.

What material do young birds learn? Most often, it is thesounds and structure of the songs their parent(s) and/or neighborssing. Some species, such as the canary and white-crownedsparrow, will copy songs played through speakers. Others, suchas the zebra finch, must have an interactive relationship with thesong model, either in the form of an adult bird (Price, 1979) or akey that is pecked to trigger a song playback (Adret, 1983;Bolhuis et al., 1999; Tchernichovski et al., 2001; see Saar et al.,this volume). In species that learn from tape recordings (such asthe chaffinch Fringilla coelebs and white-crowned sparrow),young males prefer to imitate the adults with which they interact,even if they first encounter singing males well after the time theywould normally have crystallized their songs (Petrinovitch andBaptista, 1987; Nelson, 1998). Song development is also guidedby innate predispositions (Marler, 1989). Young male sparrowsthat are deafened or deprived of the opportunity to listen to anyadult tutor (live or recorded) during song development developsongs that deviate substantially from a normal song yet still retainsome characteristics of the species-typical song (Marler andSherman, 1983 and 1985). Hand-reared young male chaffinchesexposed only to recorded songs from a variety of species tend tocopy the songs with conspecific properties (Thorpe, 1958 and1961). Young white-crowned sparrows (Rose et al., 2004) andcanaries (Gardner and Nottebohm, 2005) tutored with songs thathave normal notes but defective or incomplete syntax learn thenotes from the tutor songs and assemble those notes into a songwith species-appropriate syntax. When a young male song orswamp sparrow hears only recorded songs, both the units ofsound (syllables, notes, and elements) and the organization of

Page 4: Bird song and singing behavior

4

those units within the song, or syntax, may influence the choiceof a model to copy (Marler and Peters, 1977 and 1988). So,while exposure to a specific song from a specific tutor stronglyinfluences what a young bird learns, genetic predispositionsinfluence both the choice of a model and how it is subsequentlyproduced.

As well as copied elements, the songs of young males mayinclude altered or novel notes; such notes are considered to be“improvised” or “invented”. Learned song notes may also berearranged to generate a new order. For these reasons, thelearned song may not be an exact copy of the tutor’s song, but itusually includes many notes that are near or exact matches of thematerial copied from one or more adult models. Thus the songmaterial that is learned depends both upon geneticpredispositions for certain acoustic and structural properties andupon the characteristics of the songs sung by socially salientconspecific birds – with an admixture of “creative” contributionswhen young birds improvise new song units or arrangements ofcopied units. Species differ in the relative amounts of imitationand improvisation that contributes to an individual’s song – fromthose that produce near-exact copies of adult models to those thatimprovise nearly all of their notes (e.g. sedge wrens Cistothorusplatensis; Kroodsma and Verner, 1978).

Identifying the neural mechanisms that underlie imitative songlearning is the “holy grail” of the field (see chapters by Adret andBolhuis, this volume). Researchers have focused most heavilyupon two aspects of bird song: first, the neural basis for theproduction of stable, crystallized song, and second, the processesthat are responsible for changes in that song during development,during seasonal re-learning, or after inducing plasticity in adults.Understanding either of these processes requires a comprehensiveanalysis of the endpoint of song learning, the adult crystallizedsong. The remainder of this chapter will examine the productionof song by adults – what is sung, how it is sung, and how thesocial context of singing affects song performance – with

reference to the species most commonly used in laboratorystudies of bird song, the zebra finch.

Characteristics of adult song: stereotypy and repertoires

The most salient feature of adult song (for the human andpresumably also for the avian listener) is its stereotypy – both inthe acoustic structure of the song syllables and, for many species,the sequencing of the syllables. Crystallization, the final stage oflearning, is usually accompanied by an increase not only instereotypy, but also in the volume and amount of singing,particularly for seasonal breeders such as canaries. This is notsurprising as song is often used for territory defense andadvertisement; a louder song is more effective in establishing apresence over a wide area. Because frequent, loud singing isenergetically expensive (Oberweger and Goller, 2001), softersinging during song learning is presumably less costly, and alsoless likely to attract predators. However, zebra finch songremains relatively quiet, most probably because the object of thesong in this highly social species is generally no more than a fewmeters from the singer. An increase in stereotypy of syllablestructure and of the ordering of syllables does occur in bothzebra finches and canaries, and appears to be a universalcharacteristic of adult bird song.

An individual male’s crystallized song consists of a distinctrepertoire: a set of elements, notes, syllables, phrases, motifs, orsongs from which all of his singing is drawn. In some species therepertoire may be limited to a small set of syllables or notes thatare each sung in nearly every utterance; for others, recordings ofmany songs may be needed to define a large repertoire. A zebrafinch’s repertoire is quite small, ranging from 3 to 15 notes,sometimes called elements or syllables (Zann, 1996). Each songopens with a series of identical repeated introductory notesfollowed by one or more repetitions of a “motif” that consists ofmost or all of the repertoire of song notes delivered in a fixedsequence, without any repeated notes (Figure 1a). Most of a

Page 5: Bird song and singing behavior

5

zebra finch’s song output consists of the “canonical motif” (or“canonical song”), a note sequence that is repeated in a nearlyinvariant fashion and lasts about one second (Figure 1a). Otherspecies, such as the canary (Nottebohm and Nottebohm, 1978) orstarling (Chaiken et al., 1993), may have a repertoire of dozens ofnotes grouped into syllables that are repeated to form trills orphrases (Figure 1b). These trills or phrases are strung together toform the song, which may continue for well over a minute. Eachphrase is often associated with a particular successor, but thesequence of phrases sung within a given song is rarely an exactreplicate of the previous song. For this reason, it may benecessary to record several minutes of song to accurately describean individual’s repertoire. Still other species (such as songsparrows and redwinged blackbirds) may have a repertoireconsisting of several distinct songs, each of which is stereotypedand each of which differs from other songs, or song types, in therepertoire. Such species may sing with “immediate variety”,changing their song type with each successive song, or with“eventual variety”, (Kroodsma, 1982) repeating a song typeseveral times before changing to another song type in therepertoire.

The patterns of song behavior represented by the two mostoften studied species, zebra finches and canaries, do not span theentire range of songbird performance, but do represent relativelydistant points within that spectrum. There are important aspectsof song repertoires that are not well represented in studies of brainmechanisms for singing based on these two species: neithercanaries nor zebra finches have repertoires with multiple songtypes, and domesticated birds do not form song dialects (distinctsongs associated with specific geographic regions), nor do theymimic recently heard sounds or provide a clear example ofaction-based learning (the pruning of learned material based onthe observed behaviors of conspecifics). Nevertheless, the twospecies have two different types of repertoires organized indifferent ways and learn their songs according to different

schedules. Despite these differences, both species (like allsongbirds except mimics) have song learning trajectories thatculminate in a stereotyped and crystallized adult song. Althoughthe salient characteristic of the crystallized song is its stereotypy,there does remain some potential for variation. The types andsources of this variability are interesting in their own right andalso provide a bridge between studies of crystallized song andsong learning.

Adult song variability

Crystallized zebra finch song is among the least variablesongs, consisting as it does of a relatively few notes that aredelivered in a fixed sequence. Yet even in these songs a limitedform of variability exists: notes can be omitted from thebeginning or end of the song (Sossinka and Böhner, 1980;Williams and Staples, 1992). This type of variability is akin toflexibility in the order of phrase delivery in canary songs and inthe order of song types in species with multiple song typerepertoires. The way notes or song types are ordered duringcrystallized adult song performance may reflect both events thatoccurred during song development and events that occur duringthe performance of the song.

Although the father’s song is often the only or most importantsource of material for the son’s song, the evolution of vocallearning has made it possible for young birds to acquire songmaterial from males other than their fathers, presumably thusincreasing their ability to form dialect groups and to attract mates(Nottebohm, 1972). In the lab, however, song may be learnedexclusively from the father (as when offspring are raised in single-family cages), or from a song chosen by the researcher andplayed back through a speaker (canaries) or a dummy male that‘sings’ when a key is pecked (zebra finches). These social andsong environments are impoverished compared to thoseexperienced by wild birds; after fledging, a wild young zebrafinch male interacts with many adults within the colony.

Page 6: Bird song and singing behavior

6

1 2 3 4 5 6

phrase 2 (expanded)

a b a b

Canary song

notes:0

2

4

6

b

phrases:syllables:

i i i motif truncated motifsmotif motif

Zebra finch song

0

2

4

6

8

a motif (expanded)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7notes:a ba b a bc de a belements:

Figure 1. Structure of zebra finch and canary song.The sonograms show recordings of a portion of one zebra finch’s song (a) and one canary’s song (b); expanded segments from each of these two recordingsare also shown. The dark bar along the 0 kHz line denotes 0.5 seconds for the longer song segments and to 0.1 seconds for the expanded sections of thesong. Sound amplitude, in the form of an oscillogram, is shown along the 8 kHz (zebra finch) or 6 kHz (canary) line. The terminology for song structuresdiffers slightly from some existing practices to ensure that similar units in the two species’ songs have consistent labels.a. In the zebra finch song, introductory notes (i) precede a series of motifs, some of which may be truncated. A motif consists of a repeated sequence ofunique notes. A note is defined as an uninterrupted sound; each note may include one or more elements. An element is defined as a unit of song that has acoherent time/frequency structure that distinguishes it from neighboring elements within a note. For example, note 7 has an initial element with strongfrequency modulation, followed by a constant-frequency element. In such constant-frequency elements the harmonic nature of zebra finch vocalizations canbe clearly seen: element 7b has a fundamental frequency of approximately 650 Hz, and eleven harmonics (integer multiples of the fundamental frequency)can be readily distinguished. Note: this song included five additional motifs that are not shown here.b. Canary song consists of a string of phrases, each made up of many repeats of a single syllable. A syllable may consist of a single note (as in phrase 6) ormay be made up of two or more notes. The syllable that is repeated to form phrase 2 (see expanded version) is made up of two notes, an extended nearlyconstant-frequency 2 kHz note and a short higher-frequency note. The short sound at approximately 4 kHz is the second harmonic of the loudest portion ofnote a and does not represent a third note in this syllable. The sonogram shown here was drawn from a recording provided by Dr. Fernando Nottebohm’slaboratory.

Page 7: Bird song and singing behavior

7

A breeding aviary with multiple pairs can be used to expose ayoung male zebra finch to multiple song models, and in such anenvironment young zebra finch males often copy song elementsfrom more than one adult male (Williams, 1990; Figure 2).

Copying from more than one male occurs in many species,particularly when males acquire large repertoires and/or areexposed to widely varying song models before breeding. Whenmultiple tutors contribute to a song, elements copied from aspecific tutor are associated with each other within the structure ofyoung male’s song. Nightingales (Luscinia luscinia) haverepertoires of up to 100 songs, which they learn as “packages” ofapproximately four songs copied from a single tutor (Hultsch andTodt, 1989a, and this volume). As adults, the young males sing thesongs within a learned package in the same order the tutor used,and packages copied from a given tutor are likely to be sung insuccession (Hultsch and Todt, 1989b; Hultsch, 1991). Thestructure of canary song, with its strings of repeated syllables andpredictable transitions between syllables, may represent a similarly“packaged” structure that could also reflect units of song learnedfrom individual tutors. Zebra finches, despite their much lesscomplex song, have a similar within-song structure: “chunks” ofapproximately three notes are learned as units (Williams andStaples, 1992). When multiple tutors provide source material for ayoung male’s song, the notes copied from each tutor are notintermixed, but rather chunks of notes from each song are sung insequence. Because the order of learned chunks are oftenrearranged, the learned song may be novel even when a youngmale copies all of its song notes from a single tutor (Figure 3a). Theperformance of the song also reflects this chunked structure; if thesinger truncates a motif, the breaks in the normal sequence mostoften occur at chunk boundaries (Figure 3b). Thus, like therepertoire of the nightingale (and possibly that of the canary) theadult song of the zebra finch has a hidden level of organization thatreflects processes that occurred during song learning.

Figure 2. Multiple tutors for a single zebra finch song.The song of a young male zebra finch raised in an aviary with eight adultmales. The presumptive father of the young male sang the song denoted“tutor male 1”, but because of the potential for extra-pair copulations andegg dumping, paternity cannot be conclusively attributed to the adult inwhose nest the young male hatched. The young male copied the first fournotes (four elements) from the social father, and copied the remainder ofthe song (5 notes made up of 8 elements) from a second male (tutor male2). The young male’s introductory note was not copied from either tutor.

Page 8: Bird song and singing behavior

8

Figure 3. Chunks of zebra finch notes are copied and sung as units.a. The motif of an adult male zebra finch that was copied by several young males in an aviary environment (the same male’s motif appears as tutor male 2 inFig. 2). The motifs of six young males that copied the adult are also shown; notes that were copied from the adult male are numbered to correspond with thenotes in the tutor’s song. Beneath each of the copied motifs, bars denote notes that were copied as continuous sequences, or chunks. One of the copied songmotifs shown in A, with bars indicating the copied chunks. Arrows and associated percentages denote the proportion of motifs ending after each note duringan extended series of recordings. Two song recordings including a total of seven motifs are shown; arrows denote the end of each motif. The final note ineach motif is highlighted. Truncated motifs ended between, and not within, the chunks of notes copied as sequences from the adult male’s song.

Page 9: Bird song and singing behavior

9

Individual variation in the stereotypy of adult song

Individuals’ songs are strikingly different from each other.Even birds tutored by the same adult will sing different songs,because of variation in the number of units copied, the accuracyof the copies, and the ordering of the subunits of song. Severalfactors act in concert to generate these individual differences.Young male zebra finches mutually influence each other’s copyaccuracy; the fastest learner in a nest sings the most accuratecopy of the model song, while his brothers diverge from themodel in different ways (Tchernichovski et al., 1999). Individualdifferences in brain characteristics influence learning; birds withlarger forebrain song centers tend to have larger song repertoiresin both canaries (Nottebohm et al., 1981) and zebra finches(Ward et al., 1998; Airey and DeVoogd, 2000).

Another source of individual variation arises from differencesin the way adults perform their songs. Some males sing readily,others do not; some unmated male zebra finches sing only toparticular birds, while others sing readily to many potential mates;some males sing long bouts, while others may sing equal numbersof motifs in shorter bouts; some male’s songs may vary somewhatbetween renditions, while others’ will be more stereotyped; somemale zebra finches sing several introductory notes before eachsong bout, while others may sing only one introductory note orforego these notes altogether.

Figure 4. Motif variation in tutored and untutored zebra finch song.a. A continuous recording of 15 seconds of untutored male zebra finchsong. Each of the seven motifs sung in this segment is a different variant.In comparison, Fig. 1a shows five motifs from a song of a normally-reared male; the complete song included ten motifs (seven of them thecanonical motif) and three motif variants. b. Motif variability differsamong normally reared birds, and is lower than in untutored birds.Within a recording of approximately 100 motifs, the frequency of eachmotif variant was sung was tabulated and motif variants were orderedfrom most common to least common. This plot shows the cumulative

proportion of the total number motifs sung that are included as eachsuccessive motif variant is added to the sample. The number of motifvariants needed to generate 85% of all motifs sung (a level representedby the dotted line) can be used to describe the variability of an adultmale’s song. A typical pattern of motif variation for a normally rearedmale is shown by the diamonds (the 85% level is reached with 2.18motif variants), the extreme for normally reared males by the triangles(8.69 motif variants were required to reach the 85% level), and adistribution seen only in untutored males by the circles (21.67 motifswere required to reach the 85% level).

Page 10: Bird song and singing behavior

10

Such variability in the performance of crystallized adult song,which is apparent even in critical-period learners such as zebrafinches, has largely been ignored in studies of the neural basis ofbehavior because the presence of a ‘canonical’ song is valuable forstudying the neural patterns associated with multiple examples ofthe same complex behavior.

However, variability does exist in the adult songs of bothcanaries (where the sequences of phrases is not fixed) and zebrafinches (in the form of alternative motifs that omit or add particularnotes). One way to analyze individual variability in adult malezebra finch song is to compare the numbers of unusual syllabletransitions and canonical syllable transitions within a sample ofapproximately 100 songs (Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991). Another

measure calculates the minimum number of different motif typesneeded to account for 85% of all song motifs in a sample of at least100 motifs. Either of these measures demonstrates that there issubstantial variability between normally-reared males in songstereotypy (Figure 4).

The fact that such variation in adult song exists, even in zebrafinches – a species that sings one of the simplest and moststereotyped of songs – indicates that variability may play animportant role in behavior. One possible reason role for adult songvariability might be to allow the singer to tailor his stereotyped songto fit the preferences of a specific listener, such as a rival or apotential mate.

The influence of a listener upon song development

A male that never hears a song tutor or model develops anabnormal song. This “untutored song” retains many characteristicsof normal zebra finch song but is marked by unusual note structureand decreased stereotypy (Price, 1979; Williams et al., 1993). Highfrequency notes and upsweeps are much more common inuntutored song, as are repeated notes and a highly variable noteorder (although it is usually very difficult to define a canonicalpattern in tutored zebra finch song). It has been generally assumedthat the unusual properties of untutored song are due to theabsence of a song model, but some of the abnormal features ofuntutored song may arise in part because of the absence of lfeedback from the behavioral responses of a conspecific listener.

Hearing a model is clearly crucial for normal songdevelopment. Nevertheless, in the absence of a model, listenerscan guide song development towards a normal outcome – which isthe case in brown-headed cowbirds (which are brood parasites andso do not hear appropriate song models until relatively late indevelopment). An adult female listening to a young male respondsselectively by flipping her wing or changing her posture in response

to particular versions of a young male’s attempts at song. Themale’s observations of the female’s actions strongly influences whathe sings subsequently, and so the female directs the trajectory ofsong development (West and King, 1988). Although evidence forpowerful listener effects in other species is not as compelling as forcowbirds, zebra finches because they grow up in social coloniesand their early songs are heard by many other birds, are a speciesin which listeners’ responses could affect song development. Inthe laboratory, untutored males raised with deaf adult females singmore frequently than do untutored males raised with hearing adultfemales. The songs of the males raised with deaf adult females aresimilar to those of untutored males, with less stereotypy and moreatypical syllables than in the songs of males raised with hearingadult females (Figure 5). In contrast, the presence of a hearingfemale alters the course of untutored song development so that theoutcome approximates normal song – possibly because the hearingfemale provides feedback to the young male about the content ofhis song. Siblings also affect the outcome of song development,although it is not clear whether the effects are due to his hearer’sresponses or to what a young bird hears his siblings sing. Youngmales housed together converge on the same version of anuntutored song (Volman and Khanna, 1995), and males within a

Page 11: Bird song and singing behavior

11

Figure 5. Audience effects upon zebra finch song development. a. Examples of the songs of males reared with and without tutors(the untutored male was raised with a male sibling of the same age).Elements that are similar to zebra finch calls are shaded. Non-callelements (bold) include high-frequency elements with fewharmonics, upwardly modulated elements, and click trains. b. Thepresence of a hearing female during development decreases theproportion of non-call elements incorporated into a young male’ssong. In recordings made after birds reached 90 days of age, malesreared with at least one hearing companion (an adult male tutor, ahearing mother or a hearing adult female companion) usedsignificantly fewer non-call elements in their songs than did malesthat were housed alone or with deaf conspecifics (deaf mothers anddeaf adult female companions; p < 0.001). The condition of themother is designated first, followed by the condition of the adultfemale companion. Young males were housed with their mothers forthe first four weeks after hatching, and then housed with the adultfemale companion until reaching at least 90 days of age. c. Malesraised with an adult male song model sing a highly stereotypedsequence: fewer than 5% of transitions between elements areatypical (those not present in the canonical motif). In contrast, malesraised with adult females sing a less stereotyped song with asignificantly greater number of atypical transitions (p < 0.05).Untutored males were recorded at 200 days, well after the age whensong normally crystallizes (90 days); normal males were recorded at150 days. d. During song learning (60-75 days), males reared withdeaf females sing more frequently than than did males housed withhearing females (p < 0.001). Singing rates were measured as theaverage time spent singing during 30-minute recording sessions.

normally reared clutch influence the accuracy of each others’copies of the model (Tchernichovski et al., 1999).

Conspecifics’ influence on song development can thus beexerted in two ways: first, by providing a model song to copy, andsecond, by listening to the developing song and providingbehavioral feedback about what was sung. The relative importanceof these two contributions may vary depending upon the species

and the circumstances, but the potential role of a listener duringsong development should not be ignored.

The influence of a listener upon adult song performance

Crystallized adult male birdsong has two well-documentedfunctions – territory defense and mate attraction – that haveevolved specifically to influence listeners’ behavior. Thus it is not

Page 12: Bird song and singing behavior

12

surprising that listeners’ behaviors in turn affect adult songperformance in several ways. The visual and auditory presence aswell as the gender of conspecifics affects the amplitude and tempoof song delivery. Singing male zebra finches adjust the amplitude oftheir songs as the social context changes (Cynx and Gell, 2004), asdo nightingales (Brumm and Todt, 2004). The tempo of undirectedzebra finch song is slightly slower than that of directed song(Sossinka and Böhner, 1980), and song tempo also variessystematically throughout the day, increasing during periods whenthe birds sing frequently (Glaze and Troyer, 2006). Repeatedexposure to females over a period of weeks, as when directed songis recorded at frequent intervals, is associated with an increase insong tempo (Williams and Mehta, 1999). “Listener effects” are notrestricted to these relatively simple song parameters, but extend tothe order and identity of the notes or songs that are sung.

Species with multiple songs, such as the nightingale (andpotentially those with multiple phrases, such as the canary orstarling) may have preferred song orders, but these are strongstatistical trends rather than invariant rules. The order in whichsyllables or songs are sung can be influenced by the responses ofother birds. When two rival males “countersing” from adjacentterritories or as part of a territorial incursion, the delivery of amale’s repertoire often shifts. “Song matching” is a form of songperformance in which a male sings the song in his repertoire thatbest matches the song being sung by the rival of the same species(e.g., the indigo bunting Passerina cyanea, Payne, 1983;meadowlark Sturnella magna, Falls, 1985; song sparrow, Stoddardet al., 1992). Countersinging males may also shift the timing oftheir songs so that they overlap the songs of the rival male, as in thenightingale (Naguib and Todt, 1997; Naguib, 1999). Thechickadee not only overlaps its rival’s song (Mennill and Ratcliffe,2004) but also shifts the pitch of the song so that it assumes aspecific relationship to that of the other male (Weisman andRatcliffe, 2004). Since wild canaries sing many syllables in avariable order and their songs function in territorial advertisement

and defense, they are likely to engage in countersinging – andindeed domesticated males are more likely to sing in the presenceof other singing males or similar sounds. Canaries might thenperform a type of song matching by shifting the usual syllablesequence in order to best match that of the countersinging male. Ifshifts in syllable order could be triggered in response to specificplayback tapes, canaries could provide a valuable model forstudying variation in behavioral sequences, a phenomenon that hasnot been examined at the level of neural mechanisms.

In contrast to species that broadcast high-volume song from alarge, defended territory that provides the male with access toresources needed for reproduction, zebra finches sing a low-amplitude song intended for listeners within a few meters (Zann,1996). This song has two forms associated with different socialcontexts: a courtship song that is “directed” at a specific bird, and“undirected” song, which is sung without an obvious object(Sossinka and Böhner, 1980). The two forms of the song consist ofthe same syllables given in the same order, but undirected song ischaracterized by greater sequence variability. Undirected songsare more likely than directed songs to include truncated motifs,start in mid-motif, or use atypical transitions between notes (Figure6). Chipping sparrows (Spizella passerina) also adjust thevariability of their songs according to the social context: the dawnsong, directed at close range to other males, is variable in length,and the day song, broadcast to distant birds, is of fixed length (Liuand Nottebohm, 2005). These audience-dependent differences inthe variability of song structure are associated with differences ingene expression in the song basal ganglia circuitry. For both zebrafinches (Jarvis et al., 1998) and chipping sparrows (Liu andNottebohm, 2005), singing the more variable form of the songactivates expression of a transcription factor (ZENK) in the basalganglia portion of the song circuit, while the less variable form ofthe song does not (see Mello and Jarvis, this volume). Although therole of social context in generating the two types of neural

Page 13: Bird song and singing behavior

13

Figure 6. Differences between directed (courtship)and undirected song in the zebra finch.During courtship song the male orients himself to and directs a display, ordance, at a conspecific individual. A male singing undirected songneither orients towards an individual nor performs a courtship display.Song stereotypy also differs in these two contexts.a. Transitions between elements are less stereotyped during undirectedsong than during courtship song. Thicker lines in this diagram representtransitions that are more frequently observed. The box denoted “i”represents introductory notes.b. Males singing courtship song (black bars) use more introductory notes(p < 0.01) and sing more motifs in each song (p < 0.01).The data for this figure and analysis were drawn from Sossinka andBöhner, 1980.

activation patterns and associated song variability is not yet clear, itis of considerable interest.

Behavioral feedback from non-singing females can influencethe form of adult male song. Brown-headed cowbirds provide theclassic example of this phenomenon (King and West, 1983; 1989).After observing (and comparing) female responses to his own andother males’ songs, an adult cowbird male may shift his singing toapproximate a successful male’s song. As brood parasites that areraised by other species, cowbirds are a special case in whichflexibility in adult song may be at a premium, but female responsesto adult males also affect the delivery of zebra finch song. Theprobability that a male will truncate or add a note to his canonicalmotif is affected by any calls the female gives while he sings (Figure7). Since individual females respond differently to a male’s song,repeated exposure to a specific female may result in a shift in amale’s canonical motif when particular notes within the repertoireare either omitted or sung more often. In this way, an adult malewith a crystallized, stereotyped song may still tailor its contents tofit the fashion favored by the female.

Visual displays associated with song

Many songbirds broadcast their songs over a wide area from ahidden location, and such songs can be accurately represented as apurely auditory signal. Other songs are accompanied by stylizedmovements, or “dances”, and displays of plumage, ranging fromsimple adjustments of feather posture to complex and dramaticdeployment of specialized plumes, as in birds of paradise. Suchdisplays may be delivered by solitary singers, or be performed bygroups of males at leks (a lek is a traditional display area consistingof a cluster of small territories each occupied by a courting male;lekking species include the black grouse and several manakins).More subtle displays are common as well. Canaries sing with araised head and loosely fluffed throat feathers, and zebra finchesadjust their plumage so that the normally rounded head becomes a

Page 14: Bird song and singing behavior

14

Figure 7. Female calls affect male song delivery in zebra finches.a. Individual male and female zebra finches were placed so they could see but not hear each other through a lucite partition, and the experimenter controlledwhich bird could hear the other by relaying a recording to a speaker on the opposite side of the partition . Here the female could not hear the male, but thefemale’s vocalizations were played to the male (note the presence of the female’s calls in the recording of the male). Motifs and notes are designated,respectively, by bars and numbers under the sonogram of the male’s song (calls are not numbered). The unusual calls and motif structure sung when thefemale first called were repeated for several motifs before the male returned to a more standard motif structure. b. The average female call rate was notaffected by males’ ability to hear the calls, but female calls increased significantly (p < 0.05) when females could hear the males’ vocalizations. c. Whetheror not males could hear a female did not significantly affect the number of atypical motifs the males sang. d. Female call rate was significantly higher(p < 0.05) during atypical motifs, indicating that when males could hear a female’s call, they were more likely to sing atypical motifs.

Page 15: Bird song and singing behavior

15

“flat top”. Zebra finch males also perform a courtship dance, aseries of hopping and bobbing movements oriented to a female, aspart of directed song (Morris, 1954).

While singing, many birds open and close their beaks tochange the gape, or beak aperture. The primary function ofvariations in gape is most probably to adjust the resonance of thevocal tract, and so to emphasize sounds of a particular frequency;an open beak yields a shorter vocal tract that emphasizes higher-frequency sounds (Westneat et al., 1993; Hoese et al., 2000; seealso Goller and Cooper, this volume). Changes in beak gape aretightly coupled to specific song syllables, and beak movements arealso clearly visible to a nearby observer. In a similar fashion, throatmovements that reflect changes in the volume of the upperrespiratory tract that track changes in syllable frequency (Riede etal., 2006) can be visually emphasized by the “flashing” ofcontrasting throat feathers. For a nearby listener, changes in thebeak gape and throat feathers provide visual correlates of some ofthe structural features of song, and may serve to accentuate parts ofthe song.

The dance steps of a courting zebra finch male (hops, headmovements, and wing movements) vary somewhat from song tosong, are not as tightly coupled as beak movements to the acousticstructure of specific syllables. Like the patterns of changes in beakgape, however, the overall pattern of dance movements thataccompanies an individual’s song is very similar to the pattern ofthe bird that provided the auditory model for the song (Figure 8;

Figure 8. Coordination of zebra finch song with beak movements and the courtship dance.The motifs of an adult tutor and a young male that copied the song are shown at the top and bottom, respectively. The final note in the tutor’s song (shaded)was not copied, and the copied song includes an improved note (shaded). The beak and dance movement data from the copied song were shifted to the leftto align copied notes to corresponding notes in the tutor’s song (as indicated by arrows above the copied song). Videotapes of approximately 100 motifswere examined, and the positions of beak movements (opening or closing) within each motif were determined, as were the positions within each motif of thebeginning of “dance movements”. Beak movements were highly stereotyped within the motif, and dance movements showed less rigid but distinctivepatterns coupled to what was being sung. Movement patterns in the tutor’s song and the copied song were significantly correlated (beak movement p <0.0001; dance movement p <0.001).

Page 16: Bird song and singing behavior

16

Williams, 2001). Thus these courtship dance patterns must eitherbe learned from a model or be regulated by a rhythm tied to theacoustic structure of the song.

The reliable coupling of dance and beak movements to the songsuggests that the traditionally-defined song circuitry also engagesother motor patterns, perhaps using respiratory circuits and otherpathways outside the song system (Wild, 2004, and this volume). Itfurther suggests that listeners’ responses to a particular song may beaffected by the visual signals associated with the song as well asbythe acoustic parameters of the song; advances in monitortechnology now make it possible to use video stimuli to test suchrelationships (Galoch and Bischof, 2006). Although the acousticproperties of song are far more tractable for analysis and forresearch into vocal learning and motor patterns, the biologicalfunction of the song may be difficult to separate from themovements that make up an associated visual signal.

Hormones, motivation and song performance

As a behavior that is affected by day length, sex steroids and bythe presence of specific individuals that are potential rivals ormates, bird song is presumably under the influence of brain areasassociated with emotion and motivation (Arnold, 1975; Marler etal., 1988; Nowicki and Ball, 1989). Studies of the role ofphotoperiod and hormones on song circuitry have tended to focusupon structures related to the mammalian cortex and basal ganglia(Smith et al., 1997; Tramontin et al., 1999; Ball et al., 2002), butmotivational influences on song are likely to be mediated bystructures such as the hypothalamus and limbic system (see Ball, etal., this volume). Links between song circuitry and thehypothalamus and limbic system have not yet been fully defined,but are likely to be similar to those in non-songbird species such asthe ring dove (see Cheng and Durand, 2004).

The effects of sex steroids (testosterone, and its metabolites) onsong performance are well known. In mammals, receptors for sex

steroids are found in the hypothalamus, limbic system, andmidbrain (McEwen et al., 1979), all of which are thought to affectmotivational states. Sex steroid receptors are found in the sameareas of songbird brains as well as in the cells of song circuit nuclei(Arnold et al., 1976; Gahr, 2001). In the canary and other open-ended learners, seasonal changes in song circuitry are associatedwith fluctuations in day length and circulating testosterone levels(Nottebohm, 1980; DeVoogd and Nottebohm 1981; Nottebohm etal., 1987; Canady et al., 1988; Rasika et al., 1994; Bernard andBall, 1997; Ball et al., 2004). Testosterone plays an especiallyimportant role in mediating the rapid increase in stereotypy thataccompanies song crystallization. Artificially high testosteronelevels in development cause early crystallization of song in zebrafinches (Korsia and Bottjer, 1981), and reducing testosterone levelsor blocking testosterone receptors delays song crystallization insparrows (Marler et al., 1988). In zebra finches, the song itself isstable after crystallization at 90 days, but manipulating circulatingtestosterone levels does affect singing (Arnold, 1975; Adkins-Reganand Ascenzi, 1987). Castrated male zebra finches sing less than dotheir intact counterparts, and implanting testosterone-filled silasticcapsules is widely and effectively used to induce higher singingrates in intact adult males. Testosterone supplementation does,however, affect what is sung as well as how often the bird sings:the fundamental frequency of zebra finch song notes may dropsubstantially (Cynx et al., 2004) and it is possible that tempo andstereotypy are also increased.

Adult male zebra finch song has the potential for a latent formof plasticity. When auditory feedback is disrupted during songproduction by deafening (Nordeen and Nordeen, 1992) orintroducing auditory interference (Leonardo and Konishi, 1999;Cynx and von Rad, 2001), adult the song structure and stereotypyare lost over a period of weeks or months (see chapters by Woolleyand Brainard, this volume). Adults may also reconfigurecrystallized songs after their vocal output is disrupted by any of anumber of interventions, including syringeal nerve section

Page 17: Bird song and singing behavior

17

Figure 9. Testosterone reduces adult song plasticity in zebra finches.a. An example of adult song plasticity. The four distinct notes in the original motif were all retained (in a noisier form) in recordings made one day after oneof the nerves serving the vocal organ (syrinx) was severed. Notes that did not change subsequently are shaded throughout. By the time of a recording madetwo weeks after the injury, note 4 had been deleted and two new notes (A and B) had been inserted at the end of the motif. These changes persisted in allsubsequent recordings. The lengths of the deleted and added notes were summed and expressed as a the percentage of the length of the original motif.b. The proportion of the song motif that changed after nerve injury varied widely among males. Birds with larger testes were significantly less likely tochange their songs (p < 0.05). c. Circulating hormone levels were manipulated by implanting testosterone or flutamide (a testosterone receptor blocker).Birds with testosterone implants altered a smaller proportion of their song motifs than did birds with flutamide implants (p < 0.05).

Page 18: Bird song and singing behavior

18

(Williams and McKibben, 1992), muting (Pytte and Suthers, 2000),or mechanical interference with phonation (Hough and Volman,2002). At least one of these forms of adult plasticity is sensitive tocirculating testosterone; adult male zebra finches with hightestosterone levels are less likely to change their songs aftersyringeal nerve section (Figure 9; Williams et al., 2003). Thustestosterone’s effects on adult song performance and plasticity inzebra finches are consistent with the better-understood role oftestosterone during development.

The powerful relationships between testosterone and song rate,stereotypy, and the potential for adult plasticity all point to a rolefor testosterone in reducing variability in song performance while atthe same time increasing song production. However, testosterone’seffects cannot do not account for all of the variation in adult songstereotypy and performance (Boseret et al., 2006). Brainmechanisms unrelated to sex steroids most probably contribute tomotivational control of song variability, and factors other thanmotivation may also account for aspects of song variability. Giventhe variety of factors with a demonstrated relationship to variabilityin song performance, it is likely that additional mechanisms forsong variability are yet to be described.

Calls

In addition to their songs, songbirds’ vocal repertoires include anumber of calls - shorter, simpler vocalizations. The number andtype of calls varies from species to species, but most songbirds havecontact calls (used to identify and track other individuals), alarmcalls (to give warning of a predator), agonistic calls (to denoteintention to supplant or attack) and food-associated calls (such asbegging calls emitted by young). Some call types are used year-round, while others may be given only within the breeding season;some calls may be given only by one sex, while others are used byall members of a species. Although the ontogeny of calls has notbeen studied as systematically as song, it is clear that some calls are

learned while others appear to be innate (Güttinger and Nicolai,1973; Zann, 1985). Both learned and unlearned calls may varygeographically, and the variation can take the form of dialects(Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987; Miyasato and Baker, 1999).Because of the variety of calls and the variety of contexts in whichthey are used, calls may be more useful than song for exploringhow information is communicated; because they are usually simpleacoustically (in comparison to song), learned calls may also providea simpler model for studying the brain mechanisms used forimitating an auditory model (see Vicario, 2004).

Many species’ call repertoires are relatively easy tocharacterize: chaffinches have eight different call types, eachassociated with a specific behavioral context (Marler, 2004). Incontrast, some call repertoires are larger and may have complexsyntactic structures. Chickadees have a simple, two-noted song,but have two complex calls, the “gargle” and the characteristic“chick-a-dee” call, which may vary from utterance to utterance.The “chick-a-dee” call consists of four different note types, each ofwhich may be repeated several times before proceeding to (orskipping) the next note type (Hailman et al., 1987). Responses tovariants of these combinatorial calls suggest that differentconfigurations convey specific information about the intentions ofthe caller, such as proceeding towards or away from a food source(Freeberg and Lucas, 2002).

Adult zebra finches have nine call types (Zann, 1996): onealarm call, three calls given while nest building, an agonistic call, adistress call, and three calls given frequently in the course of dailyactivity (the long, stack, and tet calls). More than one type of callmay be given within a short interval, but the calls are not combinedto form complex sequences. The best-studied zebra finch call is thelong call, which is the loudest sound a zebra finch makes and isgiven when a bird is isolated or responding to another bird at adistance (for this reason the long call is also called the “contactcall”). The long call is sexually dimorphic: males’ calls are shorterand consist of a two parts, a harmonic “tonal” portion and a rapidly

Page 19: Bird song and singing behavior

19

modulated portion, while females’ calls include only a long tonalportion (Zann, 1984). Males learn their long calls, but females donot (Zann, 1985), and the sexually dimorphic circuitry thatunderlies song learning is also important for call learning (Simpsonand Vicario, 1990). Variation in both sexes’ calls can conveyinformation about individual identity, and the presence and identityof an audience affects how males respond to their mates and otherfamiliar females (Vignal et al., 2004).

Because zebra finches’ long calls can be elicited by placing anindividual out of sight and hearing of other birds and playing backrecorded long calls, a “dialogue” of sorts between an experimenterand a bird can be generated. The length of the long call, the sex ofthe caller, and the sex of the responder all affect the probability thatan isolated zebra finch will respond to a long call with a long callof its own (Vicario, et al., 2002). This sex difference in responses tolong calls disappears after lesioning the RA, a nucleus in the vocalmotor pathway that is important for the production of learned songsand calls (Vicario et al., 2001).

Figure 10. Zebra finch and black-capped chickadee calls.a. Among the most common zebra finch calls are the long, or

“contact” or “distance” call, which differs between males and females,and the softer stack and “tet” calls (which are similar in males andfemales). All of these calls may be given singly or in clusters. b .Combinatorial syntax in black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus) calls.The “chick-a-dee” call from which this species gets its common name isdistinctive and recognizable but is also highly variable. It consists of fournotes, labeled A, B, C, and D, with structure that varies from bird to bird(three examples of each note type are shown). “Chick-a-dee” calls tendto follow simple, semi-Markovian rules to generate sequences from thesethree syllable types: a syllable can be followed by another syllable of thesame type or by a type with a letter later in the alphabet. The 13examples shown here, drawn from a single recording of four wild birds,show some of the variety of utterances that can be assembled from thesefour syllables and the simple syntactical rules (note that these examplesinclude two “violations” of the rules).

Page 20: Bird song and singing behavior

20

Studies such as these show the potential of using the system ofsongbird calls to investigate the behavioral and brain rules forvocal communication. Applying this approach to other call types- perhaps especially to combinatorial calls such as those of thechickadee – may provide an avenue towards understanding themeanings of vocalizations as they are decoded and understood bythe birds

Summary and conclusion

Because the primary impetus for studying bird song is as amodel for vocal learning, we sometimes forget that the endpointof that process is the adult song. Adult song provides thereference that allows for retrospective understanding of thetrajectory of song development, and manipulations that skew theoutcome of the process, such as rearing young birds without asong model, allow for better understanding of what is required forappropriate song learning. But it is the normal adult song thatplays the biologically important roles that have been shaped bynatural and sexual selection - primarily territory defense andadvertisement for a mate. Adult song, however much itrepresents a completed process in its crystallized and stereotypedform, is nevertheless variable and subject to a variety ofinfluences – including steroid hormones, visual displays,conspecific listeners, and the identity of tutors for the elements ofthe song repertoire. Adult song variability may be normallylimited to relatively few degrees of freedom, as in the restrictedpossibilities for delivering alternative motifs in the zebra finch, butthis limited situational plasticity may nevertheless have importantinsights to offer. The original motivation for studying the neuralbases of bird song, its unique qualities as a model for a complex

behavior that is acquired through imitative learning, is still ofcentral importance to neuroscience. Adult birdsong, because it isa well characterized and easily analyzed behavior, also providesan opportunity for examining mechanisms responsible forvariations in song related to the unique experiences of the singerand the social context in which they occur.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from the Essel Foundation andthe Howard Hughes Medical Institute to Williams College.

ReferencesAdret P. (1993) Operant conditioning, song learning and imprinting to

taped song in the zebra finch. Anim Behav 46:149-159.Adkins-Regan E, Ascenzi M (1987) Social and sexual behavior of male

and female zebra finches treated with testosterone. AnimBehav 35:1100-1112.

Airey DC, DeVoogd TJ (2000) Greater song complexity is associatedwith augmented song system anatomy in zebra finches.Neuroreport 11:2339-2344.

Arnold AP (1975) The effects of castration and androgen replacementon song, courtship, and aggression in zebra finches (Poephilaguttata). J. Exp. Zool., 191:309-326.

Arnold AP, Nottebohm F, Pfaff DW (1976) Hormone concentrating cellsin vocal control and other areas of the brain of the zebra finch(Poephila guttata). J Comp Neurol 165:487-512.

Ball GF, Riters LV, Balthazart J (2002) Neuroendocrinology of songbehavior and avian brain plasticity: multiple sites of action ofsex steroid hormones. Front Neuroendocrinol 23:137-178.

Ball GF, Auger CJ, Bernard DJ, Charlier TD, Sartor JJ, Riters LV,Balthazart J (2004) Seasonal plasticity in the song control

Page 21: Bird song and singing behavior

21

system: steroid metabolism, brain sites, and mechanisms ofhormone action. In: Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong(Ziegler HP, Marler P eds), pp586-610. New York: Ann NYAcad Sci.

Bernard DJ, Ball GF (1997) Photoperiodic condition modulates theeffects of testosterone on song control nuclei volumes in maleEuropean starlings. Gen Com. Endocrinol 105:276-283.

Bolhuis JJ, Van Mil DP, Houx BB (1999) Song learning with audiovisualcompound stimuli in zebra finches. Anim Behav 58:1285-1292.

Boseret G, Carere C, Ball GF, Balthazart J (2006) Social context affectstestosterone-induced singing and the volume of song controlnuclei in male canaries (Serinus canaria). J Neurobiol 66:1044-1060.

Brumm H, Todt D (2004) Male-male vocal interactions and theadjustment of song amplitude in a territorial bird. Anim Behav67:281-286.

Canady RA, Burd GD, DeVoogd TJ, and Nottebohm F (1988) Effect oftestosterone on input received by an identified neuron type ofthe canary song system: a Golgi/EM/Degeneration study. JNeurosci 8:3770-3784.

Chaiken M, Böhner J, Marler P (1993) Song acquisition in Europeanstarlings, Sturnus vulgaris: a comparison of the songs of live-tutored, tape-tutored, untutored, and wild-caught males. AnimBehav 46:1079-1090.

Cheng M-F, Durand SE (2004) A new function for the bird’s own song.In: Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong (Ziegler HP, Marler Peds), pp611-627. New York: Ann NY Acad Sci.

Clayton DF (2004) Songbird genomics: Methods, mechanisms,opportunities, and pitfalls. In: Behavioral Neurobiology ofBirdsong (Ziegler HP, Marler P eds), pp45-60. New York: AnnNY Acad Sci.

Cynx, J, Bean NJ, Rossman I (2004) Testosterone implants alter thefrequency range of zebra finch songs. Horm Behav 47:446-451.

Cynx J, Gell, C (2004) Social mediation of vocal amplitude in asongbird, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim Behav 67:451-455.

Cynx J, von Rad U (2001) Immediate and transitory effects of delayedauditory feedback on bird song production. Anim Behav62:305-312.

DeVoogd TJ, Nottebohm F (1981) Gonadal hormones induce dendriticgrowth in the adult avian brain. Science 214:202-204.

Falls JB (1985) Song matching in western meadowlarks. Can J Zool63:2520-2524.

Farabaugh SM (1982) The ecological and social significance ofduetting. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 2(Kroodsma DE Miller EH eds) pp.85-124. New York:Academic Press.

Ferries MA (2004) The avian song system in comparative perspective.In: Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong (Ziegler HP, Marler Peds), pp61-76. New York: Ann NY Acad Sci.

Freeberg TM, Lucas JR (2002) Receivers respond differently to chick-a-dee calls varying in note composition in Carolina chickadees,Poecile carolinensis. Anim Behav 63:837-845.

Gahr, M (2001) Distribution of sex steroid hormone receptors in theavian brain: functional implications for neural sex differencesand sexual behaviors. Microsc Res Tech 55:1-11.

Galoch Z, Bischof H-J (2007) Behavioural responses to video playbacksby zebra finch males. Behav Proc 74:21-26

Gardner TJ, Naef F, Nottebohm F (2005) Freedom and rules: theacquisition and reprogramming of a bird's learned song.Science 308:1046-9.

Glaze CM, Troyer TW (2006) Temporal structure in zebra finch song:Implications for motor coding. J Neurosci 26:991-1005.

Güttinger H-R, Nicolai J (1973) Struktur und Funktion der Rufe beiPrachtfinken (Estrildidae). Z Tierpsychol 33:319-334.

Hailman JP, Ficken MS, Ficken RS (1987) Constraints on the structure ofcombinatorial Chick-a-dee calls. Ethology 75:62-80.

Hoese WH, Podos J, Boetticher NC, Nowicki S. (2000) Vocal tractfunction in birdsong production: experimental manipulation ofbeak movements. J Exp Biol 203:1845-1855.

Hough GE, Volman, SF (2002) Short-term and long-term effects of vocaldistortion on song maintenance in zebra finches. J Neurosci22:1177-1186.

Hultsch H, Todt D (1989a) Memorization and reproduction of songs innightingales: evidence for package formation. J Comp PhysiolA 165:197-203.

Hultsch H, Todt D (1989b) Context memorization in the song-learningof birds. Naturwissenschaften 76:584-586.

Page 22: Bird song and singing behavior

22

Hultsch H (1991) Early experience can modify singing styles: evidencefrom experiments with nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos.Anim Behav 42:883-889.

Immelmann K (1969) Song development in the zebra finch and otherestrildid finches. In: Bird Vocalizations (Hinde RA, ed.) pp61-74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F (1998) Forwhom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression.Neuron 21:775-788.

King AP, West MJ (1983) Epigenesis of cowbird song - A jointendeavour of males and females. Nature 305:704-706.

King AP, West MJ (1989) The effect of female cowbirds on vocalimitation and improvisation in males. J Comp Psychol 103:39-44.

Korsia S, Bottjer SW (1991) Chronic testosterone treatment impairs vocallearning in male zebra finches during a restricted period ofdevelopment. J Neurosci 11:2362-2371.

Kroodsma DE (1982) Song repertoires: problems in their definition anduse. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds. Vol. 2. SongLearning and its Consequences. (Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, eds.)pp.125-146. New York: Academic Press.

Kroodsma DE, Verner, J (1978) Complex singing behaviors amongCistothorus wrens. Auk 95:703-716.

Leonardo A, Konishi M (1999) Decrystallization of adult birdsong byperturbation of auditory feedback. Nature 399:466-470.

Liu W-C, Nottebohm, F (2005) Variable rate of singing and variable songduration are associated with high immediate early geneexpression in two anterior forebrain song nuclei. Proc NatlAcad Sci USA 102:10724-10729.

Marler P (1981) Birdsong: the acquisition of a learned motor skill. TrNeurosci 4:88-94.

Marler P (1989) Learning by Instinct: Birdsong. Am Sp Lang HearAssoc 31:75-79.

Marler P (1997) Three models of song learning: evidence from behavior.J Neurobiol 33:501-516.

Marler P (2004) Bird calls: Their potential for behavioral neurobiology.In: Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong (Ziegler HP, Marler P,eds.), pp31-44. New York: Ann NY Acad Sci.

Marler P, Sherman V (1983) Song structure without auditory feedback:Emendations of the auditory template hypothesis. J Neurosci

3:517-531.Marler P, Sherman V (1985) Innate differences in singing behavior of

sparrows reared in isolation from adult conspecific song. AnimBehav 33:57-71.

Marler P, Peters S (1977) Selective vocal learning in a sparrow.Science 198:519-521.

Marler P, Peters S (1988) The role of song phonology and syntax invocal learning preferences in the song sparrow, Melospizamelodia. Ethology 77:125-149.

Marler P, Peters S, Ball GF, Dufty AM, Wingfield J (1988) The role ofsex steroids in the acquisition and production of birdsong.Nature 336:770-772.

McEwen BS, Davis, PG, Parsons B, Pfaff, DW (1979) The brain as atarget for steroid hormone action. Ann Rev Neurosci 2:65-112.

Miyasato LE, Baker MC (1999) Black-capped chickadee call dialectsalong a continuous habitat corridor. Anim Behav 57:1311-1318.

Mennill DJ. Ratcliffe LM (2004) Overlapping and matching in the songcontests of black-capped chickadees. Anim Behav 67:441-450.

Morris D (1954) The reproductive behaviour of the zebra finch(Poephila guttata) with special reference to pseudofemalebehaviour and displacement activities. Behaviour 7:1-31.

Naguib M (1999) Effects of song overlapping and alternating onnocturnally singing nightingales. Anim Behav 58:1061-1067.

Naguib M, Todt D (1997) Effects of dyadic vocal interactions on otherconspecific receivers in nightingales. Anim Behav 54:1535-1543.

Nelson DA (1998) External validity and experimental design: thesensitive phase for song learning. Anim Behav 56:487-491.

Nordeen KW, Nordeen EJ (1992) Auditory feedback is necessary for themaintenance of stereotyped song in adult zebra finches. BehavNeur Biol 57:58-66.

Nottebohm F (1972) The origins of vocal learning. Am Nat 106:116-140.

Nottebohm F (1980) Testosterone triggers growth of brain vocal controlnuclei in adult female canaries. Br Res 189:429-437.

Nottebohm F, Arnold AP (1976) Sexual dimorphism in vocal controlareas of the songbird brain. Science 194:211-213.

Page 23: Bird song and singing behavior

23

Nottebohm F, Kasparian S, Pandazis C (1981) Brain space for a learnedtask. Br Res 213:99-109.

Nottebohm F, Nottebohm M (1978) Relationship between songrepertoire and age in the canary, Serinus canar ius. ZTierpsychol 46:298-305.

Nottebohm F, Nottebohm ME, Crane L (1986) Developmental andseasonal changes in canary song and their relation to changesin the anatomy of song-control nuclei. Behav Neur Biol 46:445-471.

Nottebohm F, Nottebohm M, Crane L, Wingfield JC (1987) Seasonalchanges in gonadal hormone levels of adult male canaries andtheir relation to song. Behav Neur Biol 47:197-211.

Nowicki S, Ball GF (1989) Testosterone induction of song inphotosensitive and photorefractory male sparrows. Horm Behav23:514-525.

Oberweger K, Goller F (2001) The metabolic cost of birdsongproduction. J Exp Biol 204:3379-3388.

Payne RB (1983) The social context of song mimicry: song-matchingdialects in indigo buntings. Anim Behav 31:788-805.

Pesch A, Güttinger H-R (1985) Der gesang des weiblichenKanarienvogels. J Ornithologie, 126:108-110.

Petrinovich L, Baptista, LF (1987) Song development in the white-crowned sparrow: modification of learned song. Anim Behav35:961-974.

Price P (1979) Developmental determinants of structure in zebra finchsong. J Comp Physiol Psychol 93:260-277.

Pytte CL, Suthers RA (2000) Sensitive period for sensorimotor integrationduring vocal motor learning. J Neurobiol 42:172-189.

Rasika S, Nottebohm F, Alvarez-Buylla A (1994) Testosterone increasesthe recruitment and/or survival of new high vocal centerneurons in adult female canaries. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA91:7854-7858.

Reiner A, Perkel DJ. Mello CV, Jarvis ED (2004) Songbirds and therevised avian brain nomenclature. In: Behavioral Neurobiologyof Birdsong (Ziegler HP, Marler P eds.), pp77-108. New York:Ann NY Acad Sci.

Riede T, Suthers RA, Fletcher NH, Blevins WE (2006) Songbirds tunetheir vocal tract to the fundamental frequency of their song.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:5543-5548.

Rose GJ, Goller F, Gritton HJ, Plamondon SL, Baugh AT, Cooper BG(2004) Species-typical songs in white-crowned sparrowstutored with only phrase pairs. Nature 432:753-8.

Rothstein SI, Fleisher RC (1987) Vocal dialects and their possiblerelation to status signaling in the brown-headed cowbird.Condor 89:1-23.

Scharff C, Nottebohm F (1991) A comparative study of the behavioraldeficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finchsong system: implications for vocal learning. J Neurosci11:2896-2913.

Sibley CG, Monroe BL (1990) Distribution and taxonomy of birds ofthe world. New Haven: Yale UP.

Simpson HB, Vicario DS (1990) Brain pathways for learned andunlearned vocalizations differ in zebra finches. J Neurosci10:1541-1556.

Smith GT, Brenowitz EA, Wingfield JC (1997) Roles of photoperiod andtestosterone in seasonal plasticity of the avian song controlsystem. J Neurobiol 32:426-442.

Sossinka R, Böhner J (1980) Song types in the zebra finch (Poephilaguttata castanotis). Z Tierpsychol 53:123-132.

Stoddard PK, Beecher MD, Campbell SE (1992) Song-type matching inthe song sparrow. Can J Zool 70:1440-1444.

Tchernichovski O, Lints T, Mitra PP, Nottebohm F (1999) Vocalimitation in zebra finches is inversely related to modelabundance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:12901-12904.

Tchernichovski O, Mitra PP, Lints T, Nottebohm F (2001) Dynamics ofthe vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song.Science 291:2564-2569.

Thorpe WH (1958) The learning of song patterns by birds, with especialreference to the song of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. Ibis100:535-570.

Thorpe WH (1961) Bird Song. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.Tramontin AD, Wingfield JC, Brenowitz EA (1999) Contributions of

social cues and photoperiod to seasonal plasticity in the adultavian song control system. J Neurosci 19:476-483.

Vicario DS (2004) Using learned calls to study sensory-motorintegration in songbirds. In: Behavioral Neurobiology ofBirdsong (Ziegler HP, Marler P eds), pp246-262. New York:Ann NY Acad Sci.

Page 24: Bird song and singing behavior

24

Vicario DS, Naqvi NH, Raksin JN (2001) Behavioral discrimination ofsexually dimorphic calls by male zebra finches requires anintact vocal motor pathway. J Neurobiol 407:109-120.

Vicario DS, Raksin JN, Naqvi NH, Thande N, Simpson HB (2002) Therelationship between perception and production in songbirdvocal imitation: what learned calls can teach us. J. CompPhysiol A 188:897-908.

Vignal C, Mathevon N, Mottin S (2004) Audience drives male songbirdresponse to partner voice. Nature 430:448-451.

Volman SF, Khanna H (1995) Convergence of untutored song in group-reared zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Psychol109:211-221.

Ward BC, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW (1998) Individual variation inneuron number predicts differences in the propensity for avianvocal imitation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:1277-1282.

Weisman R, Ratcliffe L (2004) Relative pitch and the song of the black-capped chickadee. Amer Sci 92:532:539

West MJ, King AP (1988) Female visual displays affect the developmentof male song in the cowbird. Nature 334:244-246.

Westneat, M.W., Long, J.H. Jr., Hoese, W., and Nowicki, S. (1993)Kinematics of birdsong: Functional correlation of cranialmovements and acoustic features in sparrows. J Exp Biol182:147-171.

Wild JM (2004) Functional neuroanatomy of the sensorimotor control ofsinging. In: Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong (Ziegler HP,Marler P, eds.), pp438-462. New York: Ann NY Acad Sci.

Williams H (1990) Models for song learning in the zebra finch: fathers orothers? Anim Behav 39:745-757.

Williams H (2001) Choreography of song, dance and beak movementsin the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Exp Biol 204:3497-3506.

Williams H, Connor DM, Hill JW (2003) Testosterone decreases thepotential for song plasticity in adult male zebra finches. HormBehav 44:402-412.

Williams H, Kilander K, Sotanski ML (1993) Untutored song,reproductive success and song learning. Anim Behav 45:695-705.

Williams H, McKibben JR (1992) Changes in stereotyped central vocalmotor patterns controlling vocalization are induced byperipheral nerve injury. Behav Neur Biol 57:67- 78.

Williams H, Mehta N (1999) Changes in adult zebra finch song requirea forebrain nucleus that is not necessary for song production. JNeurobiol 39:14-28.

Williams H, Staples K (1992) Syllable chunking in zebra finch(Taeniopygia guttata) song. J Comp Psychol 106:278-286.

Zann RA (1984) Structural variation in the zebra finch distance call. ZTierpsychol 66:328-345.

Zann RA (1985) Ontogeny of the zebra finch distance call: I. Effects ofcross-fostering to bengalese finches. Z Tierpsychol 68:1-23.

Zann RA (1996) The Zebra Finch: A synthesis of field and laboratorystudies. New York: Oxford UP.