“Assessing the Perception of Workplace Diversity. A Case ...
Embed Size (px)
Transcript of “Assessing the Perception of Workplace Diversity. A Case ...

CER
N-T
HES
IS-2
016-
286
01/1
0/20
16
European University Viadrina Frankfurt (Oder)
Faculty of Social and Cultural Sciences
Master Thesis
“Assessing the Perception of Workplace Diversity. A Case Study on Diversity Climate at CERN.”
Supervisor:
Prof. Dr. Jacek Sójka Dr. Marcin Poprawski
Author: Kristin Kaltenhäuser
Matrikel: 84674
Zu den Schafweiden 13 99098 Erfurt
Phone: +49 176 96369956
Email: [email protected]
Erfurt, 18 July 2016

1
Table of Contents
1 Introduction ......................................................................................................................................... 2
2 Literature review ................................................................................................................................. 4
2.1 Diversity in the workforce ............................................................................................................ 4
2.2 Effects of workplace diversity ...................................................................................................... 6
2.3 Managing diversity: Diversity initiatives and training .................................................................. 8
2.4 Diversity climate ........................................................................................................................ 12
3 The organisation CERN .................................................................................................................... 15
3.1 General facts ............................................................................................................................. 15
3.2 Diversity at CERN ..................................................................................................................... 16
4 Methodology ..................................................................................................................................... 21
4.1 ODNA – The instrument ............................................................................................................ 21
4.2 Adaptations to the CERN context ............................................................................................. 26
4.3 Procedure .................................................................................................................................. 27
4.4 The sample ............................................................................................................................... 27
4.5 Method of analysis .................................................................................................................... 31
5 Data analysis .................................................................................................................................... 33
5.1 Descriptive statistics and correlations of the subscales ............................................................ 33
5.2 Test of preconditions for statistical analysis .............................................................................. 35
5.3 Test of hypotheses .................................................................................................................... 36
5.3.1 Results for eight dimensions: H1 Gender .......................................................................... 37
5.3.2 Results for eight dimensions: H2 Nationality ..................................................................... 43
5.3.3 Results for eight dimensions: H3 Managerial responsibilities ........................................... 47
5.3.4 Results for eight dimensions: H4 Generation (Age) .......................................................... 50
5.3.5 Results for eight dimensions: H5 CERN Status ................................................................ 54
5.3.6 Summary of the results ...................................................................................................... 60
6 Discussion and recommendations ................................................................................................... 63
7 Discussion of methodology .............................................................................................................. 65
8 Tables and figures ............................................................................................................................ 67
9 References ....................................................................................................................................... 72
ANNEX 1 – Map of CERN Member States as of March 2016 ............................................................ 77
ANNEX 2 – Actions Points of CERN Diversity Programme ................................................................ 78
ANNEX 3 – The original ODNA ........................................................................................................... 79
ANNEX 4 – Questionnaire introduction text ........................................................................................ 82
ANNEX 5 – The Questionnaire – CERN version................................................................................. 83
ANNEX 6 – Cronbach’s alpha for all eight dimensions ....................................................................... 88
ANNEX 7 – Results ............................................................................................................................. 89
Declaration of Originality ................................................................................................................... 104

2
1 Introduction
“In the twentieth century, ecologists and agriculturalists developed an increasingly so-phisticated understanding of the value of biological diversity, specifically the resilience
and adaptability it brings to ecosystems. In the twenty-first century, the ecosystem model has been applied to human systems, particularly to understand how organisa-
tions are structured and how they operate.” (Kreitz 2008: 2)
Diversity has become a popular slogan for organisations in the 21st century. Many
companies use its promotion to attract customers, but also talent and a positive pub-
licity. Yet few employers have succeeded in implementing successful and effective
diversity management initiatives. Many agree that the management of diversity is im-
portant, but fail to put in place effective and impactful measures to manage a diverse
workforce. Educating and training employees in a diverse work setting has experi-
enced a rise in popularity and a rise in offers by experts all around the world. How-
ever, the basis of the implementation of a successful measure is the assessment of
the present situation.
Tschirhart and Wise (2000) suggest that such an assessment should entail the use
of existing research on the topic and eliciting of employees’ perception: “Studies with
implications for the practice of managing for diversity must focus on relationships
among workers with varying levels of heterogeneity.” (ibid.: 387)
This study will bring together research, employee perceptions and an inspection of
existing measures for a case study of the international organisation CERN, based in
Geneva, Switzerland. The goal will be to assess the diversity climate of the organisa-
tion, to serve as a basis for recommendations on effective and targeted diversity train-
ing initiatives to increase productivity and efficiency. The term diversity climate entails
attitudes of members of personnel towards diversity, their perception of the attitude
of people around them and how the organisation is perceived to support and manage
a diverse workforce.
The assessment will build on research that exists in this area. There is much research
that has been carried out in the area of workforce diversity, but there is no research

3
on workforce diversity in International Organisations. This is a unique setting, be-
cause these organisations are by definition culturally diverse, recruiting internationally
based on their setup.
A quantitative methodology approach was implemented to achieve the objective of
this research. The Organisational Needs Analysis (ODNA) developed by Prof. Molly
Dahm in 2009 was utilized as the data collection instrument. The instrument was de-
veloped to conduct a training needs analysis in organisations with a diverse work-
force. Social and psychological concepts are introduced and explained to interpret
the results of the data analysis.
Statistical analysis was carried out with the data collected with the ODNA to test the
following hypotheses:
H1 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on gender.
H2 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on nationality.
H3 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on level of managerial
responsibilities.
H4 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on generations.
H5 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on status at the organi-
sation.
The thesis is divided into six parts: The first part will give an overview over key re-
search and concepts in the areas of workforce diversity, its effects on an individual,
team and organisational level, as well as findings regarding the management of a
diverse workforce and the construct of diversity climate.
The second section will introduce CERN as an organisation and the setting of the
empirical research. Afterwards the methodology is explained and how it measures
the diversity climate of an organisation in general, as well as its adaptation to the
CERN context. The results of the data analysis are presented in chapter 5. The last
part will be a discussion of the results, as well as recommendations to the organisa-
tion and an evaluation of the methodology to explain its constraints.

4
2 Literature review
The following chapters will introduce the basic social and psychological concepts that
contribute to understanding dynamics of a diverse workforce, and summarize rele-
vant research on the topic of workforce diversity. Due to the popularity of the subject
in practice as well as the academic world, there is an extensive list of related research.
For the purpose of this study, the focus is initially on the term of diversity and social
dynamics in a diverse workforce. Afterwards literature reflecting the effect of a diverse
workforce on an individual, as well as group and organisational level will be reviewed,
followed by an assessment of recommended frameworks of managing diversity. The
last part of the literature review entails an examination of the implications of the con-
cept of diversity climate.
2.1 Diversity in the workforce
To understand the different perceptions that individuals have towards diversity in their
work environment, a first step is to give an overview of the different ways to define
the term.
The concept of diversity is an evolving one, since it has emerged in the business
context in the 1980s. Schneider and Northcraft (1999) note that in today’s business
world but also in research on the topic, people refer to different types of workforce
diversity. Many researchers and organisations use a vague definition to cover a broad
range of obvious and hidden qualities of an individual, such as Wentling and Palma-
Rivas (1998), who state: “Diversity is all the ways in which we differ,” (ibid.: 241).
Others prefer a more practicable approach to the term when used in the business
context and analyse diversity in terms of social category membership. Thus people
are assigned to social groups based on a shared characteristic. Organisational diver-
sity initiatives often base these groups on functional demographic traits which are
directly relevant to organisational performance, such as gender, age, ethnicity etc.
(Schneider, Northcraft 1999).
In this study, the definition of Harrison et al. (2000) was adopted, who distinguish
between surface-level and deep-level diversity. Surface-level diversity is understood

5
as differences in overt demographic characteristics, such as age, gender and ethnic-
ity. Harrison et al. note that not only organisations, but also individuals use these
characteristics to assign themselves and the people around them to groups, which
are ascribed certain attitudes and behaviors. These groups are often based on obvi-
ous demographic characteristics. The researchers found that individuals divide the
people they interact with into “in-groups”, with which they share traits they identify
with, as well as “out-groups”, which are social groups, they don’t identify with. In this
thesis, the term diversity, if not specified otherwise, connotes the various dimensions
of surface-level diversity.
Deep-level diversity on the other hand, refers to differences in psychological charac-
teristics, such as attitudes, personality, preferences and values. The paradigm of
deep-level diversity has begun to emerge in diversity research just recently and is
believed to be the basis of the effects of surface-level diversity, insofar as deep-level
traits are assumed to be the underlying motive for similarity-attraction (Byrne 1971)
and the definition of “in-groups”.
Harvey and Allard (2015) define the surface-level and deep-level dimensions of Har-
rison et al. according to their visibility: the dimensions of surface-level diversity are
the most visible and are primarily age, gender, race, mental and physical abilities,
ethnicity and sexual orientation. Harvey and Allard find them to be the more perma-
nent and less changeable, while most central to the in-group/out-group perception.
Whereas the deep-level dimensions are less visible.
This study focusses on surface-level diversity to assess the sample of members of
personnel at CERN. Cultural, age and gender diversity will be tested, as well as the
status of the individual in the organisation. Whereas the questions refer to surface-
level, as well as deep-level diversity, such as attitudes and values.
As broad as the definitions of diversity are, as much vary the methods to measure
and analyse it in an organisational context. Organisations and companies with a di-
verse workforce often track the demographics of their employees and compile the
data in a diversity report. Another measure of diversity aspects are surveys, as used
in this thesis. Konrad et al. (2006) note that the various ways of studying workplace
diversity provide scholars with different genres of workplace diversity research, which

6
has to be taken into account when reviewing literature on the topic and using it as a
basis for further research.
One example of a potential erroneous assumption is the case of Allen et al. (2007).
They study the effect diversity has on organisational performance by surveying the
“perception of workplace diversity” (ibid.: 20). They therefore utilize the same term as
this thesis but describe it as the degree employees “perceive their workplace to be
diverse” (Allen et al. 2007: 22), contrary to the present paper which defines the per-
ception of workplace diversity as the openness towards and appreciation of diversity
(see chapter 2.4 on diversity climate).
2.2 Effects of workplace diversity
The focus of this study is the assessment of individual perceptions of the diversity
climate in an organisation and therefore from a social-psychological viewpoint. This
chapter will connect this individual level perspective to the performance of teams and
an organisation as a whole, by giving an overview of the effects of a diverse work-
force.
The questions raised in this paper are based on the notion that in a complex social
environment such as a diverse organisation, the individual forms and maintains its
identity by identifying with social groups. As mentioned in the previous chapter, re-
search suggests that in-groups and out-groups are defined by individuals based on
mostly overt demographic traits, such as culture, age, gender etc.
The theoretical premises of these finding are the well supported social identity theory
(Tajfel, Turner 1986) and the corresponding similarity-attraction paradigm (Byrne
1971). The two theories propose that individuals form their social identity by affiliating
to a social group in their immediate environment (in-groups), whereas they prefer to
affiliate with individuals they perceive to be similar to themselves based on surface-
level diversity groups. Several studies found that this affiliation has positive outcomes
on collaboration in teams who share characteristics: “interpersonal similarity in-
creases ease of communication, improves predictability of behaviour, and fosters re-
lationships of trust and reciprocity.” (MorBarak et al. 1998: 88, also: Hobman et al.
2004).

7
As a result, in diverse organisations, where employees inevitably work with col-
leagues who are dissimilar, there are several theories suggesting that surface-level
diversity has a negative effect on the workforce due to the fact, that the individual
form unfavourable bias towards their out-groups: “Team members tend to have less
positive attitudes toward, and form fewer social attachments with those who are per-
ceived to be less like themselves” (Harrison et al. 2000: 5)
While the previously described research speaks about demographic traits and differ-
ences in general, there has been studies conducted featuring concrete surface-level
dimensions and specific performance outcomes of teams and the organisation as a
whole.
In the 1990s there were two opposing views emerging on whether a diverse workforce
has a generally positive or negative impact on performance. A group of organisational
scientists propose that team performance is enhanced by multiple perspectives of
people with different cultural backgrounds, gender and ages. Diversity is therefore
said to have positive effects on problem-solving, organisational flexibility and creativ-
ity based on the avoidance of a phenomenon called “groupthink”. This phenomenon
is defined as the absence of critical thinking in a homogeneous group due to the
preoccupation with maintaining the degree of cohesiveness in a group (Cox, Blake
1991). It was found that a certain degree of cultural diversity prevents this effect.
At the same time there was an opposing view, proving negative effects or none at all
of heterogeneous groups on team performance. Harrison et al. (2000) found that di-
versity has no benefit to team outcomes. Elvira and Cohen (2001) found a higher
turnover, as well as higher absenteeism, among employees of diverse workforces,
by examining personnel records of Fortune 500 companies. Jayne and Dipboye
(2004) and Tschirhart and Wise (2000) found by conducting empirical research on
collaboration in teams, effects such as less social integration of individuals, a lack of
team cohesion and greater communication problems. The studies suggest that these
negative effects occur with regard to several dimensions of surface-level diversity,
but mainly cultural, age and gender diversity.
In a fairly recent meta-analysis of research of the last 20 years on effects of a diverse
workforce on group performance by Joshi and Roh (2007), it was found that there are

8
a fairly equal amount of studies concluding positive effects and of studies finding neg-
ative effects of a cultural, gender and age diverse group on performance. Tschirhart
and Wise (2000) furthermore propose that “We cannot claim that diversity has any
clear positive or negative effect on individual, group or organisational outcomes.”
(ibid.: 392)
In any case, most scholars defending the positive effects of a diverse workforce agree
that these are not achieved by hiring more cultural, age, and gender diverse employ-
ees alone: “Simply having a diverse workforce does not necessarily produce the pos-
itive outcomes that are often claimed by some of the more optimistic proponents.”
(Jayne, Dipboye 2004: 411). They underline the importance of managing a diverse
workforce in order to reap its benefits and avoid possible negative effects: “Workforce
social category diversity is not so much a benefit to have as it is a problem if avoided”
(Schneider, Northcraft 1999: 1450)
In an age of globalisation, there is an inevitable trend towards changing de-
mographics in the general population and therefore an increase in workforce diversity
for most companies and organisations. Therefore, paying attention to and managing
a diverse workforce is not only a concern of organisational performance but also a
matter of improving relationships with employees, who are becoming more diverse,
enhancing social responsibility, or at least avoiding legal conflicts, according to
Wentling and Palma-Rivas (1998: 242), who interviewed a group of diversity experts
in the American workforce,
2.3 Managing diversity: Diversity initiatives and training
This chapter will give a brief overview of the rationale behind diversity management
as well as a theoretical framework of diversity training efforts.
As shown, managing diversity is a strategic business issue for organisations in times
of globalisation. Historically, it evolved originally in the USA to provide a legally de-
fensive position against charges of discrimination, mainly in the form of affirmative-

9
action programmes. However, in the 1990s diversity management emerged beyond
legal compliance: the premise became to not only recognize but leverage the benefits
of a diverse workforce described in the previous chapters, while avoiding possible
detriments. Managing diversity can be described as “an approach to fair treatment
that encourages employers to harness and value a wide range of visible differences
in their employees” (Foster, Harris 2005).
The starting point of every diversity initiative is as Digh (1998) states, that the man-
agement must “articulate, clearly and simply, what is meant by diversity and then
decide what approach to take. Does the organisation want to tolerate, celebrate,
value, manage, harness or leverage diversity?” (ibid.: 117).
There have been numerous studies on how to improve specifically Human Resource
practices to ensure fair processes throughout an organisation (e.g. Shen et al. 2009).
Cox (1994) suggests to address diversity on three levels: organisational, group and
individual level. All of them are tightly linked together, which can be explained with
the concept of organisational culture.
According to Ravasi and Schultz (2006) organisational culture is a set of shared as-
sumptions that guides what happens in an organisation by defining appropriate be-
haviors for various situations (ibid.). When managing diversity, these shared assump-
tions among employees need to be identified, analysed and addressed: “Managing
diversity means changing the culture - that is, the standard operating procedures. It
requires data, experimentation, and the discovery of the procedures that work best
for each group. It is more complex than conventional management but can result in
more effective organisations.” (Triandis et al. 1992: 773). Therefore, ensuring an in-
clusive organisational culture starts with a thorough assessment of needs, which
serve as a basis for the planning and building of targeted initiatives.
The assessment tool used in this study, the ODNA was developed to evaluate the
organisational culture and specifically the diversity climate of an organisation to be
utilised as a basis for developing successful diversity initiatives. The goal is to lay the
ground for giving recommendations on the areas to be targeted by diversity trainings
but also broader initiatives, by probing the individual perception of diversity in the
organisation. The underlying assumption here is that an impact on individual level,
will eventually lead to an impact on group as well as organisational level.

10
Jayne and Dipboye (2004) warn, that there is no one-fits-all solution. Every organisa-
tion has its own delicacies that must be taken into account when developing diversity
initiatives. They argue however that good diversity programs have concrete goals and
action plans.
A common tool, and integral part of diversity management from its beginnings are
targeted diversity trainings. Ferdman and Brody (1996) conducted a comprehensive
analysis of diversity training models. They found that efforts can vary broadly in how
they are conceptualized and implemented, although most training initiatives focus on
domestic diversity and its implications for the workplace. The goals of training initia-
tives are often framed as either individual development and growth, or as a tool to
improve productivity.
When analysing the positioning of training initiatives in the timeline of an overall di-
versity effort, they identify two perspectives: Some scholars advocate to begin the
effort with educating employees and especially management. They reason that peo-
ple are drivers for change (ibid.: 295). Another group of researchers argue that a
change in attitude is a long-term process, which an organisation in today’s economy
cannot wait for. They point out that “behavioural changes will foster corresponding
changes in attitudes, rather than the other way around.” (ibid.) Thus it is believed that
system changes have to be made before emphasizing trainings. Both sides propose
valid arguments, however further research suggests that isolated diversity trainings
are less effective than a comprehensive diversity program that works on both fronts
(Paluck 2006: 585).
Ferdman and Brody propose four objectives for diversity training programs: The pro-
vision of knowledge and information, as well as the increase in awareness and un-
derstanding of differences are suggested to be a common goal among organisations.
Furthermore, the development of skills to bridge these differences, mainly through
communication training, is suggested as the necessary next step to have an impact
on the organisation’s diversity climate.
Successfully implemented trainings often feature two types of learning: didactic and
experiential learning. The basic premise of the didactic learning is that a reciprocal
understanding of other cultures is vital when it comes to interactions in a diverse
workplace. Its focus is to provide a knowledge base for trainees. Experiential learning

11
on the other hand involves active participation and engagement as a mean to
strengthen intercultural sensitivity. According to Ferdman and Brody the tools of ex-
periential learning such as role plays and group discussions on the basis of personal
experiences, promote communication skills and affect attitudes, who can lead from
individual change to group and organisational change: “For many trainees, it is the
first time they realize that not everyone experiences the work environment in the
same way they do and that the variations are often connected to racial, ethnic and
gender identities.” (ibid.: 296)
There has been little reliable research conducted to measure diversity training suc-
cess. Studies either lack a sufficient sample size or miss the establishment of casual
effects of training components (Paluck 2006). The widely adopted assumption that
training will diminish the negative effects of a diverse workforce (as described in chap-
ter 2.2) and enforce the positive effects, such as a lower employee turnover, reduced
absenteeism and increased productivity (Dahm 2009, Larkey 1996) was never sup-
ported by properly conducted research.
One exception is a thorough study by Duguid and Thomas-Hunt (2015) who re-
searched the effect of raising awareness on stereotyping, such as unconscious bias.
Training on unconscious bias has become a very popular version of diversity training
within organisations across the US and Europe. It can be delivered in various ways
(e.g. e-learning, seminars and workshops), and aims to raise awareness to the fact,
that all people harbour implicit bias and to teach the trainees to be more mindful in
situations, where these might occur. Duguid and Thomas-Hunt’s findings point out
that unconscious bias awareness trainings can unintentionally encourage more bi-
ased thinking and behaviors among employees. They suggest that trainees feel less
motivated to change biases by hearing that others are biased and it’s ‘normal’ to hold
stereotypes.
Another problem is the transfer of training to the work situation. Since the training
context is different from the ongoing work context, it can be difficult for the trainees to
adapt the behaviors they learned in the training environment (Kossek et al. 2005: 63).
The review of the literature suggests, that more research is needed to assess the
positive effects of diversity training initiatives. When it comes to the most impactful
training concept, there is a lack of reliable research, but also the failure of training

12
programs can be attributed to a lack of a suitable training needs assessment (Kirk-
patrick 1998). It is therefore essential to identify and address specific training targets
to enable a reliable assessment of training success. The assessment of the diversity
climate can result in recommendations on possible training targets. Depending on the
findings, alternative initiatives have to be taken into account to address the identified
issues.
2.4 Diversity climate
The diversity climate is part of the organisational culture, because it is as Cox (1994)
argues, determined by individual, group and organisational factors. “Diversity climate
refers to employee behaviors and attitudes that are grounded in perceptions of the
organisational context related to women and minorities” (MorBarak et al. 1998: 83).
The underlying assumption is, that employees’ behavior is driven by perceptions,
whether or not they are consistent with reality (Ensher et al. 2001: 53). This work
takes over this premise and therefore focusses on the assessment of the perceptions
of the employees and their openness towards and appreciation of the diversity in the
organisation.
Wentling and Palma-Rivas (1998) interviewed workforce diversity experts throughout
the US and found a consensus that the biggest barriers of leveraging the value of
diversity are the “negative attitudes to and the discomfort around people who are
different” (ibid.: 241). This can be due to the similarity-attraction paradigm, described
in chapter 2.2: people feel generally more comfortable around people who are similar
to them. Discomfort also comes from the widespread and often unconscious assump-
tion that approaches which differ from personal approaches are inferior. Kreitz (2008)
states that individuals prefer working in homogeneous groups and are prone to “avoid
and resist change” (Kreitz 2008: 4), which poses the main challenge for a positive
diversity climate in an organisation.
With regards to the social identity theory, which states that people create their social
identity by dividing the people around them in in- and out-groups (see chapter 2.1), a
strong diversity climate implies that the employees identify not only with groups based
on demographic characteristics, but simultaneously with their organisation. They dis-
play a dual identity (Hofhuis et al. 2011: 966).

13
Kreitz (2008) argues that a goal of diversity management should be to change peo-
ple’s attitudes towards differences, and therefore how people interact. For this pur-
pose it is important to study and predict the range of interactions that occur in cultur-
ally diverse organisations and implement targeted communication and awareness
training initiatives.
This study is built on the assumption that surface-level diversity such as gender, age
and nationality determine the attitude towards diversity in the immediate (work) envi-
ronment. Research suggest e.g. that women have a more positive attitude towards
diversity than men (MorBarak et al. 1998). However, Sawyerr et al. (2005) found that
gender, race and age don’t generally determine the attitude towards differences, but
it’s more the underlying value structure of the individual. They found significant cor-
relations between value constructs such as openness to change, self-enhancement
and self-transcendence. Age, gender and race are likely to have a moderating effect
but don’t determine the attitude towards diversity generally. This implies that the
deep-level diversity determines attitudes towards diversity more severely than sur-
face-level diversity.
Kossek et al. (2005) found that individuals have more favourable attitudes towards
diversity initiatives when their work teams are more diverse, regardless of their own
demographics. Therefore, their attitudes towards organisational efforts to promote
cultural and gender diversity were more positive compared to other teams with less
diversity (ibid.: 60). They underline the importance to study to which extent employees
support an organisation’s pronounced value of diversity. The results can have impli-
cations on how future diversity initiatives are accepted.
It can be concluded that the diversity climate as well as the composition of the imme-
diate work team can predict the acceptance of diversity initiatives such as training.
Diversity training however is also aimed at improving the attitudes towards diversity
and the capability of individuals to work in a diverse team and leverage its benefits.
Cox (1994) argues that improvements in the diversity climate by introducing diversity
initiatives, enhances organisational effectiveness, measured by outcomes such as
productivity, quality, less turnover, better problem-solving and profitability.

14
One strategy to involve and engage employees early in the process, is assessing the
diversity climate. It is suggested to help to set the stage for future initiatives and give
the employees a chance to be involved in the design of them: “Assessments take into
consideration the experiences and attitudes of employees from different backgrounds
and signal to them that they are valued in the organisation.” (Cukier, Smarz 2012).
As demonstrated, research proposes the assessment of the diversity climate to early
identify possible conflicts or negative developments for the organisation. Further, data
collected over time, using the same instrument, can be used to evaluate the effec-
tiveness of organisational diversity initiatives. Kreitz (2008) calls for a systematic di-
versity climate assessment based on qualitative and quantitative research, because
it “studies root causes rather than symptoms” (ibid.:10). There are several tools avail-
able for practitioners to assess diversity, as well as diversity climate. Cukier and
Smarz (2012) compared and evaluated the most prominent tools in the US. They
conclude that every tool uses a different “diversity lens” (ibid.: 59), meaning the ap-
proaches to measuring the diversity climate in an organisation vary. The tool used in
this study, the ODNA was chosen because it is based on previous versions of as-
sessment tools, and an extensive review of literature on the topic. It will be further
explained in chapter 4.1.
Hofhuis et al. (2011) furthermore propose to include the attitude of the organisation
when studying the perception of diversity. They define the diversity climate as follows:
“Diversity climate is the degree to which an organisational climate facilitates the pres-
ence of cultural differences, and views this diversity climate as an asset.” (ibid.: 969).
This position is taken into account in chapter 3 of this thesis, where the organisation
CERN as the place where the study was carried out is described as well as its diver-
sity policy and training initiatives.

15
3 The organisation CERN
The following chapter will give an overview of the organisational structure at CERN,
as well as the evolvement of diversity management at the organisation and the most
relevant recent diversity initiatives. It explains the organisation’s attitude towards di-
versity and gives a context to the empirical study, which is described in section 4 of
this thesis.
3.1 General facts
CERN, the European Organisation for Nuclear Research, is the world’s largest parti-
cle physics laboratory, based in Geneva, Switzerland. It has the status of an intergov-
ernmental organisation and is made up of 21 members states. The member states,
funding the organisation, are mostly European states and Israel, so far the only Non-
European state which was granted membership (for a map of all member states, see
Annex 1). CERN was founded in 1954 with the goal to provide and operate particle
accelerators and other infrastructure needed for high-energy physics research.
While the members of personnel (MPE) account for around 3000 of the CERN popu-
lation, there are an additional approximated 12 000 visiting scientists, associates and
users of the laboratory (MPA), who are using the CERN facilities1. For the purpose of
this study it is important to understand the population of the CERN site to grasp the
nature of its diversity. The MPEs are employed and paid by the research organisation.
They are sourced mainly from the member states, accounting for their financial con-
tribution to the organisation. The 12 000 MPAs of the laboratory are not employed by
CERN but sent by their home institutes. These collaborating institutes are mostly re-
search institutes and universities, which are located all over the world. Collaboration
agreements govern the relationship between the institutes and CERN and the dis-
patch of students and researchers, either full- or part-time, to carry out research at
the particle accelerators and other facilities at CERN.
1 http://cds.cern.ch/record/2154389/files/CERN-HR-STAFF-STAT-2015.pdf?version=1, access 07/0672016.

16
The organisation’s main purpose is to facilitate and carry out fundamental research
in the area of particle physics. CERN’s official mission statement also poses (1) the
advancement of technology and their impact on society, (2) the training of scientists
and engineers2 and (3) bringing nations together through science, as raison d’être of
its existence3.
The fact that CERN is by its constitution obliged to hire from all its 21 member states,
as well as the fostering of cross-cultural collaboration as part of its mission statement,
makes a culturally diverse workforce the premise.
3.2 Diversity at CERN
Diversity is embedded in the declared values of the organisation, as stated in the
CERN Code of Conduct: “CERN’s excellence derives from an environment in which
the knowledge and perspectives of a diverse workforce are valued and dialogue is
encouraged at all levels.”4
Organisational efforts to analyse and manage diversity at CERN go back to 1993
when an advisory group was created to reflect on the situation of women at CERN
and to address the gender imbalance in the organisation. This effort emerged into an
equal opportunity policy in 1996, as well as the appointment of an officer to enforce
this policy and the launch of a diversity programme in 20115.
The management and enhancement of gender diversity was therefore the starting
point and main focus of the diversity efforts in the organisation. The equal opportunity
policy from 1996 widens this scope and includes two other surface-level dimensions.
2 In accordance with this point, there are various student programmes at CERN, which are also rele-vant to this study. The Administrative and Technical student programme are aimed at undergraduate university students who spend up to 14 months at CERN during their studies as interns. A trainee is also an undergraduate intern at CERN. This programme is not tied to an official selection committee, as is the case for the Administrative and Technical student programme, and the internship cannot be longer than 6 months. The Doctoral student programme employs PhD students at CERN conducting their doctoral thesis in various fields. 3 http://jobs.web.cern.ch/content/our-mission, access: 07/06/2016. 4 http://jobs.web.cern.ch/content/culture-and-values, access: 07/06/2016. 5 More about the history of diversity at CERN: http://diversity.web.cern.ch/2015/03/equal-opportunities-diversity-1996-today.

17
It “rules out discrimination between members of its personnel on account of sex, race
or religion.” (Equal Opportunities Policy at CERN)6
The Diversity Programme launched in 2011 broadens the scope further. The CERN
diversity policy covers the following dimensions: gender, nationality/culture, profes-
sion, age/generation and individual differences7. Individual differences include ethnic
origin, sexual orientation, belief, disability, and opinions. It therefore takes into ac-
count surface-level as well as some deep-level diversity dimensions.
In 2008, before the launch of the diversity programme, the Equal Opportunities Office
carried out a survey among CERN staff members (MPE) The questionnaire had two
goals: “to spread awareness of equal opportunities at CERN”, as well as gather in-
sight into the issues CERN employees face in the context of diversity8. The question-
naire addressed the perception of harassment and discrimination at CERN, as well
as fair treatment in recruitment, career advancement and parental support. It also
investigated the knowledge of existing support structures and processes. Before the
launch of the Diversity Office, there were also interviews with employees, investigat-
ing expectations on the role of a diversity office, as well as general questions to in-
quire the perception of diversity support structures at CERN.
CERN therefore approached diversity management as described and recommended
in chapter 2.3 with collecting data before putting in place initiatives, with the benefit
of meeting assessed needs and involving employees in the diversity management
effort from the start. This is described in the CERN Diversity Policy:
“The Diversity Programme is monitored internally through surveys, interviews and
studies with in CERN with the aim of collecting input and feedback from both CERN
Management and CERN Contributors in order to continually adjust and align ac-
tions to the needs of the Organisation.” (CERN Diversity Policy, p. 5)9.
6 https://cds.cern.ch/record/1474077/files/1996_EOAPuk_leaflet.pdf, access 07/06/2016 (restricted access). 7 http://diversity.web.cern.ch/sites/diversity.web.cern.ch/files/DiversityPolicy.pdf, access, 07/06/2016. 8 Results of the Questionnaire on Equal Opportunities at CERN (2008). https://cds.cern.ch/rec-ord/1474082, access: 07/06/2016 (restricted access). 9 http://diversity.web.cern.ch/sites/diversity.web.cern.ch/files/DiversityPolicy.pdf, access, 07/06/2016.

18
However, the posed questions inquire broadly about fairness, discrimination and di-
versity management, without probing the understanding of these terms and the atti-
tude the employees have towards colleagues who are different.
Furthermore, a big part of the population at CERN, the 12 000 visiting scientists, user
and associates weren’t included in the survey. While it is true that many diversity
efforts are Human Resource activities, aimed at the individuals who are employed by
the organisation, the MPAs are a part of the organisation and influence the diversity
climate significantly. Therefore, they should be included in surveying the perception
of diversity at CERN.
The current activities of the Diversity Office can be tracked via the website of the
programme: http://diversity.web.cern.ch/. It shows that they are categorized into three
areas: recruitment, career development and work environment10. For the period from
2012 to 2015, there are seven strategic objectives defined, related to these areas and
with regard to the diversity dimensions (see Annex 2). For each objective there are
distinct actions. These actions imply an emphasis on addressing organisational pro-
cesses, in areas such as recruitment and career advancement, but also the provision
of support structures and the organisation of workshops and events to address an
inclusive work environment. The objectives as well as the CERN diversity website
imply that the topic of gender diversity is a main focus of the programme, which could
be explained by its historical evolvement from a working group addressing gender
imbalance at the organisation.
As mentioned in chapter 2.3, this thesis focusses on diversity competency trainings.
The following section will give a brief overview over the conducted trainings and
events, that can be seen as addressing an inclusive work environment and a positive
diversity climate.
Every half year CERN offers a diversity training that aims to “help to develop greater
sensitivity to differences and explore ways to recognise and overcome biases”11. The
half-day workshop involves group discussions and exercises, live audience surveys
10 http://diversity.web.cern.ch/framework/strategic-objectives, access 07/06/2016. 11 http://diversity.web.cern.ch/2016/05/diversity-action-workshop-7th-edition, access 07/06/2016.

19
and the provision of information on unconscious bias and diversity in general. It there-
fore features a mixture between experiential and didactic learning. The training is
offered for the whole CERN population, taking into account that all people on CERN
premises are part of an inclusive work environment. It is a non-mandatory training.
The goal of the workshop is framed as individual growth for the participants. As for
the objectives for diversity training as categorized by Ferdman and Brody (1996, see
chapter 2.3), the workshop aims to raise awareness, but also to develop skills, such
as dissecting and eliminating unconscious bias, when recognized.
Another training, that can be categorized as diversity training, are two full-day work-
shops on communication skills. This training is mandatory for all new staff members
at CERN since 2015, but open to the whole CERN population. It addresses commu-
nication skills, taking into account different personalities and different cultural com-
munication styles.
Furthermore, CERN offers a workshop which addresses gender inclusive writing in
the French language, offered by a language expert at CERN. This is a voluntary train-
ing aimed at writers in the CERN context, mainly administrative assistants and em-
ployees in the outreach and communication sections.
A last example of a training initiative at CERN, is a one-day workshop aimed at hiring
managers, to ensure a fair and competency based selection process at CERN. It is a
mandatory training, aimed at managers at CERN, who are about to be part of a se-
lection committee. Its goal is to prepare the participants for a structured selection
interview, that focusses on competencies, rather than subjectivity.
Furthermore, the Diversity Office has organized a number of talks and events to ad-
dress certain topics related to diversity.
While the former mentioned trainings involve both didactic as well as experiential
learning types, the talks feature didactic learning methods, providing knowledge of
contemporary research in the respective areas. One example, which took place in the
timeframe of this research, is a scientific empirical study with the CERN population,
investigating expectations on early career support from CERN12. An aim was to find
differences in the expectation of men and women. The presentation to the CERN
12 http://diversity.web.cern.ch/2016/03/support-early-careers-science-cern-understanding-expecta-tions, access 08/06/2016.

20
population, which was at the same time the object of the research was intentional, to
raise awareness on the issue.
The previous explanations show, that there are training initiatives in place at CERN
already to address the diversity of its workforce. It is aimed at different groups of
employees with varying goals, ranging from raising awareness to developing skills.
The following part of the study aims to analyse the CERN population with regard to
their perception of diversity and to give recommendations in addition to the diversity
trainings and support structures already in place.

21
4 Methodology
The following chapters will introduce the survey, used to carry out the quantitative
research, as well as the measured constructs (or subscales), the adaptations that
have been made to tailor it to the specific context of CERN and the procedure of the
data collection.
4.1 ODNA – The instrument
The Organisational Diversity Needs Analysis (ODNA) is a survey created by the so-
cial psychologist Dr. Molly Dahm (Lamarr University, USA) in 2009 (Dahm 2009). It
was designed to measure the perception and awareness of diversity in the workplace,
using eight dimensions (subscales): organisational inclusion, cultural group inclusion,
valuing differences, work load, Affirmative-Action Group Perception, trust, adaptation
and sensitivity/ flexibility.
The questions are to be rated on a five-point Likert-scale, ranging from “Strongly Dis-
agree” to “Strongly Agree”. A list of the original 53 questions, covering the dimensions
of the tool, can be found in Annex 3. Dahm constructed the questions based on the
study of literature and the learnings from previously constructed instruments, which
were used in the field, primarily the WDQ (Workforce Diversity Questionnaire), devel-
oped by Larkey (1996). Larkey’s approach was to predict communicative behaviour
to occur in diverse work teams (ibid.). Dahm altered and extended the scales accord-
ing to her own statistical and literature research to reflect the perception of diversity
among employees. Dahm conducted two field tests to ensure the validity of the new
instrument, the ODNA.
The advantage of using an existing instrument to analyse the diversity needs of an
organisation, is the reduction of risks and mistakes that can be made when using an
untested questionnaire. The ODNA can rely on previous research, statistical valida-
tion and experiences with previous diversity assessment tools. That is why it was
chosen to serve for the purpose of this study, with only slight wording adjustments to
fit the context of CERN. Furthermore, it has been used before to assess diversity
climate in companies and organisations (e.g. Brown 2008).

22
Dahm created a “general, theoretically anchored measure of diversity needs [...] used
to evaluate the existing diversity climate in an organisation” (Dahm 2009: 283). It was
constructed to identify what is important, relevant and meaningful among diverse em-
ployees. Some of the questions refer specifically to age, gender and cultural diversity
as a basis of individual perception, while others speak more broadly about group
similarities or differences. ODNA results are intended to identify attitudes based on
group identification as explained in chapter 2.1 and to what extent employees support
the organisation’s pronounced value of diversity. Dahm uses the principles and pro-
cesses of a training needs analysis, while putting an emphasis on diversity. The
scales were identified as relevant and expected to exist among a diverse workforce
(Dahm 2009).
The following section will give a description of the eight scales that are covered by
the ODNA, what they measure and how they fit into the overarching construct of di-
versity climate. In the analysis of the results, the subscales are analysed as the de-
pendent variables or “outcome variable”. Dahm divided them into two groups: the first
two dimensions are organisational dimensions, and indicate the perception of the or-
ganisation’s inclusiveness. The other six dimensions measure the individual’s views
and prejudices towards people who are different. The dimension of Valuing Differ-
ences is an exception and measures both individual attitude, as well as the perception
of their immediate work environment.
1. Cultural Group Inclusion reflects the in-group/out-group perception on a group
level. It represents two aspects: the individual’s personal identification with a cultural
group and the marginalization experiences based on that group membership. The
term “cultural group” in this case doesn’t refer to national or ethnic implications, but
rather generally to social identity groups based on a distinct demographic character-
istic.
This dimension as well as Organisational Inclusion is based on the social identity
theory, explained in chapter 2.1, which argues that individuals create their social iden-
tity by assigning themselves and the people around them to groups, based on demo-
graphic characteristics. Larkey (1996) and Schneider and Northcraft (1999) both pro-
pose, that the more individuals interact within their group and with people they share

23
a common social identity with and limit interaction with out-groups, the more groups
become differentiated from each other, which further affects patterns of interaction.
Therefore, the formation of groups has a reflexive nature (Larkey 1996: 300). The
segregation of employee groups can lead to the exclusion of minority groups in the
network of information, contacts and career advancement. The higher the score in
this subscale, the more negatively it reflects on the overall diversity climate in an
organisation. A training initiative could address groups that feel marginalized. This
subscale uses seven items/questions.
2. Organisational Inclusion reflects the in-group/out-group perception on an individual
level. It indicates the degree of affiliation the individual feels towards the organisation
(Dahm 2009: 294) and inclusion in the network of information and career opportuni-
ties. Results can show if the individual feels marginalized in the organisation based
on individual traits. For example, a person that feels excluded from the organisation,
would agree with question 8: “It seems that the real reason people are denied pro-
motions or raises is that they are not seen as fitting in.” (see Annex 3). Therefore, a
high agreement rating in this scale reflects negative on the overall diversity climate of
the organisation.
A prediction can be made when testing this dimension for the gender categories.
MorBarak et al. (1998) found in their study that “Men perceive the organisational di-
mension more favourably (more fair and inclusive) than did women […]” (ibid.: 97).
They conducted a study on the perception of diversity in an environment where men
pose the majority, similar to the CERN context. This subscale uses seven items.
3. Valuing differences. This personal dimension reflects two aspects: Firstly, the atti-
tude of the individual towards people who are different from themselves and whether
or not they perceive differences to bring added value to the organisation. The ques-
tions are constructed in a “we-mode”, underlining the positive aspects of diversity and
are meant to reflect societal platitudes about diversity (Dahm 2009: 295). The ques-
tions are proposed to indicate how much the individual is willing to support the pro-
claimed value of diversity, but not go as far as to reflect whether it is only lip-service

24
that is paid to the organisation’s value, or the individual truly supports diversity initia-
tives. The second aspect tested, is the perception of how differences are valued in
the immediate work environment.
MorBarak et al. (1998) found that “Group membership, is a powerful variable influ-
encing attitudes toward the value of diversity for the organisation” (MorBarak et al.
1998: 87). They argue that individuals of minority cultural and gender groups tend to
value differences more, than the majority group. This could be due to the fact that
minority groups are often perceived to be the one being different from the majority.
This subscale uses seven items.
4. Work load. Employees’ attitudes towards their work load has implications on their
desire to accommodate differences (Dahm 2009: 296). It is proposed that the heavier
the perceived work load is, the less open an individual is to pay attention to and em-
brace different working styles. In the evolvement of the questionnaire this scale
proved to be an important indicator of how an employee conforms to organisationally
prescribed behaviors. A higher work load therefore influences the diversity climate
negatively. This subscale uses four items.
5. Trust describes the feeling of loyalty the individual has towards its work colleagues
as well as the organisation. It brings up the notion of group identity again, which is a
possible obstacle when e.g. cultural group identity overrides commitment to the or-
ganisation (Dahm 2009: 296). Question 26 is an example for that: “People of the
same nationality, gender, belief or sexual orientation tend to look out for each other”
Results can reflect on undercurrents, despite professed valuing of differences (di-
mension 3). Trust therefore tends to be more difficult to achieve among members of
a diverse workforce (Dahm 2009). This subscale uses six items.
6. Affirmative-Action Group Perception. This dimension provides insight into the atti-
tudes employees have towards affirmative-action initiatives. Affirmative-action is a
product of the American civil rights movement in the 1960s. In essence it was per-
ceived by many companies as a governmental mandate to hire more people belong-
ing to a minority in the US. Shortly after, it has triggered many heated discussions

25
worldwide, as the preferential treatment of one group, implies necessarily a disad-
vantage for another, the majority group.
For the purpose of this study, affirmative-action is understood as the preferential treat-
ment of minority groups mainly in hiring and career advancement, but also other rel-
evant activities of an organisation. It enforces a we-they perception and can be per-
ceived as either positive or negative by individuals: “Minorities endorsed affirmative-
action programmes more than employees representing the majority.” (MorBarak et
al. 1998: 87). A high agreement score may reflect negatively on the organisation’s
diversity climate, because it can indicate a we-they perception (Dahm 2009: 296).
This dimension however has to be treated carefully. A low score can also indicate a
high percentage of majority group members, who regard affirmative-action as disad-
vantageous for themselves. This subscale uses six items.
7. Adaptation. This dimension reflects two aspects. It indicates how much the individ-
ual feels the need to adjust to colleagues’ different work styles to work effectively.
Secondly, it also reveals the motivation to do so (Dahm 2009: 296). A high agreement
rate in this scale therefore suggests a positive diversity climate. This subscale uses
seven items.
8. Sensitivity/Flexibility. This dimension picks up on Cox’s (1994) argument, that mi-
norities are and are perceived to be more sensitive to differences and flexible when
it comes to adapting to them (Dahm 2009: 297). This argument is based on the as-
sumption that minorities had to adjust to differences in the past and therefore more
flexible. The scale indicates to what degree minorities are perceived to be different
by their colleagues. A high agreement rate in this scale therefore indicates a strong
in-group/out-group perception of the employees, which can be a hindrance to the
overall diversity climate. This subscale uses seven items.

26
4.2 Adaptations to the CERN context
Without endangering the integrity of the questionnaire, some modifications have been
made in the wording of the questions to adapt them to the CERN context. The original
questionnaire has been developed in the US and therefore emphasizes aspects of
diversity that are specific to that context. One example is the use of the term “people
of color” to describe racial minorities in the US. This chapter will give an overview
over the alterations that have been made for the specific context of this study.
Some items in the original questionnaire referenced specifically to age, gender and
racioethnicity, while other items speak about broad group similarities and differences.
Dahm took over this strategy from Larkey 1996 who used specific questions as well
as general difference items, to address the difficulty in obtaining unbiased responses
when referring specifically to race and gender. Respondents directly connect the con-
cepts of racism and discrimination to these wordings, and therefore answer socially
expected, rather than stating their own opinion (Larkey 1996). The researcher took
over the interchanging use of specific and general terms, but adjusted the wording to
the context of the CERN population and the terms used in its diversity policy. There-
fore, the word “minorities” was used when specifying the groups referred to. In the
explanation of the questions for the survey respondents, the term “minority” was de-
scribed to include the diversity dimensions gender, nationality, belief, sexual orienta-
tion and physical ability.
Responses to the items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale and coded from 1
to 5, ranging from “strongly disagree“ (1) to “strongly agree“ (5). The only exception
is the second dimension “Organisational Inclusion”. In this dimension, a fifth option
“N/A” was added after sending out three test-surveys. It takes into account that the
performance of members of personnel under certain status at CERN are not evalu-
ated at the organisation, but by the supervisors at their home institute. This is the
case e.g. for visiting scientists and users. The additional option aims to avoid falsifi-
cation of the results.
A number of demographic questions regarding age group, gender, nationality, status
at the organisation, department, home institute (only applicable for the status of

27
“user”) and supervisory/managerial responsibilities were asked of the participants
(see Annex 5). Additionally, it was asked if the respondents “feel part of a minority at
CERN other than related to [their] gender or nationality (e.g. sexual orientation, phys-
ical ability, belief, etc.)?”. This last question however was not used in the analysis of
the survey.
4.3 Procedure
The questionnaire was designed as a randomized trial. The assessment took place
between 15 February 2016 and 10 March 2016. A purposive snowball sampling strat-
egy was adopted. “Purposive sampling is described as a random selection of sam-
pling units within the segment of the population with the most information on the char-
acteristic of interest.” (Guarte, Barrios 2006). Before the assessment, three test-sur-
veys were sent out to volunteers, after which one of the subscales was adapted (see
chapter 4.2).
The questionnaire was sent via email and posted in the Facebook group “Young at
CERN” with over 5000 members at the time of the post. The group is a platform of
exchange between current and former employees and associates of the organisation,
regardless of their status. This strategy ensured to include a variety of CERN status
in the questionnaire. Respondents were asked to confirm, that they were present at
the organisation for at least 4 months during the last year. It could therefore be as-
sumed that the participants have an adequate knowledge of their respective teams
and the organisation for the purpose of this research. They were informed, that the
questionnaire would take about 15 minutes and their answers would remain anony-
mous (Annex 4). The data analysis was conducted with SPSS.
4.4 The sample
The sample comprises 183 cases. Table 1 shows an overview over the five inde-
pendent variables13 used to group and analyse the sample.
13 Independent variables are cause variables, as opposed to the eight subscales of the ODNA, which are the outcome variables. The present study will assess how the variation in the outcome variable depends on the independent variable.

28
Table 1: Sample and independent variables
Independent variable
Groups
N %
Gender (N = 183) Male 98 54%Female
85
46%
Nationality (N = 183) European 154 84%Non-European
29
16%
Level of managerial responsibilities (N = 183)
People with supervisory responsi-bilities
41 22%
People with no supervisory respon-sibilities
142
78%
Generation (Age) (N = 182) Generation X (36-45) 34 19%
Generation Y (26-35) 104 57%Generation Z (18-25)
44
24%
CERN Status (N = 170) Staff 39 23%
Fellow 33 19%
Doctoral Student 29 17%
Student (Admin Student, Technical Student, Trainee)
29 17%
User 40 24%
The study sample comprised of 53% men and 47% women. This does not reflect the
overall population of CERN, in which women make out 20% (see Figure 1).
Figure 1: Gender distribution of sample and CERN population14
14 CERN Annual Personnel Statistics 2015: http://cds.cern.ch/record/2154389/files/CERN-HR-STAFF-STAT-2015.pdf?version=1
54%
46%
Survey respondents
Male Female
81%
19%
CERN population (2015)
Male Female

29
The variable of managerial or supervisory responsibilities was dichotomized in true
or false, without differentiating the level of supervisory responsibility to maintain the
anonymity of the respondents. 22% of the respondents indicated to have a supervi-
sory role at CERN. As a result, the group of people with managerial responsibilities
is heterogeneous and ranges from employees supervising students to department
heads. There is no public data available to compare the composition of the survey
respondents with the CERN population for this variable.
The nationality variable was also dichotomized, into Europeans and Non-Europeans
to ensure a sufficient group size to compare. CERN was founded as a European
laboratory and the majority of the member states are still European. It is therefore
assumed that Europeans are in the majority, while Non-Europeans are the minority
at the organisation, since no public data is available to compare the composition of
the survey respondents with the CERN population. This is also reflected in the sample
(Non-Europeans make out 16%). However, the group of Non-European consists of
52% (15 cases) US nationals. This majority can influence the results significantly and
has to be taken into account when interpreting the data.
Age groups were identified by generational categories, to test if there are differences
in perception between the widely used differentiation of generations: Generation Z,
which is at the time of the study aged between 18 and 25 years (19%), Generation Y,
which is aged between 26 and 35 years (57%) and Generation X which is between
36 and 55 years (19%). Compared to the overall age distribution of the CERN popu-
lation, it can be noted, that the average age of the survey respondents is much
younger, that the CERN average. Figure 2 shows a clear shift of the Bell curve of the
two groups.

30
Figure2: Composition of survey respondents and CERN population: Generation (Age)
The fifth independent variable is CERN status. CERN has various status, but for the
purpose of this study, the most common once were selected to ensure a sufficient
group size. This leads to the exclusion of 13 cases, for the analysis of this independ-
ent variable, because the cases could not be assigned to the selected groups.
CERN staff are employees, which is also valid for the group of fellows. The latter
however are graduates and entry-level employees with a short-term contract up to
three years. The group of Doctoral students are associated with and paid by CERN,
taking over organisational responsibilities, but at the same time completing their doc-
toral thesis with a university. The group of students merges various types of interns
at CERN at university level, who are therefore also associated with a university,
spending up to 14 months at the organisation.
The group of users is comprised of mostly visiting scientists, associated with research
institutes (and employed by them), carrying out their research at CERN. For this
group, CERN is merely a place to work, not an employer. Often the supervisor is not
present at CERN, but at their home institute. Users are the biggest group of the CERN
population with a share of 70%. However, many of them spend only part of their work-
ing time at CERN (this ranges from a couple of days a year to a permanent stay). To
avoid biasing the data, respondents were asked to confirm, that they have spent at
least 4 months in the last year on CERN premises. Users are also the biggest group
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
Generation Z (18-25) Generation Y (26-35) Generation X (36-55) Above
Survey respondents CERN population (2015)

31
of survey respondents. The respondent rate of students, doctoral students and fel-
lows are exceptionally high, which can be ascribed to the sampling method, using the
social media platform Facebook, which is very often used by students and young
adults.
Figure3: Composition of survey respondents and CERN population15: CERN Status
For each of the independent variables a hypothesis was formulated, which will be
tested with statistical data analysis:
H1 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on gender.
H2 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on nationality.
H3 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on level of manage-
rial responsibilities.
H4 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on generations.
H5 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on status at the
organisation.
15 CERN Annual Personnel Statistics 2015: http://cds.cern.ch/record/2154389/files/CERN-HR-STAFF-STAT-2015.pdf?version=1
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80%
Other(not included in analysis)
User
Student
Doctoral Student
Fellow
Staff
CERN population (2015) Survey respondents

32
4.5 Method of analysis
The following chapter will explain the statistical tests used to analyse the results of
the survey.
Descriptive statistics (mean, standard deviation) were calculated for every subscale.
To test the reliability of the subscales, Cronbach’s α was calculated for all subscales.
The Pearson correlation was used to measure the degree of relation between the
dimensions. The strength of the relationship between the dimensions is analysed us-
ing the following guidelines: small r = .10 to .29, medium r =.30 to .49, and large r=.50
to 1.0 (Cohen 1988).
The Independent t-test was applied to compare the means of gender, nationality and
managerial responsibilities (see table 1). The t-test assesses whether the mean of
two groups are statistical significantly different from each other. The t-test assumes
that the sample is derived from a normally distributed population and that the data is
measured at least at the interval level. 20 participants per group are required to get a
large effect size (Cohen 1992). The test was conducted with all eight dimensions:
organisational inclusion, cultural group inclusion, valuing differences, work load, trust,
Affirmative-Action Group Perception, adaptation and sensitivity/flexibility.
The one-way ANOVA (Analysis of variance) is a statistical procedure, that tests the
overall fit of a linear model and the statistically significant difference of means when
more than two groups are being compared. It compares the amount of systematic
variance in the data to the amount of unsystematic variance. When a statistically sig-
nificant difference is detected, a Tukey post-hoc analysis is conducted to identify the
groups that differ from each other. It compares simultaneously the means of one
group to the means of any other group, basically conducting a simultaneous inde-
pendent t-test with all groups.
The following three assumptions should be tested prior to running ANOVAs: The data
should be normally distributed, homogeneity of variance needs to be ensured and the
data is measured on at least an interval scale. ANOVA has been conducted to test
the eight dimensions of the diversity climate with two independent variables: age
groups and CERN status (see table 1).

33
For the independent t-test, as well as the ANOVA, normal distribution was tested
using the Kolmogorov-Smirnoff test. The data was tested for homogeneity of variance
using Levene’s statistic. If homogeneity of the variances is not given, the Welch cor-
rection has to be applied, which is an adaptation of the t-test assuming unequal vari-
ances.
5 Data analysis
In the following section, the process and the results of the statistical data analysis are
described. Initially, the sample will be analysed as a whole, reflecting the diversity
climate in the organisation, with the help of descriptive statistics and the correlations
of the dimensions. After that, it will be tested whether the preconditions for the de-
scribed procedures are fulfilled. Then, the results of the analysis for each of the five
hypotheses are described. Each independent variable will be tested with each diver-
sity climate dimension. There will be a summary of the results at the end of this chap-
ter (see page 62).
5.1 Descriptive statistics and correlations of the subscales
Table 2 shows the means for all tested dimensions of the diversity climate, as well as
the Pearson r, indicating the correlations between the dimensions. A high mean gen-
erally suggests a negative implication for the diversity climate, while M = 3 shows a
neutral position. There are three subscales which are reverse: Valuing Differences,
Trust and Adaptation. In these scales, a high score has positive implications for the
diversity climate.

34
Table 2: Descriptive Statistics and correlations of the eight dimensions and overall diver-sity climate
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Cultural Group Inclusion 2.62 .97
2. Organisational Inclusion 2.54 .99 .74**
3. Valuing Differences 3.35 .69 -.73** - .62**
4. Work Load 2.94 1.09 .27** .32** -.32**
5. Trust 3.37 .64 -.56** -.61** .61** -.24**
6. Affirmative-Action Group Perception 3.11 .74 .29** .30** -.23** .21** -.14
7. Adaptation 3.94 .49 .25** .18* -.11 .15* -.06 .43**
8. Sensitivity/Flexibility 2.98 .67 .31** .36** -.16* .15* .04 .41** .41**
Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; The correlations are based on 183 subordinates (N = 183).
* p < .05. **p < .01.
Generally, it can be concluded, that the respondents stayed mostly neutral in their
answers, but there are tendencies, which can be identified.
Regarding the feeling of inclusiveness of the CERN population (subscale 1 + 2), peo-
ple have a neutral perception, but leaning towards disagreeing to being marginalized
as an individual (M (Organisational Inclusion) = 2.54), as well as a part of a cultural
group M (Cultural Group Inclusion) = 2.54). For these two dimensions it will be signif-
icant to compare majority and minority group perceptions (chapter 5.3).
A tendency to support the CERN value of diversity can be determined (M (Valuing
Differences) = 3.35), as well as the observance that colleagues do the same.
The perception of the work load was rated neutral without a significant tendency,
however the standard deviation is significantly high, which indicates significant differ-
ences between groups. The results in chapter 5.3 suggest that supervisors, as well
as users and doctoral students regard their work load as overwhelming, while stu-
dents and fellows disagree with the notion of being overloaded.
When it comes to trust toward the organisation as an employer (or hosting institute),
answers stayed also neutral (M (Trust) = 3.37). However, this result also means that
e.g. the statement “I feel confident that CERN will always try to treat me fairly” was
evaluated neutrally rather than confirmative, which can suggest a lack of trust thereof.
The only dimension which shows a statistically significant tendency towards one side
is the dimension of adaptation (M (Adaptation) = 3.94). The data shows that the

35
CERN population is open to adapting to the differences of their colleagues and rec-
ognize the benefit of a diverse workforce. This is a positive indicator for the overall
diversity climate.
The dimension of sensitivity and flexibility indicates whether minority groups are being
perceived (and treated) as different. The results show a generally neutral perception
by the CERN population. For this dimension, it will be interesting to identify whether
majority groups have a significantly different attitude compared to minority groups.
Table 2 demonstrates the relationship between the eight dimensions of the diversity
climate. The results show that there were positive and negative correlations between
each of the eight dimensions.
There is a strong positive correlation between Cultural Group Inclusion and Organi-
sational Inclusion (r =.74). Therefore, Cultural Group Inclusion is perceived to be
strongly associated with Organisational Inclusion. That means that the individual feel-
ing of inclusion in the organisation is directly related to the belonging to a cultural
group and how that cultural group is perceived to be included.
There was a large negative correlation between Cultural Group Inclusion (as well as
Organisational Inclusion) and Valuing Differences (r = -.73). This is due to the fact,
that the Valuing differences subscale is reversed. It means that the more an individual
identifies with a cultural group and feels this cultural group is included in the organi-
sation (as well as an individual), the more the individual is likely to support the as-
sumption that differences bring a positive contribution to the work environment at
CERN.
As predicted, another strong correlation exists between Trust and Valuing Differ-
ences: The more the individual trusts the organisation rather than their own cultural
group, the more it values differences and supports a diverse workforce.
5.2 Test of preconditions for statistical analysis
Interval measurement. To measure on an interval level means that the collected data
is measured in an ordered scale with assumed same distances between each inter-
val. Thus, “distances” are equivalent along the scale from the low interval to the high
interval. In this case, the 5-point Likert scale, used to measure the 8 dimensions from

36
“strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” is indeed an ordered scale (ranked from 1 to
5) and the distances between the measurements are assumed to be equal for the
purpose of this study.
Reliability. The Cronbach’s α were calculated for all subscales to test reliability of the
53 items, meaning whether the items of a subscale correlate with each other posi-
tively and therefore measure the same construct. A Cronbach’s α of .6 and above
was assumed to prove reliability (see Annex 6 for the Cronbach’s α of all dimensions).
All items except of three proved reliability. Items 27 (Trust), 33 and 35 (both Affirma-
tive-Action Group Perception) were removed before the analysis, due to insufficient
reliability.
Normal distribution. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test was used to examine whether the
sample comes from a group, which is normally distributed, by comparing the defined
group to a normally distributed reference group. All dependent and independent var-
iables have been tested for normality. For all groups, the significance level was below
.05, which indicates not normally distributed groups. However, Eid et al. (2013) indi-
cate that the t-test and ANOVA are still reliable if the sample size is > 30. Therefore,
even though the precondition of normal distribution was not fully met, the t-test and
ANOVA are used for the analysis.
Test of homogeneity of variance. For each independent variable and dimension,
Levene’s test was applied to test for equal variances across the tested groups. Find
the results of the calculations in Annex 7. All except two cases proved equal vari-
ances. In the cases of unequal variances, the Welch correction is applied (Eid et al.
2013). Annex 7 shows the results of the t-test and ANOVA test for both cases: equal
and unequal variances.
5.3 Test of hypotheses
The following chapter gives a description of the results of the statistical tests con-
ducted. The mathematical explanations of the calculations are not explained. The
numbers can be found in Annex 7, together with the test for equal variances. Statis-
tical significance was determined based upon a significance level of p < 0.05.

37
When differences occur between the groups, it suggests that interactions in social
encounters in the organisation are determined to varying degrees by the participants’
membership to a group (gender, nationality, age) (MorBarak et al. 1998: 99).
5.3.1 Results for eight dimensions: H1 Gender
H1 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on gender.
The following results indicate that hypothesis 1 was confirmed. The perception dif-
fered significantly in the dimensions Cultural Group Inclusion, Organisational Inclu-
sion, Valuing Differences, Affirmative-Action Group Perception, Adaptation and
Sensitivity/Flexibility.
Gender – Cultural Group Inclusion.
There was a significant difference (t(181) = 3.32, p < 0.05) in the perception of women
(M = 2.87, SD = .97) and men (M = 2.40, SD = .92). Women and men at CERN do
not share similar perceptions regarding Cultural Group Inclusion. While women have
a neutral perception towards whether minority groups are integrated at CERN, men
reject the notion of demographic groups being marginalized.
Table 3: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Cultural Group Inclusion Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
2.87
.97
Men
98 2.40 .92
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
When looking at question 2, an item suggested by Dahm (2009: 295) to be a strong
indicator of the dimension of Cultural Group Inclusion, it shows that 43% of men
strongly disagree with the notion, that their work is judged based on whether their
behaviour conforms to the organisational norm. At the same time only 27% of women
strongly disagree with that statement and 28% agree or strongly agree. Therefore,
the minority group of women have a more negative perception of the marginalization
of cultural groups, than the majority group.

38
Figure 4: Detailed results for question 2 (Cultural Group Inclusion): Gender
The overall perception of Cultural Group Inclusion can be evaluated as neutral,
which indicates that exclusion practices are not absent, but also not seen as preva-
lent.
Gender – Organisational Inclusion
The results indicate that there is a significant difference (t(181) = 3.23, p < 0.05) in
the perception of women (M = 2.79, SD = 1.02) and men (M = 2.32, SD = .90). Women
and men do not share a similar perception regarding Organisational Inclusion and
exclusion practices. Men generally reject the notion of marginalisation of individuals
who are different, whereas women, as the minority group at CERN have a more neu-
tral point of view.
Table 4: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Organisational Inclusion Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
2.79
1.02
Men
98 2.32 .90
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
When looking at item 8, which is noted by Dahm (2009: 294) as particularly significant
for this subscale, it confirms the calculation and reveals, that 25% of the men strongly
disagree with the statement, that people are overlooked in promotions because of not
conforming to the majority, whereas only 13% of women strongly disagree with the
statement. However, in both groups, approximately 48% either strongly disagree or
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
Male
Female
Question 2: "Sometimes people who talk and act differently are treated like they aren’t capable or smart." (N=183)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

39
disagree with the statement, giving a generally positive indicator for the diversity cli-
mate.
Figure 5: Detailed results for question 8 (Organisational Inclusion): Gender
Gender – Valuing Differences
There is a small significant difference (t(181) = -3.10, p < 0.05) in the perception of
women (M = 3.19, SD = .72) and men (M = 3.50, SD = .64). Men and women are
generally neutral, whereas men perceive the valuing of differences more positively at
the organisation.
Table 5: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Valuing Differences Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
3.19
.72
Men
98 3.50 .64
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
The results of question 15, which tested the perception of how the immediate work
environment values differences, shows that while 48% of men agree with the state-
ment, only 35% of women do (Figure 6).
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
Male
Female
Question 8: "It seems that the real reason people are denied promotions or raises ist that they are seen as not fitting in" (N=166)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

40
Figure 6: Detailed results for question 15 (Valuing Differences): Gender
Gender – Work Load
The data indicates no significant difference (t(181) = .52, p > 0.05) in the perception
of women (M = 2.98, SD = 1.09) and men (M = 2.90, SD = 1.09). Therefore, women
and men at CERN share similar perceptions regarding work load, which is a neutral
one.
Table 6: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Work load Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
2.98
1.09
Men
98 2.90 1.09
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Gender – Trust
There is no significant difference t(181) = -.60, p > 0.05) in the perception of women
(M = 3.34, SD = .60) and men (M = 3.39, SD = .67) regarding trust towards the or-
ganisation. Men and women share a neutral perception.
Table 7: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Trust Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
3.39
.60
Men
98 2.90 .67
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
Male
Female
Question 15: When people have a different orientation or style, they take the time to explain and try to understand the other person's point of view. (N=183)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

41
Gender – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
The results demonstrate a small significant difference t(181) = 2.50, p > 0.05) in the
perception of men (M = 2.98, SD = .73) and women (M = 3.26, SD = .72) towards
affirmative-action initiatives. While for both groups the perception is generally neutral,
women have a slightly more positive attitude, which corresponds to the expectations
according to MorBarak et al. (1998) who prognose a more positive attitude of minority
groups, which are the beneficiaries of such initiatives.
Table 8: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Affirmative-Action Group Per-ception Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
3.26
.72
Men
98 2.98 .73
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Gender – Adaptation
The results of the t-test show that there is a small significant difference t(174.198) =
3.14, p < 0.05) in the perception of women (M = 4.05, SD = .38) and men (M = 3.83,
SD = .54). Both groups have a clear positive attitude towards adapting to their diverse
work environment, whereas women at CERN are slightly more open than men.
Table 9: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Adaptation Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
4.05
.38
Men
98 3.83 .54
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
One of the questions, regarded as a significant indicator for this dimension by Dahm
(2009: 296) is item 40:

42
Figure 7: Detailed results for question 40 (Adaptation): Gender
It shows that overall only 8% disagree with the notion to learn about other peoples’
cultural backgrounds, while for both groups around 20% strongly agree.
Gender – Sensitivity/Flexibility The results of the t-test demonstrate a small significant difference t(181) = 2.97, p <
0.05) in the perception of men (M = 2.84, SD = .67) and women (M = 3.14, SD = .64)
regarding sensitivity and flexibility. Both groups have a rather neutral attitude towards
minorities being more sensible to differences and ready to adapt to them. However,
women have a slightly stronger agreement with statements like “It seems that minor-
ities have a special sensitivity and understanding of diversity issues.” This overall
neutral perception points to a positive diversity climate. Since a strong positive per-
ception suggests a strong in-group/out-group perception, meaning minorities are per-
ceived as different and treated as an out-group.
Table 10: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Sensitiv-ity/Flexibility
Gender
N
M
SD
Women
85
3.14
.64
Men
98 2.84 .67
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
Male
Female
Question 40: "I go out of my way to learn about others‘ cultural backgrounds, traditions and points of view." (N=183)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

43
5.3.2 Results for eight dimensions: H2 Nationality
H2 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on nationality.
The following results indicate that hypothesis 2 was confirmed. The perceptions
based on nationality differed in the dimensions Affirmative-Action Group Perception
and Sensitivity/Flexibility.
Nationality – Cultural Group Inclusion
The data shows that there is no significant difference (t(181) = 1.81, p > 0.05) in the
perception of Europeans (M = 2.56, SD = .95) and Non-Europeans (M = 2.92, SD =
1.04). Europeans and Non-Europeans at CERN share a similar neutral perception
regarding Cultural Group Inclusion.
Table 11: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Cultural Group Inclusion
Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
2.56
.95
Non-European
29 2.92 1.04
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Nationality – Organisational Inclusion
Regarding Organisational Inclusion there is no significant difference t(181) = .144, p
> 0.05) in the perception of Europeans and Non-Europeans. Therefore Europeans (M
= 2.53, SD = .96) and Non-Europeans (M = 2.56, SD = 1.10 ) at CERN share the
same neutral perception towards Organisational Inclusion.
Table 12: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Organisa-tional Inclusion
Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
2.53
.96
Non-European
29 2.56 1.10
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation

44
Nationality – Valuing Differences
There is no significant difference ((t(181) = -1.623, p > 0.05) in the perception of Eu-
ropeans (M = 3.39 , SD = .66) and Non-Europeans (M = 3.16, SD = .82). Europeans
as well as Non-Europeans at CERN have an equally neutral perception of valuing of
differences.
Table 13: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Valuing Differ-ences
Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
3.39
.66
Non-European
29 3.16 .82
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Nationality – Work Load
The independent t-test shows no significant difference (t(181) = 1.77, p > 0.05) in the
perception for Europeans (M = 2.87, SD = 1.10) and Non-Europeans (M = 3.26, SD
= .99). Thus, Europeans and Non-European at CERN share similar perceptions re-
garding Work Load.
Table 14: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Work Load
Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
2.87
1.10
Non-European
29 3.26 .99
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Nationality – Trust
There is no significant difference t(181) = 407, p > 0.05) in the perception of Non-
Europeans (M = 3.41, SD = .76) and Europeans (M = 3.36, SD = .61). Therefore,
Europeans and Non-Europeans at CERN share similar perception regarding trust to-
wards the organisation.

45
Table 15: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Trust Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
3.41
.76
Non-European
29 3.36 .61
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Nationality – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
The results demonstrate a small, but significant difference (t(181) = 2.98, p < 0.05)
in the perception of Europeans (M = 3.04, SD = .74) and Non-Europeans (M = 3.48,
SD = .59) regarding affirmative-action initiatives. Non-Europeans have a more posi-
tive attitude. Since the majority of the Non-European group indicated to be from the
USA, the origin of the affirmative-action movement, the result can be lead back to a
historically negative attitude towards favouring one demographic group over another
(see chapter 4.1).
Table 16: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Affirmative-Action Group Perception
Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
3.04
.74
Non-European
29 3.48 .59
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Nationality – Adaptation
The results indicate no significant difference t(181) = 1.307, p > 0.05) in the percep-
tion of Europeans and Non-Europeans. Thus, Europeans (M = 4.04 , SD = .41) and
Non-Europeans (M = 3.91 , SD = .50 ) at CERN share a similar open perception
towards adapting to colleagues who might be different.

46
Table 17: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Adaptation Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
4.04
.41
Non-European
29 3.91 .50
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Nationality – Sensitivity / Flexibility
The results demonstrate a small, but significant difference t(181) = 2.00, p < 0.05) in
the perception of Europeans (M = 2.94, SD = .68) and Non-Europeans (M = 3.20, SD
= .59) regarding Sensitivity/Flexibility. The minority group of Non-Europeans perceive
minorities in general to be more sensitive and flexible when it comes to a diverse work
environment.
Table 18: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Sensitiv-ity/Flexibility
Nationality
N
M
SD
European
154
2.94
.68
Non-European
29 3.20 .59
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
When looking at question 45, proposed by Dahm (2009) as a good indicator for the
dimension, it shows, that 47% of Europeans, who are the majority group at CERN
perceive minorities to be different from the rest of the organisation (Figure 8).
Figure 8: Detailed results for question 45 (Sensitivity/Flexibility): Nationality
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
European
Non-European
Question 45: "It seems that minorities have a special sensitivity and understanding of diversity issues." (N=183)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

47
5.3.3 Results for eight dimensions: H3 Managerial responsibilities
H3 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on level of managerial
responsibilities.
The following results indicate that hypothesis 3 was not confirmed. The perception
only differs in the dimension work load, but not in any other dimension. By itself the
dimension work load cannot be an indicator of an overall diversity climate (Dahm
2009).
Managerial responsibilities – Cultural Group Inclusion
The results indicate that there is no significant difference (t(181) = -.953, p > 0.05) in
the perception of employees with no supervisory responsibilities (M = 2.58, SD = .96)
and with supervisory responsibilities (M = 2.74, SD = .99). Employees with or without
supervisory responsibilities share a similar perception regarding Cultural Group In-
clusion.
Table 19: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Cultural Group Inclusion
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
2.58
.96
Yes
41 2.74 .99
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Managerial responsibilities – Organisational Inclusion
The results demonstrate a small, but statistically not significant difference t(181) = -
.736, p > 0.05) in the perception of employees with no supervisory responsibilities (M
= 2.51, SD = .94) and employees with supervisory responsibilities (M = 2.64, SD =
1.12) regarding Organisational Inclusion. Therefore, employees with and without su-
pervisory responsibilities share similar perceptions regarding Organisational Inclu-
sion.

48
Table 20: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Organisational Inclusion
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
2.51
.94
Yes
41 2.64 1.12
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Managerial responsibilities – Valuing Differences
The results of the independent t-test show, that there is no statistically significant
difference (t(181) = .018, p > 0.05) in the perception of employees with no supervisory
responsibilities (M = 3.35, SD = .69) and with supervisory responsibilities (M = 3.35,
SD = .72). Employees with or without supervisory responsibilities share a similar per-
ception of Valuing Differences.
Table 21: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Valuing Differences
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
3.35
.69
Yes
41 3.35 .72
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Managerial responsibilities – Work Load
The independent t-test shows a significant difference (t(181) = -2.46, p < 0.05) in the
perception of employees with no supervisory responsibilities (M = 2.83, SD = 1.09)
and employees with supervisory responsibilities (M = 3.30, SD = 1.02). Both groups
regard their workload generally neutral, however employees with supervisory roles
tend to agree more with the notion to be overloaded with work.

49
Table 22: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Work load
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
2.83
1.09
Yes
41 3.30 1.02
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Managerial responsibilities – Trust
There is no significant difference t(181) = -.458, p > 0.05) in the perception of em-
ployees with no supervisory function (M = 3.35, SD = .67) and employees with super-
visory function (M = 3.40, SD = .51). Therefore, employees with or without supervisory
responsibilities share a similar level of trust towards the organisation.
Table 23: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Trust
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
3.35
.67
Yes
41 3.40 .51
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Managerial responsibilities – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
The results demonstrate a small but statistically not significant difference t(181) = -
.262, p > 0.05) in the perception of employees with no supervisory responsibilities (M
= 3.10, SD = .77) and employees with supervisory responsibilities (M = 3.14, SD =
.64) regarding affirmative-action initiatives.
Table 24: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Affirmative-Action Group Perception
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
3.10
.77
Yes
41 3.14 .64
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation

50
Managerial responsibilities – Adaptation
The independent t-test indicates no significant difference (t(181) = -.082, p > 0.05) in
the perception of employees with or without supervisory responsibilities. Thus, em-
ployees with no supervisory responsibilities (M = 3.93 , SD = .52) and employees with
supervisory responsibilities (M = 3.94 , SD = .39 ) at CERN share a similar neutral
attitude concerning the need and openness to adapt to their colleagues.
Table 25: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Adaptation
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
3.93
.52
Yes
41 3.94 .39
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Managerial responsibilities – Sensitivity/Flexibility
The results demonstrate a small but not statistically significant difference t(181) = -
.759, p > 0.05) regarding sensitivity and flexibility.
Table 26: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial responsi-bilities: Sensitivity/Flexibility
Supervisor
N
M
SD
No
142
2.96
.68
Yes
41 3.04 .66
Note: N = 183. M = mean; SD = standard deviation
5.3.4 Results for eight dimensions: H4 Generation (Age)
H4 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on generations.
The following results indicate, that hypothesis 4 was not confirmed. None of the eight
tested dimensions of diversity climate differ based on generations.

51
Table 27: ANOVA for eight dimensions: Generation (Age)
Dimension
Sig.
Cultural Group Inclusion .294
Organisational Group Inclusion .510
Valuing Differences .308
Work Load .318
Trust .654
Affirmative-Action Group Perception .578
Adaptation 579
Sensitivity/Flexibility .545
Note: N = 182 *p < .05. *** p <.001.
Generation (Age) – Cultural Group Inclusion
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = 1.233, p= .294), there is no statistically signif-
icant difference between the three age groups regarding Cultural Group Inclusion.
Table 28: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Cultural Group Inclu-sion
Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
2.75
.92
Generation Y (26-35)
104 2.66 1.01
Generation X (36-45)
44 2.43 .93
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Generation (Age) – Organisational Inclusion
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = .676, p= .510), there is no statistically signifi-
cant difference between the three age groups regarding Organisational Inclusion.

52
Table 29: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Organisational Inclu-sion
Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
2.70
.17
Generation Y (26-35)
104 2.54 .10
Generation X (36-45)
44 2.44 .15
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Generation (Age) – Valuing Differences
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = 1.186, p= .308), there is no statistically signif-
icant difference between the three age groups regarding the valuing of differences.
Table 30: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Valuing Differences Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
3.37
.68
Generation Y (26-35)
104 3.29 .72
Generation X (36-45)
44 3.48 .64
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Generation (Age) – Work Load
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = 1.154, p= .318), there is no statistically signif-
icant difference between the three age groups regarding Work Load.
Table 31: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Work Load Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
2.96
1.18
Generation Y (26-35)
104 3.03 1.09
Generation X (36-45)
44 2.73 1.02
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation

53
Generation (Age) – Trust
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = .426, p= .654), there is no statistically signifi-
cant difference between the three age groups regarding trust towards the organisa-
tion.
Table 32: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Trust Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
3.32
.60
Generation Y (26-35)
104 3.35 .66
Generation X (36-45)
44 3.44 .63
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Generation (Age) – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = .551, p= .579), there is no statistically signifi-
cant difference between the three age groups regarding the perception of affirmative-
action initiatives.
Table 33: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Affirmative-Action Group Perception
Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
3.21
.65
Generation Y (26-35)
104 3.12 .79
Generation X (36-45)
44 3.03 .72
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Generation (Age) – Adaptation
As determined by ANOVA (F(2, 179) = .745, p= .476), there is no statistically signifi-
cant difference between the three age groups regarding Adaptation.

54
Table 34: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Adaptation Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
4.00
.51
Generation Y (26-35)
104 3.94 .47
Generation X (36-45)
44 3.87 .53
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
Generation (Age) – Sensitivity/Flexibility
As determined by one-way ANOVA (F(2, 179) = .610, p= .545), there is no statistically
significant difference between the three age groups regarding sensitivity and flexibil-
ity.
Table 35: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Sensitivity/Flexibility Generation (Age)
N
M
SD
Generation Z (18- 25)
34
2.88
.64
Generation Y (26-35)
104 3.02 .68
Generation X (36-45)
44 2.95 .69
Note: N = 182; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
5.3.5 Results for eight dimensions: H5 CERN Status
H5 There is a difference in the diversity perspectives based on status at the organi-
sation.
The following results indicate that hypothesis 5 was confirmed. The results differed in
the dimensions of Cultural Group Inclusion, Valuing Differences and Work Load.

55
Table 36: ANOVA for eight dimensions: CERN Status Dimension
Sig.
Cultural Group Inclusion .040*
Organisational Group Inclusion .113
Valuing Differences .013*
Work Load .000***
Trust .592
Affirmative-Action Group Perception .054
Adaptation .592
Sensitivity/Flexibility .512
Note: N = 170 *p < .05. *** p <.001.
CERN Status – Cultural Group Inclusion
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = .716, p= .04), there is a statistically significant
difference between the groups based on the CERN Status regarding Cultural Group
Inclusion. A Tukey post-hoc test revealed, that students have a very positive percep-
tion of how cultural groups are included in the organisation. This perception is statis-
tically significantly different from the perception of doctoral students as well as users,
who tend to be more neutral.
Table 37: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Cultural Group Inclusion CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
2.64
.89
Fellow
33 2.69 1.03
Doctoral Student
29 2.84 1.02
Student
29 2.14 .90
User
40 2.79 .97
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
When having a look at the answers to question 2 (Figure 9), which was specified by
Dahm (2009: 295) as a significant indicator for the dimension, it shows that 72% of

56
the students disagree with the notion that people who are different are being margin-
alized, while it is only 52% (Doctoral students) and 60% (Users) for the other two
groups. This still indicates an overall rejection of the marginalization of cultural
groups. This is specifically shown by the high rate of “strongly disagree” answers by
all groups.
Figure 9: Detailed results of question 2 (Cultural Group Inclusion): CERN Status
CERN Status – Organisational Inclusion
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = 1.89, p= .113), there is no statistically signifi-
cant difference between the CERN status regarding Organisational Inclusion.
Table 38: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Organisational Inclusion CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
2.55
.94
Fellow
33 2.54 .95
Doctoral Student
29 2.71 1.00
Student
29 2.11 .86
User
40 2.71 1.08
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
Staff
Fellow
Doctoral Student
Student
User
Question 2: "Sometimes people who talk and act differently are treated like they aren’t capable or smart." (N=170)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

57
CERN Status – Valuing Differences
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = 3.26, p= .013), there is a statistically significant
difference between the CERN status regarding the valuing of differences. A Tukey
post hoc test revealed that students support the value of diversity more, as well as
perceive their work environment to do the same. Users have a more neutral percep-
tion.
Table 39: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Valuing Differences CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
3.50
.67
Fellow
33 3.23 .77
Doctoral Student
29 3.27 .72
Student
29 3.68 .55
User
40 3.16 .67
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
CERN Status – Work Load
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 79.63 = 11.47, p= .000), there is a statistically signif-
icant difference between the CERN status groups regarding Work Load. A Tukey post
hoc test revealed four significant differences (all groups are affected): between stu-
dents and staff, between users and staff, between users and fellows and between
students and doctoral students.
Table 40: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Work Load CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
2.88
1.17
Fellow
33 2.77 1.13
Doctoral Student
29 2.96 .97
Student
29 2.21 .79
User
40 3.51 .79
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation

58
When looking at the means of the groups, it shows, that users generally perceive their
work load to be very high and agree with the notion to be overwhelmed by work (M =
3.51). Looking at the answers to questions 23 (Figure 10): “I feel like I am given more
work than I can reasonably handle”, there was no user who strongly disagreed with
that statement, while 45% of users agree (Figure 9). Doctoral students are ranked
second when looking at high work loads (M = 2.96). Students on the other hand dis-
agree and perceive their work load not to be very high (M = 2.96). Fellows and staff
have a neutral attitude towards their work load.
Figure 10: Detailed results for question 23 (Work Load): CERN Status
CERN Status – Trust
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = .701, p= .592), there is no statistically signifi-
cant difference between the CERN status regarding trust towards the organisation.
Table 41: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Trust CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
3.42
.57
Fellow
33 3.33 .71
Doctoral Student
29 3.23 .68
Student
29 3.48 .73
User
40 3.45 .58
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
Staff
Fellow
Doctoral Student
Student
User
Question 23: I feel like I am given more work than I can reasonably handle.
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree

59
CERN Status – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = 2.38, p= .54) there is no statistically significant
difference between the CERN status groups regarding affirmative-action initiatives.
Table 42: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Affirmative-Action Group Per-ception
CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
3.20
.73
Fellow
33 2.92 .71
Doctoral Student
29 3.23 .75
Student
29 2.90 .74
User
40 3.34 .76
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
CERN Status – Adaptation
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = .702 p= .59) there is no statistically significant
difference between the CERN status regarding Adaptation.
Table 43: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Adaptation CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
3.98
.45
Fellow
33 3.85 .55
Doctoral Student
29 4.00 .49
Student
29 3.88 .56
User
40 4.00 .43
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
CERN Status – Sensitivity/Flexibility
As determined by ANOVA (F(4, 165) = .823 p= .51) there is no statistically significant
difference between the CERN status regarding Sensitivity/ Flexibility.

60
Table 44: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Sensitivity/Flexibility CERN Status
N
M
SD
Staff
39
2.94
.61
Fellow
33 2.87 .75
Doctoral Student
29 3.00 .70
Student
29 2.99 .66
User
40 3.14 .62
Note: N = 170; M = mean; SD = standard deviation
5.3.6 Summary of the results
Table 45 shows a summary of the results of the data analysis and testing of the five
hypotheses. It can be stated that despite the generally neutral perception of the di-
versity climate at CERN, there are some significant differences within the population.
Differences for five demographic variables were predicted. For the variables of gen-
eration (age) and the level of managerial responsibilities no differences could be
found.
The most differences in perception could be identified between men and women. Men
perceive the inclusion of cultural groups and individuals who are different more posi-
tively. They also perceive their work environment more welcoming to differences than
women. Women on the other hand are more open to affirmative-action initiatives and
to adapting to people who are different in their work environment. However, men also
show a clear confirmative attitude towards adapting to people who are different.
For the dimension Cultural Group Inclusion, which measures the perception of minor-
ity social identity groups being marginalized, the only difference that could be de-
tected was for gender. When it comes to nationality and generation, there was no
different perception between minority and majority groups, which indicates a positive
diversity climate.

61
When it comes to affirmative-action initiatives, the prediction that minority groups view
them more positively (as often being the targeted group), than majority groups, has
been proved correct along the dimension of nationality and gender.
Looking at the CERN status, students have a more positive view on the inclusiveness
at CERN as well as the valuing of differences. Furthermore, supervisors, users and
doctoral students report a higher work load than the rest of the CERN population.

62
Table 45: Summary of results of data analysis
Group Difference: Status at CERN
M H1 Group Differ-ence: Gender
H2 Group Difference: Nationality
H4 Group Difference: Generation
H3 Level of managerial responsibilities
H5 CERN Status
Cultural Group Inclusion
2.62 Male perception more positive
None None None students more positive perception than users and doctoral students
Organisational Inclusion
2.54 Male perception more positive
None None None None
Valuing Differences
3.35 Male perception more positive
None None None students more positive perception than users
Work load 2.94 None None None supervisors more negative perception
users and doctoral stu-dents more negative per-ception than students and fellows
Trust 3.37 None None None None None
Affirmative- Action Group Perception
3.11 Female perception more positive
Non-Europeans more positive perception
None None None
Adaptation 3.94 Female perception more positive
None None None None
Sensitivity/ Flexibility
2.98 Female perception less positive
Non-Europeans less positive perception
None None None

63
6 Discussion and recommendations
The results of this non-representative sample of the CERN population show, that
there is a positive diversity climate at CERN, with some different group perceptions
mostly when it comes to gender. Men as the majority group at CERN have a more
positive view especially on inclusiveness at the organisation than women do. This
result is not surprising taking into account that women are traditionally underrepre-
sented in the male dominated field of SET (Science, Engineering and Technology).
The results of the study suggest that training initiatives aimed at raising awareness
on the implications of gender diversity could have a valuable impact on the diver-
sity climate at the organisation. Taking into account that gender diversity is the
origin of the current programme and still a main target of the CERN Diversity Office,
a continuation of this strategy is recommended.
The results show that there are differences in the perception of work load. Super-
visors, as well as users and doctoral students report a higher work load than the
rest of the CERN population. This can have a negative impact on the diversity
climate, because a heavy work load can lead to a lower disposition to pay attention
to and embracing different working styles. A peculiarity of the structure of CERN
is, that users, as the biggest population group at CERN is not employed by the
organisation. The performance appraisal system and control of the work load
therefore lies with another institute. However, the offer of trainings for e.g. time
management for the whole population is an option to address this issue.
As managers were also identified to report a heavier work load, initiatives to sup-
port supervisors in their managerial responsibilities are recommended. The sup-
port of and by management is an important aspect of a successfully implemented
diversity strategy in general: “Senior managers must support diversity initiatives
and must be willing to commit sufficient resources to the effort. Managers must
recognize that effectively implementing workplace diversity requires sustained
commitment to organisational change” (Kreitz et al. 2008). Therefore, support of
employees with managerial responsibilities through transparent processes, time
management trainings, but also personal support and work load monitoring can
aid the successful implementation of diversity initiatives. A targeted training for

64
managers at the organisation is also recommended to take into account leadership
implications of managing a diverse workforce.
CERN has in place general training initiatives, as a stand-alone offer, as well as
part of other trainings e.g. in recruitment. In addition to these, informal training
offers are recommended to be implemented, as e.g. suggested by Harrison et al.
(2000): “Provide opportunities for employees to experience interactions with di-
verse others”. The researchers added the factor of time and frequency of interac-
tions to their studies of collaboration in diverse teams. They suggest that regular
interactions with people of a different demographic group can reduce the negative
bias and increase the probability of effective collaboration. In the subscale Sensi-
tivity/Flexibility, differences were indicated between gender and nationality groups.
Regular interactions could influence the perception of minority groups as being
characterized as out-groups positively.
Even though training is an important part of diversity management, the setup of
processes, structures and reward mechanisms is also essential for a positive di-
versity climate. On a team level, this could take the form of collective appraisal
systems. Harrison et al. (2000) suggest “that when members’ individual outcomes
depend more on team performance, they collaborate more frequently” (ibid.: 27).
Measures to enhance team building can be beneficial for the diversity climate of
an organisation. As Hofhuis et al. (2011: 966) proposed, when employees not only
identify with groups based on demographic characteristics, but simultaneously with
the organisation, they display a dual identity, strengthening the trust in the organi-
sation. The results of the survey suggest a neutral attitude when it comes to trust
toward the organisation, which can be influenced positively by building a stronger
relationship with the organisation by supporting team building.

65
7 Discussion of methodology
It is appropriate to point out some limitations of this study and the survey that
should be addressed in future research.
First of all, this study was conducted with a convenience sample, involving a Fa-
cebook group and the network of the researcher. This method targeted a certain
age group. The question therefore arises, whether the sample is representative of
the organisation. As shown in chapter 4.4, the demographics of the CERN popu-
lation differ significantly from the sample demographics. Further insight into a pop-
ulation that might have been underrepresented, could be provided by conducting
interviews with a selection of CERN employees. Due to time constraints, the re-
searcher refrained from conducting interviews.
Furthermore, throughout this survey the term diversity was used to include a vari-
ety of dimensions: cultural diversity, gender diversity, age diversity, disability etc.
However, when questioning individual perceptions, the different types of diversity
should be specified, as different ideas and issues are connected to them. This was
especially problematic, since the ODNA was originally used to test cultural diversity
(Dahm 2009: 283), and marginally age and gender diversity. For the purpose of
this study, the wording was adapted to include a broader definition of diversity.
However, the problem arising is the assumption that heterogeneous groups face
the same interactional issues. To gain more detailed knowledge about the diversity
climate and differences of perception the adaptation of the questions to a specific
type of diversity and its interactional implications is recommended.
Moreover, the ODNA was developed in the USA, which implicates different as-
pects due to the specific historical and political context. For example, the question-
naire subscale “Affirmative-Action Group Perception” has especially political impli-
cations in the USA, the origin of such initiatives, where it has a negative reputation.
In a population which consists mainly of Europeans other relevant subscales might
be identified.
In the analysis groups were defined as minority groups, based on an overall com-
parison to the population. This was for example the case for women. However, in
their immediate work environment, this might not be the case.

66
In addition, when questioning the perception of a sample group, the results are
based on self-reflexivity. Bias regarding group-identity, favorable self-concept and
preferred social traits could influence the results of the study (Tajfel, Turner 1979).
Furthermore, there is an issue in coherence of the subscales of the ODNA. The
subscale Valuing Differences questions whether differences are evaluated posi-
tively, which thus contributes to a positive diversity climate. At the same time, the
subscale Sensitivity/Flexibility indicates a negative outcome, when minority groups
are perceived as different, as an “out-group”. The researcher tried to get in contact
with Prof. Dahm to request more detailed information on the subscales than pub-
licly available, but was not successful. When applying the ODNA again however,
it is desirable to gain additional knowledge on the theory behind the subscales,
than given in Dahm 2009.
As for the forming of the independent variables, the nationalities were grouped into
European and Non-European to ensure sufficient sized sample groups. However,
the group of Non-Europeans is very heterogeneous. For further research a more
detailed division of this sample group should be considered.
This research was based on the assumption that surface-level diversity such as
gender, age and nationality determines the attitude of the employee towards diver-
sity. However, as explained in chapter 2.4, it was suggested that deep-level diver-
sity, meaning the value structures of the individual is more significant. After con-
firming that there are some dimensions for which the perceptions are equal, a fu-
ture research should correlate diversity perceptions with an assessment of individ-
ual value structures, in order to get a better insight into employees’ perception.
Another issue to be taken into account for future research, is the age of the re-
search on which the literature review is based. Many of the used articles are over
20 years old: “Factors related to time are important in assessing research applica-
bility - earlier findings may not be relevant to today’s workplace.” (Tschirhart, Wise
2000: 392). There is numerous research and a fast development in the area of
diversity management. Therefore, the paradigm under which it is evaluated and
analysed is shifting rapidly. An example is the evolvement of the ODNA itself: It is
based on a survey by Larkey (1996) and adapted to the context of the early 21.
Century.

67
8 Tables and Figures Figures
Figure 1: Gender distribution of sample and CERN population .................................. 28 Figure 2: Composition of survey respondents and CERN population: Generation (Age) 30 Figure 3: Composition of survey respondents and CERN population: CERN Status .. 31 Figure 4: Detailed results for question 2 (Cultural Group Inclusion): Gender .............. 38 Figure 5: Detailed results for question 8 (Organisational Inclusion): Gender ............... 39 Figure 6: Detailed results for question 15 (Valuing Differences): Gender .................... 40 Figure 7: Detailed results for question 40 (Adaptation): Gender .................................. 42 Figure 8: Detailed results for question 45 (Sensitivity/Flexibility): Nationality .............. 46
Figure 9: Detailed results of question 2 (Cultural Group Inclusion): CERN Status ...... 56 Figure 10: Detailed results for question 23 (Work Load): CERN Status ...................... 58
Tables
Table 1: Sample and independent variables ................................................................ 28 Table 2: Descriptive Statistics and correlations of the eight dimensions and overall diver-sity climate ................................................................................................................... 34 Table 3: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Cultural Group Inclusion 37 Table 4: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Organisational Inclusion 38 Table 5: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Valuing Differences ..... 39 Table 6: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Work load .................... 40 Table 7: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Trust ............................ 40

68
Table 8: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Affirmative-Action Group Perception .................................................................................................................... 41 Table 9: Means and standard deviation for women and men: Adaptation ................... 41 Table 10: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Sensitiv-ity/Flexibility .................................................................................................................. 42 Table 11: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Cultural Group Inclusion ............................................................................................................ 43 Table 12: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Organisa-tional Inclusion ............................................................................................................. 43 Table 13: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Valuing Dif-ferences ....................................................................................................................... 44 Table 14: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Work Load..44 Table 15: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Trust .. 45 Table 16: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Affirmative-Action Group Perception .............................................................................................. 45 Table 17: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Adapta-tion.46 Table 18: Means and standard deviation for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Sensitiv-ity/Flexibility .................................................................................................................. 46 Table 19: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Cultural Group Inclusion ............................................................................... 47 Table 20: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Organisational Inclusion ............................................................................... 48 Table 21: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Valuing Differences ....................................................................................... 48 Table 22: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Work load ..................................................................................................... 49 Table 23: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Trust .............................................................................................................. 49

69
Table 24: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Affirmative-Action Group Perception ............................................................. 49 Table 25: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Adaptation ..................................................................................................... 50 Table 26: Means and standard deviation for people with and without managerial respon-sibilities: Sensitivity/Flexibility ...................................................................................... 50 Table 27: ANOVA for eight dimensions: Generation (Age) .......................................... 51 Table 28: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Cultural Group In-clusion ......................................................................................................................... 51 Table 29: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Organisational In-clusion .......................................................................................................................... 52 Table 30: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Valuing Differ-ences...52 Table 31: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Work Load ...... 52 Table 32: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Trust ............... 53 Table 33: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Affirmative-Action Group Perception ......................................................................................................... 53 Table 34: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Adaptation ...... 54 Table 35: Means and standard deviation for the generational groups: Sensitivity/Flexibil-ity..54 Table 36: ANOVA for eight dimensions: CERN Status ................................................ 55 Table 37: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Cultural Group Inclusion . 55 Table 38: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Organisational Inclusion . 56 Table 39: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Valuing Differences ........ 57 Table 40: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Work Load ...................... 57 Table 41: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Trust ............................... 58

70
Table 42: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Affirmative-Action Group Per-ception ......................................................................................................................... 59 Table 43: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Adaptation ...................... 59 Table 44: Means and standard deviation for CERN Status: Sensitivity/Flexibility ....... 60 Table 45: Summary of results of data analysis ............................................................ 62 Table 46: Cronbach’s α for all eight dimensions .......................................................... 88 Table 47: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Cultural Group Inclusion ..................................................................................... 89 Table 48: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Organisational Inclusion ..................................................................................... 89 Table 49: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Valuing Differences ............................................................................................ 90 Table 50: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Work Load .......................................................................................................... 90 Table 51: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Trust ................................................................................................................... 91 Table 52: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Affirmative-Action Group Perception .................................................................. 91 Table 53: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Adaptation .......................................................................................................... 92 Table 54: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Sensitivity/Flexibility ........................................................................................... 92 Table 55: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Cultural Group Inclusion ........................................................... 93 Table 56: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Organisational Inclusion ............................................................ 93 Table 57: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Valuing Differences ................................................................... 94 Table 58: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Work Load ................................................................................. 94

71
Table 59: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Trust ........................................................................................... 95 Table 60: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Affirmative-Action Group Perception.......................................... 95 Table 61: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Adaptation .................................................................................. 96 Table 62: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Sensitivity/Flexibility ................................................................... 96 Table 63: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Cultural Group Inclusion .................................................................... 97 Table 64: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Organisational Inclusion ..................................................................... 97 Table 65: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Valuing Differences ............................................................................ 98 Table 66: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Work Load .......................................................................................... 98 Table 67: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Trust .................................................................................................. 99 Table 68: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Affirmative-Action Group Perception .................................................. 99 Table 69: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Adaptation ......................................................................................... 99 Table 70: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Sensitivity/Flexibility ........................................................................... 100 Table71: Equality of variances for 8 dimensions: Generation (Age) ............................ 100 Table 72: Equality of variances for 8 dimensions: CERN Status ................................. 102

72
9 References
Allen, R.S., Dawson, G., Wheatley, K., White, C.S. (2007): Perceived diversity and
Organisational performance. In: Employee Relations, 30, 1, 2008, pp. 20-33.
Barry, B., Bateman, T.S. (1996): A social trap analysis of the management of di-
versity. In: The Academy of Management Review, 21, 3, 1996, pp. 757-790.
Brown, S.L. (2008): Diversity in the workplace: A study of gender, race, age and
salary level. Ann Arbor, US: ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
Byrne, D. (1971): The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.
Cohen, J. W. (1988): Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd
edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, J.W. (1992): A power primer. In: Psychological Bulletin, 112, pp. 155-159.
Cox, T. (1993): Cultural Diversity in Organisations: Theory, Research and Practice.
San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.
Cukier, W., Smarz, S. (2012): Diversity Assessment Tools: A Comparison. In: The
International Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Change Management, 11, 6, pp.
29-62.
Dahm, M.J., Willems, E.P., Ivancevich, J.M. (2009): Development of an Organisa-
tional Diversity Needs Analysis (ODNA) Instrument. In: Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 39, 2, pp. 283-318.
Digh, P. (1998): Coming to terms with diversity. In: HR Magazine, 43, pp. 117-132.
Duguid, M.M., Thomas-Hunt, M.C. (2015): Condoning stereotyping? How aware-
ness of stereotyping prevalence impacts expression of stereotypes. In: Journal of
Applied Psychology, 100, 2, pp. 343-359.

73
Eid, M., Gollwitzer, M., Schmitt, M. (2013): Statistik und Forschungsmethoden (3.
Edition). Weinheim: Beltz.
Ensher, E.A., Grant-Mallone, E.J., Donaldson, S.I. (2001): Effects of perceived dis-
crimination on job satisfaction, Organisational commitment, Organisational citizen-
ship behaviour, and grievances. In: Human Resource Development Quarterly, 12,
pp. 53-72.
Elvira, M., Cohen, L. (2001): Location matters: A cross-level analysis of the effects
of Organisational sex composition on turnover. In: Academy of Management Jour-
nal, 44, pp. 591-605.
Ferdman, B.M., Brody, S.E. (1996): Models of Diversity Training. In: Handbook of
Intercultural Training, 2nd edition, pp. 282-303
Foster, C., Harris, L. (2005): Easy to say, difficult to do: diversity management in
retail. In: Human Resource Management Journal, 15, 3, pp. 4-17.
Grice, C. D. (2011): Diversity awareness perceptions among classified staff at a
Midwestern Land grant university. Kansas State University.
Guarte, J., Barrios, E. (2006): Estimation Under Purposive Sampling. In: Commu-
nications in Statistics - Simulation and Computation, 35, 2, pp. 277-284.
Harrison, D.A., Price, K.H., Gavin, J.H., Florey, A.T. (2000): Time, teams and task
performance: Changing effects of surface- and deep-level diversity on group func-
tioning. In Press – AMJ.
Harvey, C.P., Allard, M.J. (2015): Understanding and Managing Diversity. Sixth
Edition. New Jersey US: Pearson.

74
Hobman, E.V., Bordia, P., Gallois, C. (2004): Perceived dissimilarity and work
group involvement: The moderating effects of group openness to diversity. In:
Group & Organizatiion Management, 29, pp. 560-587.
Hofhuis, J., Van der Zee, K.I., Otten, S. (2011): Social Identity Patterns in Culturally
Diverse Organisations: The Role of Diversity Climate. In: Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 42, 4, pp. 964-989.
Jayne, M.E.A., Dipboye, R.L. (2004): Leveraging diversity to improve business per-
formance: research findings and recommendations for organisations. In: Human
Resource Management, 43, 4, pp. 409-424.
Joshi, A., Roh, H. (2007): Context matters: a multilevel framework for work team
diversity research. In: Research in Personnel and Human Resource Management,
26, pp. 1-48.
Kirkpatrick, D. (1998): Great Ideas revisited. In: Training and Development, 50,
pp.54-59.
Konrad, A.M., Prasad, P., Pringle, J.K. (2006): Handbook of Workplace Diversity.
London: Sage.
Kossek, E.E., Lobel, S.A., Brown, J. (2005): Human Resource Strategies to Man-
age Workforce Diversity. Examining the Business Case.
Source: http://ellenkossek.lir.msu.edu/documents/handbook2.pdf (Access:
17/05/2016)
Kossek, E.E., Zonia, S. (1993): Assessing diversity climate: A field study of reac-
tions to employer efforts to promote diversity. In: Journal of Organisational Behav-
ior,14, pp. 61-81.
Kreitz, P.A. (2007): Best practices for Managing Organisational Diversity. In: Jour-
nal of Academic Librarianship, 34, 2, pp. 101-120.

75
Larkey, L.K. (1996): The development and validation of the workforce diversity
questionnaire. An instrument to assess interactions in diverse workgroups. In:
Management Communication Quarterly, 9, 3, pp. 296-337.
Paluck, E.L. (2006): Diversity Training and Intergroup Contact: A Call to Action
Research. In: Journal of Social Issues, 62, 3, 2006, pp. 577-595.
MorBarak, M.E., Cherin, D.A., Berkman, S. (1998): Organisational and Personal
Dimensions in Diversity Climate. Ethnic and Gender Differences in Employee Per-
ceptions. In: Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 34, 1, pp. 82-104.
Ravasi, D., Schultz, M. (2006): Responding to Organisational identity threats: Ex-
ploring the role of Organisational culture. In: Academy of Management Journal, 49,
3, pp. 433-458.
Ryan, Connor (2015): Managing diversity. Study of staff awareness of cultural
workforce diversity within an organisation; a case study. National College of Ire-
land.
Rynes, S., Rosen, B. (1995): A field survey of factors affecting the adoption and
perceived success of diversity training. In: Personnel Psychology, 48, 2, pp. 247-
270.
Sawyerr, O.O., Strauss, J., Yan, J. (2005): Individual value structure and diversity
attitudes. The moderating effects of age, gender, race and religiosity. In: Journal
of Managerial Psychology, 20, 6, pp. 498-521.
Schneider, S.K., Northcraft, G.B. (1999): Three social dilemmas of workforce di-
versity in organisations: A social identity perspective. In: Human Relations, 52, 11,
pp. 1445-1467.

76
Shen, J., Chanda, A., D’Netto, B., Monga, M. (2009): Managing diversity through
human resource management: an international perspective and conceptual frame-
work. In: The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 20, 2, pp.
235-251.
Tajfel, H., Turner, J. (1979): An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin,
W.G., Worchel, S. (Ed.): The social psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey,
CA: Brooks/Cole, pp. 33-47.
Triandis, H.C., Kurowski, L.L., Gelfand, M.J. (1992): Workplace diversity. In: Hand-
book of industrial and Organisational psychology, pp. 770-827.
Tschirhart, M., Wise L.R. (2000): Examining Empirical Evidence on Diversity Ef-
fects: How useful is Diversity Research for Public-Sector Managers? In: Public Ad-
ministration Review, 60, 5, pp. 386-394.
Wentling, R.M., Palma-Rivas, N. (1998): Current Status and Future Trends of Di-
versity Initiatives in the Workplace: Diversity Experts’ Perspective. In: Human Re-
source Development Quarterly, 9, 3, pp. 235-253.

77
ANNEX 1 Map of CERN Member States as of March 2016
Member States: Austria, Belgium, Bulgaria, Czech Republic, Denmark, Finland, France, Germany, Greece, Hungary, Israel, Italy, Netherlands, Norway, Poland, Portugal, Slovakia, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, United Kingdom (Source: http://international-relations.web.cern.ch/International-Relations/ms/; access: 17.05.2016)

78
ANNEX 2
Action Points of CERN Diversity Programme
STRATEGIC OBJECTIVES ACTIONS
RECRUITMENT
# 1
Improve the distribution of under‐represented national‐ities through proactive measures in sourcing and pre‐selection, with “excel‐lence” remaining the over‐arching criterion
Reinforce efforts to widen the applicant pool in sourcing and shortlisting stages
Monitor to maintain progress and redress
anomalies
Develop further contacts to attract more appli‐
cants through enhanced outreach activities
# 2
Achieve optimal gender
distribution in recruit‐
ment for all professional
categories, with “excel‐
lence” remaining the
over‐arching criterion
Reinforce efforts to achieve gender distribution
in sourcing and shortlisting stages
Monitor to maintain progress and redress
anomalies
Explore ways of assuring temporary solutions for
maternity leave cover
CAREER DE‐
VELOPMENT # 3
Provide more gender role mod‐
els
Succession planning (m/w)
Leadership training (m/w)
Coaching and mentoring (m/w)
# 4
Propose parallel career
development opportuni‐
ties (technical and mana‐
gerial paths in parallel)
Align advancement criteria
Provide development planning – technical or
managerial as appropriate
WORK ENVI‐
RONMENT # 5 Promote the exchange of ideas
and understanding between generations and professions
Workshops within departments, sectors or CERN‐wide – facilitated discussions on specific themes related to Organisation life
# 6
Explore ways to improve
work/life balance
Assess the necessity of e‐mail/meetings outside
of working hours – enhance awareness of the
possible impact within hierarchical relationships
Support requests for part‐time work, SLS and teleworking, in line with individual and service needs
# 7
Promote a work environ‐ment based on mutual respect and inclusiveness
Design and deliver events to raise awareness and exchange experiences of diversity in the workplace
Assure regular communication
Continually improve support structures such as
reserved places in local crèches, kindergartens,
etc., in line with needs
Ensure access and equipment as needed for
disabled individuals
Source: CERN Diversity Programme Activity Report 2012-2015 (restricted access)

79
ANNEX 3
The original Organisational Diversity Need Analysis (ODNA) Answer options: Strongly agree – Agree – Neutral – Disagree - Strongly disagree Organisational Inclusion
- It seems that the real reason people are denied promotions or raises is that they are seen as not fitting in.
- It’s hard to get ahead here unless you are part of the “old boys’ network”. - When it comes to getting support for getting ahead here, I feel like I have
fallen through the cracks. - I have to prove myself more and work a lot harder to get into that next posi-
tion because of my gender or nationality. - Performance evaluations seem to be biased against those who are different
because supervisors focus on very traditional ways of getting things done. - Whenever I‘ve confronted someone for giving me a hard time because of
my nationality or gender, they have denied the problem or played it down. - I feel I have been treated differently here because of my nationality, gender
or age. Cultural Group Inclusion
- Some people in our team are “talked down to” because they are different. - Sometimes people who talk and act differently are treated like they aren’t
capable or smart. - There are people in our team who have a hard time accepting ideas when
they are offered by someone who is different from them. - When people begin working on a problem from a very different perspective
they have a hard time seeing each other‘s point of view. - When people from different backgrounds work together in groups, some
people feel ignored because their ideas are not acknowledged. - People get ahead by using “pull” and not because of what they know. - Diversity issues keep some teams here from performing to their maximum
effectiveness. Valuing differences
- When people have a different orientation or style, they take the time to ex-plain and try to understand the other person‘s point of view.
- When people in our team take the offense or miscommunicate they sit down and talk about the differences until they understand each other.
- Employees share their knowledge/expertise with other employees regard-less of their gender or nationality.

80
- We all seem to learn and grow from our differences. - When someone is timid or reluctant to assert their ideas, others will point it
out and ask for their opinion. - People in our team don‘t notice culture or gender differences since we are
really all the same. - Peoples‘ habits or ways of thinking may be different because of their back-
ground, but when it comes to working, we‘re pretty much all the same. Work Load
- The amount of work I have makes me feel extremely stressed at the end of the day.
- I feel like I am given more work than I can reasonably handle. - My workload is so heavy that I feel like I can‘t possibly finish during an ordi-
nary workday. - The amount of work I have interferes with how well I can do my job.
Affirmative-Action Group Perceptions
- Women, minorities and older people are more likely to get promoted and get ahead now.
- Women, minorities and older people don‘t adjust their style to fit the busi-ness context.
- People are sometimes hired for a job with less qualifications than other ap-plicants because they are minorities or women.
- Members of some nationality have a unique way of acting in workgroups which makes them stand out as being different.
- Sometimes women, and minorities and older people should be given special consideration in hiring and promotions.
- Women, minorities and older people don‘t have the same employment op-portunities as others have.
Trust
- People of the same nationality or gender tend to look out for each other. - With many people, you don‘t know how you stand. - If I were having difficulties, I know members of my group would try to help
me out. - I can trust people I work with to lend me a hand if I need it. - I feel confident that my company will always try to treat me fairly. - My company is sincere in its attempts to understand the employee‘s point
of view. Adaptation
- I feel like it‘s up to me to adjust to others when their nationality or gender presents differences in styles or mannerisms.

81
- Working with employees with different backgrounds forces me to reconsider how I approach things.
- I go out of my way to learn about others‘ cultural backgrounds, traditions and points of view.
- Knowing more about the cultural norms of diverse groups would help me to be more effective in my job.
- I think that diverse viewpoints add value. - I can enjoy being with people whose culture, gender or age is very different
from mine. - I believe women, minorities and older people should adopt the values and
lifestyle of the dominant culture. Sensitivity/Flexibility
- It seems that minorities and women have a special sensitivity and under-standing of diversity issues.
- Women and minorities are better able to value the different ways of looking at things and doing work than white males.
- Women and minorities often interpret things differently than white males. - Women and minorities use the diverse backgrounds of others to help with
group problem-solving. - Women and minorities often encourage others to express their opinion. - It seems that minorities and women are more likely to adjust their styles of
work when necessary. - It seems that women and minorities express more empathy for others than
white males.

82
ANNEX 4
Questionnaire introduction text Link: http://kristin-dev.web.cern.ch/survey I am currently a Fellow in the CERN Diversity Office and at the same time com-pleting a Master's degree in Intercultural Communication Studies. For my Master thesis, I am researching the diversity climate at CERN, using the Organisational Diversity Needs Analysis (ODNA), a questionnaire which is a validated instrument in social science to measure diversity awareness. It was used at universities, com-panies as well as public institutions. I’m using it now to measure your perception of diversity at CERN. I'm looking for people who have been at CERN for at least 4 months in the past year. Your support by filling out the survey is much appreciated. It should not take you more than 15 minutes to complete. I kindly ask you to com-plete the survey by 10 March 2016.
The research carried out is "personal/academic" and not related to CERN's diver-sity initiatives, although I am receiving support from CERN through access to doc-uments and data. The information you provide will be treated anonymously. Con-fidentiality is guaranteed and only the global results will serve as a source of infor-mation to the CERN Diversity Office, not individual replies. The data you provide will not be used by any other service of the CERN Human Resources Department. The questionnaire covers the following dimensions: organisational inclusion, cul-tural group inclusion, the value of differences, work load, trust, affirmative-action, adaptation and sensitivity. If you have any comments or questions that you would like to address, please don't hesitate to get in touch with me: [email protected] Kristin

83
ANNEX 5
The Questionnaire – CERN version Link: http://kristin-dev.web.cern.ch/diversity-survey Have you been at CERN for at least four months in the last year?
- Yes - No
Following questions have been posed to rate on a 5-item Likert scale: ● Strongly disagree ● Disagree ● Neutral ● Agree ● Strongly agree I. Cultural Group Inclusion/Exclusion Explanation: Think about the people you work with regularly, particularly those you consider to be your team at CERN. Please respond by indicating how much you agree or disagree with each statement as it relates to your experience in the team you work with, particularly when you are at CERN.
1. Some people in our team are “talked down to” by their colleagues because they are different.
2. Sometimes people who talk and act differently are treated like they aren’t capable or smart.
3. There are people in our team who have a hard time accepting ideas when they are offered by someone who is different from them.
4. When people begin working on a problem from a very different perspective they have a hard time seeing each other‘s point of view.
5. When people from different backgrounds work together in groups, some people feel ignored because their ideas are not acknowledged.
6. People get ahead by being persuasive and not because of what they know. 7. Diversity issues keep some teams at CERN from performing to their maxi-
mum effectiveness. II. Organisational Inclusion/Exclusion Explanation: Please select "N/A" if not applicable.
8. It seems that the real reason people are denied promotions or raises is that they are seen as not fitting in.

84
9. It’s hard to get ahead at CERN unless you are part of the “old boys’ net-work”.
10. When it comes to getting support for getting ahead at CERN, I feel like I have been overlooked.
11. I have to prove myself more and work a lot harder to get into that next posi-tion because of my gender, nationality, sexual orientation, belief or disabil-ity.
12. Performance evaluations seem to be biased against those who are different because supervisors focus on very traditional ways of getting things done.
13. Whenever I‘ve confronted someone for giving me a hard time because of my gender, nationality, sexual orientation, belief or disability, they have de-nied the problem or played it down.
14. I feel I have been treated differently at CERN because of my gender, na-tionality, sexual orientation, belief or disability.
III. Valuing Differences
15. When people have a different orientation or style, they take the time to ex-plain and try to understand the other person‘s point of view.
16. When people in our team take offense or miscommunicate, they sit down and talk about the differences until they understand each other.
17. Colleagues share their knowledge/expertise with other colleagues regard-less of their nationality, gender, belief, sexual orientation or physical ability.
18. We all seem to learn and grow from our differences. 19. When someone is timid or reluctant to assert their ideas, others will point it
out and ask for their opinion. 20. People in our team don‘t notice differences related to nationality, gender,
belief or sexual orientation since we are really all the same. 21. People's habits or ways of thinking may be different because of their back-
ground, but when it comes to working, we‘re pretty much all the same. IV. Work Load
22. The amount of work I have makes me feel extremely stressed at the end of the day.
23. I feel like I am given more work than I can reasonably handle. 24. My workload is so heavy that I feel like I can‘t possibly finish during an ordi-
nary workday. 25. The amount of work I have interferes with how well I can do my job.

85
V. Trust
26. People of the same nationality, gender, belief or sexual orientation tend to look out for each other.
27. With many people, you don‘t know where you stand. (removed after Cronbach’s α test)
28. If I were having difficulties, I know members of my team would try to help me out.
29. I can trust people I work with to lend me a hand if I need it. 30. I feel confident that CERN will always try to treat me fairly. 31. CERN is sincere in its attempts to understand the employee‘s point of view.
VI. Affirmative-Action Group Perception Explanation: The term minority is used here along the following dimensions: na-tionality, gender, belief, sexual orientation and physical ability.
32. Minorities are more likely to get promoted and get ahead now. 33. Minorities don‘t adjust their style to fit the business context. (removed after
Cronbach’s α test) 34. People are sometimes hired for a job with less qualifications than other ap-
plicants because they are minorities. 35. Members of some nationality have a unique way of acting in workgroups
which makes them stand out as being different. (removed after Cronbach’s α test)
36. Sometimes minorities should be given special consideration in hiring and promotions.
37. Minorities don‘t have the same employment opportunities as others have. VII. Adaptation Explanation: The term minority is used here along the following dimensions: na-tionality, gender, belief, sexual orientation and physical ability.
38. I feel like it‘s up to me to adjust to others when e.g. their nationality or gender presents differences in styles or mannerisms.
39. Working with colleagues with different backgrounds forces me to reconsider how I approach things.
40. I go out of my way to learn about others‘ cultural backgrounds, traditions and points of view.
41. Knowing more about the cultural norms of diverse groups would help me to be more effective in my job.
42. I think that diverse viewpoints add value. 43. I can enjoy being with people whose culture, gender, belief or sexual orien-
tation is very different from mine.

86
44. I believe minorities should adopt the values and lifestyle of the dominant culture.
VIII. Sensitivity/Flexibility Explanation: The term minority is used here along the following dimensions: na-tionality, gender, belief, sexual orientation and physical ability.
45. It seems that minorities have a special sensitivity and understanding of di-versity issues.
46. Minorities are better able to value the different ways of looking at things and doing work than most others.
47. Minorities often interpret things differently than most others. 48. Minorities use the diverse backgrounds of others to help with group prob-
lem-solving. 49. Minorities often encourage others to express their opinion. 50. It seems that minorities are more likely to adjust their styles of work when
necessary. 51. It seems that minorities express more empathy for others than most others.
Demographics Age:
- Under 18 - 18-25 - 26-35 - 36-45 - 46-55 - 56-65 - Over 65
I identify my gender as…
- Female - Male - Trans*
Nationality (Dropdown list) Status at CERN (Dropdown list) Department

87
(Dropdown list) Do you feel part of a minority at CERN, other than related to your gender or na-tionality (e.g. sexual orientation, physical ability, belief, etc.)? - Yes (If you want, please specify) - No Do you have supervisory responsibilities at CERN? - Yes - No Are you interested to participate in a one-on-one interview with the researcher? - Yes - No - Maybe Thank you for participating in this research project! Feel free to use this area for comments, concerning this questionnaire or how you feel about diversity in your team or at CERN.

88
ANNEX 6
Cronbach’s α for all eight dimensions
Table 46: Cronbach’s α for all eight dimensions
Subscale Number of
items Cronbach's
α Deleted items
Cronbach's α af-ter deletion
Cultural Group Inclusion
7 0.908* - -
Organisational Inclusion
7 0.914* - -
Valuing Differences
7 0.789* - -
Work load
4 0.913* - -
Trust
6 (5) 0.403* Q27 0.693*
Affirmative-action Group Perception
6 (4) 0.353 Q33, Q37 0.634*
Adaptation
7 0.664* - -
Sensitivity/Flexibility
7 0.866* - -
* p > 0.6

89
ANNEX 7 Results for test of Equality of variances, t-test and ANOVA for all independent variables and dimensions A7.1 Results for eight dimensions: H1 Gender A7.1.1 Gender – Cultural Group Inclusion Table 47: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Cultural Group Inclusion
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .282 .596 3.318*** 181 .001 .188 .741
Not assu-med
3.305 174.321 .001 .187 .742
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
The table shows that the assumption of equal variances was not violated. The
significance level for Levene’s test was .60, which is larger than the cut-off at .05;
therefore equal variances were assumed.
A7.1.2 Gender – Organisational Inclusion Table 48: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Organisational Inclusion
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.542 .216 3.293*** 181 .001 .187 .748
Not assu-med
3.264 169.073 .001 .185 .751
Note. N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
In the table, it was noted that the assumption of equal variances had not been
violated because the significance level for Levene’s test was .22, which is larger
than the cut-off of .05; therefore, equal variances were assumed.

90
A7.1.3 Gender – Valuing differences Table 49: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Valuing Differences
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.356 .246 -3.104* 181 .002 -.510 -.113
Not assu-med
-3.079 170.017 .002 -.511 -.111
Note. N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
The data supports the assumption of equal variances because the significance
level for Levene’s test was .25, which is larger than the cut-off at .05.
A7.1.4 Gender – Work Load Table 50: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Work Load
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .007 .932 .522 181 .602 -.235 .405
Not assu-med
.522 177.408 .602 -.235 .405
Note. N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
The table proves that the assumption of equal variances was not violated because
the significance level for Levene’s test was .93, which is larger than the cut-off at
.05; therefore, equal variances were assumed.

91
A7.1.5 Gender – Trust Table 51: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Trust
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .009 .926 -.602 181 .548 -.244 -.130
Not assu-med
-.606 180.683 .545 -.242 -.128
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .93 (p > .05).
A7.1.6 Gender – Affirmative-Action Group Perception Table 52: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Affirmative-Action Group Perception
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .002 .961 2.503* 181 .013 .057 .485
Not assu-med
2.506 178.166 .013 .057 .485
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .96 (p > .05)

92
A7.1.7 Gender – Adaptation Table 53: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Adaptation
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 5.899 .016 3.067 181 .002 .077 .358
Not assu-med
3.141* 174.198 .002 .081 .355
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
The data does not support the assumption of equal variances because the signifi-
cance level for Levene’s test was .02, which is smaller than the cut-off at .05.
Therefore unequal variances were assumed and the Welch correction was applied.
A7.1.8 Gender – Sensitivity/ Flexibility Table 54: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for women and men: Sensitivity/Flexibility
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .000 .986 2.976* 181 .003 .098 .485
Not assu-med
2.984 178.984 .003 .098 .484
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom.
*p < .05. *** p <.001.
Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .99 (p > .05).

93
A7.2. Results for eight dimensions: H2 Nationality A7.2.1 Nationality – Cultural Group Inclusion Table 55: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Cultural Group Inclusion
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .048 .826 1.813 181 .072 -.031 .739
Not assu-med
1.698 37.230 .098 -.068 .776
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .83 which is larger than the cut-off at .05.
A7.2.2 Nationality – Organisational Inclusion Table 56: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Organisational Inclusion
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .445 .506 .144 181 .886 -.365 .423
Not assu-med
36.440 .897 -.416 .474
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
The data in the above table supports the assumption of equal variances because
the significance level for Levene’s test was .51 (p > .05)

94
A7.2.3 Nationality – Valuing Differences Table 57: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Valuing Differences
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.923 .167 -1.623 181 .106 -.503 .048
Not assu-med
-1.409 35.290 .168 -.554 .100
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .17, which is larger than the cut-off at .05.
A7.2.4 Nationality – Work Load Table 58: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Work Load
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .811 .369 1.768 181 .079 -.04508 .82307
Not assu-med
1.900 42.104 .064 -.02412 .80211
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001. The data shows that the assumption of equal variances was not violated because
the significance level for Levene’s test was .37, which is larger than the cut-off at
.05.

95
A7.2.5 Nationality – Trust
Table 59: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Trust
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.450 .230 .407 181 .685 -.203 .308
Not assu-med
.351 35.75 .727 -.252 .357
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001. Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .23 (p > .05).
A7.2.6 Nationality – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
Table 60: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Affirmative-Action Group Perception
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.184 .278 2.983* 181 .003 .148 .729
Not assu-med
3.509 46.810 .001 .187 .690
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001. The table demonstrates that the assumption of equal variances was not violated
because the significance level for Levene’s test was .28 (p > .05).

96
A7.2.7 Nationality - Adaptation
Table 61: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Adaptation
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.021 .314 1.307 181 .193 -.066 .325
Not assu-med
1.504 45.530 .139 -.043 .303
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001. The data supports the assumption of equal variances because the significance
level for Levene’s test was .31 (p > .05)
A7.2.8 Nationality – Sensitivity/ Flexibility
Table 62: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for Europeans and Non-Europeans: Sensitivity/Flexibility
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.139 .287 2.003* 181 .047 .004 .539
Not assu-med
2.217 43.475 .032 .024 .519
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001. Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .29 (p > .05).

97
A7.3. Results for eight dimensions: H3 Managerial responsibilities
A7.3.1 Managerial responsibilities – Cultural Group Inclusion
Table 63: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Cultural Group Inclusion
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .047 .829 -.953 181 .342 -.503 .175
Not assu-med
-.938 63.410 .352 -.513 .185
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
The table shows that the assumption of equal variances was not violated. The
significance level for Levene’s test was .83, which is larger than the cut-off at .05.
A7.3.2 Managerial responsibilities – Organisational Inclusion
Table 64: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Organisational Inclusion
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 1.477 .226 -.736 181 .463 -.473 .216
Not assu-med
-.669 57.383 .506 -.513 .256
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
The table demonstrates that the assumption of equal variances was not violated
because the significance level for Levene’s test was .23 (p > .05).

98
A7.3.3 Managerial responsibilities – Valuing Differences
Table 65: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Valuing Differences
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .249 .618 .018 181 .986 -.241 .246
Not assu-med
.018 62.884 .986 -.250 .254
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
The data in the above table supports the assumption of equal variances because
the significance level for Levene’s test was .62, which is larger than the cut-off at
.05.
A7.3.4 Managerial responsibilities – Work Load
Table 66: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Work Load
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .065 .798 -2.46* 181 .015 -.847 -.093
Not assu-med
-2.54 68.220 .013 -.839 -.101
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
The above table proves that the assumption of equal variances was not violated
because the significance level for Levene’s test was .80, which is larger than the
cut-off at .05, therefore, equal variances were assumed.

99
A7.3.5 Managerial responsibilities – Trust
Table 67: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Trust
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 2.04 .155 -.458 181 .647 -.276 .172
Not assu-med
-.533 84.299 .596 -.246 .142
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
Equal variances can be assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .16 (p > .05).
A7.3.6 Managerial responsibilities – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
Table 68: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Affirmative-Action Group Perception
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 2.78 .097 -.262 181 .793 -.295 .225
Not assu-med
-.290 76.431 .773 -.272 .203
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
The above table demonstrates that the assumption of equal variances was not
violated because the significance level for Levene’s test was .10 (p > .05).
A7.3.7 Managerial responsibilities – Adaptation
Table 69: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Adaptation
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed 3.31 .070 -.082 181 .935 -.179 .165
Not assu-med
-.096 85.980 .923 -.154 .140
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.

100
The data in above table supports the assumption of equal variances because the
significance level for Levene’s test was .07 (p > .05).
A7.3.8 Managerial responsibilities – Sensitivity/Flexibility
Table 70: Equality of variances (Levene’s) and Mean (independent t-test) for managerial responsibilities: Sensitivity/Flexibility
95% CI
Equal vari-ances
F Sig. t df p Lower Upper
Assumed .020 .888 -.759 181 .935 -.327 .145
Not assu-med
-.772 66.552 .923 -.326 .144
Note: N = 183. CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .89 (p > .05).
A7.4. Results for eight dimensions: H4 Generation (Age)
Table71: Equality of variances for 8 dimensions: Generation (Age)
Levene‘s statistic
df1
df2
Sig. 95 % CI
Lower Upper
Cultural Group Inclu-sion
.557
2
179
.574
2.477 2.762
Organisational Inclu-sion
.005 2 179 .995 2.40 2.69
Valuing Differences .524 2 179 .593 3.25 3.45 Work Load 1.188 2 179 .307 2.78 3.10 Trust .255 2 179 .775 3.27 3.45 Affirmative-Action Group Perception
.369 2 179 .692 3.00 3.22
Adaptation .442 2 179 .643 3.86 4.01 Sensitivity/Flexibility .143 2 179 .867 2.88 3.08
Note: N = 182; CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.

101
A7.4.1 Generation (Age) – Cultural Group Inclusion
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .57 (p > .05).
A7.4.2 Generation (Age) – Organisational Inclusion
Equal variances were assumed because the significance level for Levene’s test
was .99 (p > .05).
A7.4.3 Generation (Age) – Valuing Differences
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .59 (p > .05).
A7.4.4 Generation (Age) – Work Load
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .31 (p > .05).
A7.4.5 Generation (Age) – Trust
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .78 (p > .05).
A7.4.6 Generation (Age) – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .69 (p > .05).
A7.4.7 Generation (Age) – Adaptation
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .64 (p > .05).
A7.4.8 Generation (Age) – Sensitivity/Flexibility
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .87 (p > .05).

102
A7.5. Results for eight dimensions: H5 CERN Status
Table 72: Equality of variances for 8 dimensions: CERN Status
Levene‘s statistic
df1
df2
Sig. 95 % CI
Lower Upper
Cultural Group In-clusion
.527
4
165
.716
2.48
2.78
Organisational In-clusion
.189
4
165
.944
2.38
2.68
Valuing Diffe-rences
.992 4 165 .413 3.25 3.46
Work Load 3.77 4 165 .006* 2.74 3.06 Trust .652 4 165 .626 3.28 3.48 Affirmative-Action Group Perception
.255 4 165 .906 3.01 3.24
Adaptation .63 4 165 .645 3.87 4.02 Sensitivity/Flexibi-lity
.68 4 165 .605 2.89 3.09
Note: N = 170; CI = confidence interval; df = degrees of freedom. *p < .05. *** p <.001.
A7.5.1 CERN Status – Cultural Group Inclusion
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .72 (p > .05).
A7.5.2 CERN Status – Organisational Inclusion
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .94 (p > .05).
A7.5.3 CERN Status – Valuing Differences
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .41 (p > .05).
A7.5.4 CERN Status – Work Load
The assumption of equal variances was violated because the significance level for
Levene’s test was .006, which is smaller than the cut-off at .05. Therefore unequal
variances are assumed and the Welch correction is applied.

103
A7.5.5 CERN Status – Trust
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .63 (p > .05).
A7.5.6 CERN Status – Affirmative-Action Group Perception
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .91 (p > .05).
A7.5.7 CERN Status – Adaptation
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .65 (p > .05).
A7.5.8 CERN Status – Sensitivity/Flexibility
The assumption of equal variances was not violated because the significance level
for Levene’s test was .61 (p > .05).

104
Declaration of Originality
I declare herewith, that this thesis paper is my own original work. Furthermore, I
confirm that:
− this work has been composed by me;
− I have clearly referenced all sources used in the work;
− all data and findings in the work have not been falsified or embellished;
− this work has not been previously, or concurrently, used either for other
courses or as an exam work;
− this work has not been published before.
I understand that any false claim in respect of this work will result in failure of this
assessment due to plagiarism.
……………………………………………………
Erfurt, 18 July 2016