ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

download ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

of 65

Transcript of ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    1/65

    M. ERNANDES & M. WINKELMAN

    Theopoiesis

    or

    The Production of God Concepts byBrain Structures and Operators

    in Early Humans

    Michele (Michael) Ernandes:

    Department of Historical-Archaeological, Socio-Anthropological and Geographical

    Heritage,

    Palermo University, Scientific Centre of the Polytechnic of the Mediterranean, Italy.

    (i. e., Dipartimento di Beni Culturali Storico-Archeologici, Socio-Antropologici e

    Geografici, Universit di Palermo, Italia)

    Email: [email protected]

    Michael Winkelman:

    School of Human Evolution & Social Change

    Arizona State University

    Email: [email protected]

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    2/65

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    3/65

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    4/65

    of Hominini endowed with low sexual dimorphism (as in genusHomo), there wasnt any

    individual that could act as realistic example of a Being with immense power. How did

    it happen, therefore, that among human beings with a low male-male competition social

    system we can find a projection in the super- human world of a being with immense

    power that should have to be associated with a high male-male competition social

    system? A possible answer is that the human brain preserved structures and hierarchy

    forming functions in its R-complex. Consequently we can deduce that at some time in the

    course of human evolution the hierarchy forming structure of the R-complex has been set

    free from the inhibitory action of the Limbic System; this was presumably a consequence

    of a strong external stimulus which, causing a psychic trauma, weakened the inhibitory

    action of the Limbic System. Indeed, according to evolutionary psychology, all human

    behaviors are a product of internal mechanisms in conjunction with inputs that cause

    activation of those mechanisms. We may presume that the input for the Prehistoric man

    consisted in the acquisition of the awareness of his own mortality. Homo sapiens is

    conscious of being mortal, but he does not recognize this fact as a natural datum. Instead

    he feels that death is a violence he has to suffer: we may presume that in this way death

    could have been considered by early humans. As Neocortex seeks to determine agents

    who cause phenomena, early humans tried to find the cause of death, but failed to find an

    empirical cause. Because of this shock, the Limbic System activity on R-complex would

    have had a variation that caused the activation of hierarchic R-complex structures and that

    led the Neocortical structures to accept the idea, proposed by the R-complex, that aPowerful, but unseen, Being was the agent of death. After this, the Neocortical

    association areas, in relations to environment, developed various systems of religion.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    5/65

    I. Definition, observations and assumption concerning the Biological Basis of

    Religion

    Tylor (1871) defined religion as belief in immaterial and extraordinary beings as

    souls, ghosts, saints, angels, demons, and gods. This definition has been emphasized by

    many authors, such as M. Harris (1989), who characterized religion in terms of belief in

    the existence of one or more of such beings. There are other broader definitions of

    religion, but the supernatural agent concept is invariably central, as it is for our present

    purpose.

    Since the 16th century, ethnographic studies have suggested that such religious ideas

    are universal. In 1744 Giambattista Vico noticed that all cultures have had three

    institution: Religion, marriage, and burial. Contemporary scholars also asserted the

    universality of religious ideas, such as A. Brelich (1970): No society has been found,

    even among the most primitive, which was devoid of any faith in divine person-like

    beings. Such sentiments were elaborated by Rappaport (1971, p. 23), who claimed

    Neither history nor anthropology knows of societies from which religion has been totally

    absent and echoed more recently by Boyer (2003, p. 119) who asserts that Religious

    beliefs and practices are found in all human groups.

    Many anthropologists would concur that when an aspect of behavior is universal to

    human societies, it is likely that it has some kind of genetic basis. It seems reasonable to

    suppose that religion has a biological basis given that it is universal, and surmise that

    some aspects of religious behavior and thought may be hardwired in a genetic

    predisposition. Spirits seem naturally conceivable, thinkable, to the human mind - and the

    human brain must have certain anatomic and physiologic structures which have favored

    the birth of religious ideas that subsequently become a part of the cultural heritage. In this

    vein, Boyer (2003, p. 123) notes that Cognitive science and neuroscience suggests a less

    dramatic but perhaps more empirically grounded picture of religion as a probable,

    although by no means inevitable by-product of the normal operation of human cognition.

    Religious thoughts and behaviors may be studied at various levels:

    afirst levelmay regard the biological bases, neurological or ethological, of religious

    thought (Mandell 1980, Gazzaniga 1985, Alper 1996, Burkert 1996, Ernandes &

    Giammanco 1998, Winkelman 2000, W. and Baker 2008, Boyer 2001, 2003, Hamer

    2004);

    a second level, phenomenologic-structural, the subordinate relationship of human

    beings to divine beings (Hubert and Mauss 1898, van der Leew 1933, Widengren 1969,

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    6/65

    Burkert 1972), as well as semeiotic values of religious thoughts and behaviors;

    a third, the relationships between social structures and peculiar shapes of credence

    and rituals (Weber 1904/05, Durkheim 1912, and their sociological schools) or the social

    value of religious rites (Sosis and Bressler 2003);

    afourth, the economic-ecological one, the material goods used for religious rituals

    (Firth 1963, Harris 1977).

    These four approaches can coexist and interact synchronically with a fifth one, the

    historical ordiachronic aspect.

    Obviously, for a given religion, results of studies carried out on one level must be

    coherent with the results obtained for other levels, since a comprehensive study must be

    non-contradictory. Until now the tendency to favor a particular type of study to the

    exclusion of others has prevailed (Pals 1996). This may have been reasonable in the past,

    as these various approaches were discovered little by little, but it is less excusable now. In

    1966 C. Geertz noted stagnation in the field of anthropology of religion and foreboded the

    utilization of other disciplines such as philosophy, history or hard sciences with the aim of

    achieving a thick description of religion.

    As noted by Sosis and Alcorta (2003, p. 264), the multiple roles and complex

    functions of religion render it difficult to capture within a single theoretical approach, so

    it requires different methodological tools. In the present article we start from biological

    evolution, neurobiology and ethology with the aim of furnishing the ground for a

    multilevel study of religion.

    Returning to Tylor's definition, religious thought consists of belief in extra-natural

    power beings. We conceptualize an Immense Power Being as the most general and

    comprehensive of these beings, defined as a supernatural being with immense power over

    human lives. As has been established by Kant (1781), it is impossible to prove rationally

    the existence or the non-existence of any Immense Power Being (or, in Kants words,

    God).Consequently, our purpose is not to know if God exists or does not, but rather

    consists in an effort to understand why God concepts exists so naturally and

    comfortably in the human mind, why such ideas exist universally in human beings. It is

    pertinent to investigate such a question by means of the natural sciences, because

    Thoughts and beliefs are necessarily dependent on neuro-physiological activity of the

    brain (Delgado 1969).

    According to evolutionary psychology, all human (or animal) behavior is a product

    of mechanisms internal to the person (or to the animal), in conjunction with inputs that

    trigger the activation of those mechanisms. No mechanisms, no behavior; no input, no

    behavior(adapted from Buss & Shackelford 1997, p. 607). If we consider the production

    of ideas as a particular kind of behavior (an internal brain behavior), our aim then

    consists in investigating the internal mechanisms and the input that had lead human mind

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    7/65

    to conceive the Immense Power Being concept.

    II. MacLeans Triune Model of the Brain

    Genetic predisposition operates through the development of encephalic structures.

    In Vertebrates the Central Nervous System grows up as a dorsal tube that,

    subsequently, is surrounded by vertebrae (thespinal cord) and by skull (the encephalon).

    A canal goes along the tube and forms at first three vesicle with surrounding swellings

    (forebrain, midbrain and hindbrain). These formations then become five ones:

    telencephalon (endbrain), diencephalon (interbrain), mesencephalon (midbrain),

    metencephalon (pons and cerebellum), and myelencephalon (medulla oblongata). (Fig. II.

    1).

    At the end of the development, the hindbrain vesicle is termed fourth ventricle, the

    midbrain vesicle becomes thin and is called aquaeductus mesencephali (orcerebri), the

    interbrain vesicle forms the third ventricle.

    The telencephalic vesicle, in growing forward impeded by the frontal bone, in its

    development overturns and covers the diencephalon, splitting and forming two lateral

    ventricles. In a section of each telencephalic vesicle we may note four parts: the lateral

    and medial basal ones, and the lateral and medial dorsal ones. (Fig. II. 2 a).In the basal parts neurons form some masses (called nuclei organglia), while in the

    dorsal parts neurons are disposed in layers (normally three).

    P. D. MacLean (1970-1990) elaborated a model of brain structure and evolution. He

    described it as a Triune brain, because he located in it three principal phylogenetic

    structures that have been superimposed and that have become integrated during evolution.

    He termed these three basic types reptilian (Protoreptilian, R-complex), old mammalian

    (Paleomammalian, Limbic System) and new mammalian (Neo-mammalian). The

    heuristic value of this model, for a long time underappreciated, has been revalued by

    many scholars (Winkelman 2000; Cory & Gardner 2002; Oatley 2004; Whybrow 2005).

    Here we examine the triune brain model, reappraising it in relation with recent knowledge

    in neurobiological and evolutionary sciences, and in terms of its applicability to

    explaining the neurological basis of the Immense Power Being.

    The R-complex

    The protoreptilian brain represents a fundamental core of the nervous system: it

    consists in the upper spinal cord, parts of the midbrain and the diencephalons, and the

    lateral basal part of the telencephalic vesicle. The lateral basal telencephalon consists in

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    8/65

    the striatum and pallidum as main components, and in the substantia nigra, ventral

    tegmental area and subthalamic nucleus as smaller components. The striatum comprises

    dorsally the caudate nucleus and putamen, ventrally the nucleus accumbens and the

    olfactory tubercle. The globus pallidus (orpallidum) consists in three parts: the internal

    and the external segments, and the ventral pallidum (fig. II. 3). The ventral part of the

    striatum and the ventral pallidum are also called paleostriatum, the dorsal striatum

    neostriatum: however there is no evolutionary evidence supporting these names, that

    must be considered old-fashioned (fig. II.2 c).

    The medial basal part of the telencephalon gives rise to the septum, while the dorsal

    part forms the pallium (or cortex): these structures are not included in the R-complex, not

    because they are absent, but because they are less developed and are not indispensable in

    determining the fundamental behaviors in Reptiles and Birds (so that the term Striatal

    complex is more accurate but less suggestive than R-complex). The presence of the R-

    complex as a stage in the brain evolution from Fish to Mammals has been corroborated by

    subsequent research in anatomy and physiology. Reiner, Medina and Veenman (1998)

    have found that both striatum and pallidum are typically much more cell poor in

    Anamniotes (Fish and Amphibians) than they are in Amniotes (Reptiles, Birds and

    Mammals). Moreover, neural pathways connecting basal ganglia and the telencephalic

    cortex are less important in Reptiles and Birds than in Mammals (Striedter 2005), as it is

    shown in fig II. 5 f.

    According to MacLean, the R-complex in the brains of existing mammals derivesfrom a form of mammal-like reptiles that, in Permian and Triassic geologic periods,

    populated the earth in large numbers. Even if for comparative neurobehavioral study, no

    existing reptiles are directly in line with mammals, it has been noticed that most ancient

    mammal-like reptiles were lizard-like in appearance, so that MacLean and following

    scholars have favored the use of lizards in their comparative studies. Moreover, the

    lizards chose are ones in which the cortical part of telencephalon is small, and not those

    ones provided with evident pallial gyri, as the tegu lizard (Tupinambis teguixin), which

    has a paleo and an archeo cortical gyri surrounding a neocortical gyrus.

    MacLean (1973a, p. 8) wrote that the counterpart of the reptilian brain in mammals

    is fundamental for genetically constituted forms of behavior as selecting homesites,

    establishing territory engaging in various types of display, hunting, homing, mating,

    breeding, imprinting, forming social hierarchies, and selecting leaders. In mammals, the

    R-complex is necessary for ritualistic displays and the averbal communication associated

    with them. At the human level certain behavioral tendencies are due to an inheritance

    of dispositions mediated by this same, primal brain region. These include certain violent

    reactions, the preference for routine or ritualistic actions, and some forms of

    displacement activities (Isaacson 1982, p. 246).

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    9/65

    The Limbic System.

    From Fish to Reptiles, the basal parts of the telencephalon are more abounding in

    neurons than the dorsal parts, which appear as a thin mantle (the pallium) or cortex that

    covers the lateral vesicle. In Mammals the cortex expands a lot, wrapping the basal part of

    the telencephalon. Arins Kappers (1909) termed the lateral cortex (whose main derived

    region is called piriform cortex) as paleopallium, and the medial cortex (whose main

    structure is the hyppocampus) as archipallium. In these nouns the paleo- and archi-

    prefixes have no more the temporal or evolutionary significance that first XX century

    authors intended give them. Their significance, related topallium, is now only spatial (fig.

    II 2 b, c and d). Paleo- and archipallium neurons are disposed in three layers: this

    disposition is termed allocortex. In the phylogenetically subsequent neocortex, derived

    from the dorsal pallium, neurons are disposed in six layers to form the isocortex. Paleo-

    and archi-pallium form the largest regions of the Limbic System, which also includes the

    olfactory bulb, septum, fornix, amygdala, and cingulated gyrus.

    The paleomammalian brain, or the limbic system, represents an advance in neural

    tissue because it represents a device for providing the animals that have this tissue with

    better means of coping with the environment. Parts of the limbic system are concerned

    with primal activities related to food and sex; others are related to emotions and feelings;

    and still others combine messages from the external world with those from inside(Isaacson 1982, p. 246).

    MacLean (1990) has identified three main limbic subdivisions located respectively in

    the amygdale, septum, and thalamus that are a source of afferents to the limbic cortex (fig.

    II. 4).

    I) The amygdalar division comprises the amygdala together with the frontotemporal

    limbic cortex; the amygdala is also connected with the hippocampus. This division is

    primarily involved with fear, aggression, and mimic judgment (Morris et al. 1998;

    Adolphs et al. 1998).

    II) The septal division projects to the entire hippocampal formation and also includes

    the entorhinal cortex (gyrus hippocampi). This division is mainly connected with sex

    activities. The amygdala and septum serve as telencephalic internodes for neural circuits

    relating the rostrally located olfactory apparatus: all together they form the

    rhinencephalon.

    III) The thalamocingulate division is comprised of the mesocortical cingulated areas

    receiving afferents from the anterior and other thalamic nuclei. It also comprises

    mammillary bodies of the hypothalamus. Differently from other parts of Limbic System,

    the thalamocingulate division seems to have no representation in the reptilian brain

    (MacLean 1990, p. 247). This division is concerned with three cardinal behavioral

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    10/65

    developments that, in addition to endothermy, characterize the evolutionary transition

    from ancient Reptiles to Mammals. Namely: 1) Nursing, in conjunction with maternal

    care; 2) audio-vocal communication for maintaining maternal-offspring contact; and 3)

    play (MacLean 1990, p. 380). Nursing is due to the action of oxytocin, the mammalian

    hypothalamic hormone that derives from the reptilian hormone mesotocin. This change

    may be considered the biochemical border marking the transition from the primordial

    Limbic System of Reptiles to the regular Limbic System of Mammals (Insel & Young

    2000).

    Another important modification has affected the vagus nerve. Only Mammals have a

    myelinated, and therefore faster, vagus, so that heart activity may be quickly linked to the

    Limbic systems emotions. According to the polyvagal theory (Porges 2001), in

    mammals, both neocortical and reptilian strucutres, but primarily Limbic ones, control the

    activity of the vagus and other cranial nerves that govern facial expressions and

    vocalizations in social interactions: such non verbal communications of unconscious

    emotions and conscious intentions constitute the bases of Primates social life.

    The Limbic System may be seen as a regulator of the R-complex. Most of this

    regulation seems to be inhibitory and acted via the serotonergic system. Laboratory

    stimulation of the Limbic System often produces a suppression of R-complex ongoing

    behaviors, whereas lesions made in it often seem to release various reptilian activities(adapted from Isaacson 1982, p. 246). This means that the mammalian R-complex is not

    exactly the same as the reptilian R-complex: in fact the mammalian one receives new

    inputs from limbic structures and it has also fit receptors (often different compared with

    reptilian ones) for neuromodulators or neurotransmitters that mediates these inputs.

    Furthermore, as early noted by MacLean (1970), alterations of the usual activities of

    the Limbic System, e. g. by epileptic episodes, can produce various experiences and

    feelings, as those associated with knowledge of fundamental truths, feeling of

    depersonalization, hallucinations, and paranoid feelings.

    Other observations about the action of the Limbic System on the R-complex will be

    shown in the subsequent section ( III).

    The cerebral cortex is the most evident part of human encephalon, and it is divided in

    two hemispheres, which are connected by three fibrous structures: the corpus callosum,

    and the anterior and posteriorcommissures. Each hemisphere is constituted by numerous

    convex formations calledgyri, divided each other bysulchi. In each hemisphere we may

    also note four regions called lobes: the frontal lobe comprises the upon ocular socket area

    and behind the forehead up to near the top of the brain, where the central sulcus divides it

    from the parietal lobe; the temporal lobe is located under frontal and parietal lobes and it

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    11/65

    covers a little lobe, the insular one; the occipital lobe forms the back of the brain (fig II. 5

    a).

    In a side view of a cross section of the brain (fig. II. 5 b), we can see a large gyrus

    surrounding the corpus callosum: it is called cingulate gyrus and is constituted by

    mesocortex, which is intermediate between allo- and isocortex. The cingulate gyrus, in

    the basal brain, continues as piriform cortex (derived from the ancient lateral pallium).

    Inside this latter is a structure derived from the ancient medial pallium (archicortex),

    which is called hippocampus. Cingulate gyrus, piriform cortex and hippocampus

    constitute the limbic cortex. Cingulate and rhinal sulchi divide the cerebral limbic cortex

    from the Neocortex, that in apes and humans constitutes the major part of the cerebral

    hemispheres.

    The Neomammalian brain

    The Neomammalian brain consists of the Neocortex and structures of the brainstem

    with which it is connected, as lemnisci, pyramidal tracts, and neothalamus. The

    Neocortex is, on the human level, the seat of language and, in general, it is the seat of

    those behaviors that allow a person to tackle new and unexpected situations. The ability to

    foresee the future resides in it. We owe conscious thought to the Neocortex: it is the main

    seat of mind capabilities as self-consciousness or the connections of causality, as well

    see in the following III .

    The whole brain. As MacLean (1973a: 7; 1973b: 114) has made clear, the three

    basal brain types show differences both in structure (1) and chemistry (2), and so they are

    capable of functioning somewhat independently; but they are in no sense separate,

    autonomous entities: they must intermesh and function together as a triune brain.

    (1) As regards the structure, we have seen that in the R-complex neurons form

    masses (nuclei or ganglia); in the Limbic System besides nuclei there are pallial (or

    cortical) structures, in which neurons are disposed in three (allocortex) or more layers

    (mesocortex). Neocortex has neurons disposed in six layers (isocortex).

    (2) As regards the chemistry, researchers have found less marked differences: while,

    for example, acetylcholine is typical, but not exclusive, of the R-complex (MacLean

    1990: 38-43), dopamine and serotonin are well allocated in all three brain types.

    Dopamine is synthesized by hydroxylation and subsequent decarboxylation of the

    amino acid tyrosine (that may also derive from phenylalanine). In Mammals there are four

    main dopaminergic pathways: (a) the nigro-striatal, (b) the infundibular, (c) the

    mesolimbic and (d) the mesocortical ones (fig II 5 d, modified from Kandel et al. 1996).

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    12/65

    They function respectively most in (a) tuning of sensori-motor programs, (b) regulation of

    hormone release in the pituitary gland, (c) modulation of sensory perception, maintaining

    reward values from life experiences and, thus, motivation up to addiction or aversion, (d)

    regulation of frontal cortex activity (Kandel et al. 1996).

    Dopamine reward system originates in the ventral tegmental area of the midbrain and

    projects to the nucleus accumbens of the ventral striatum. This system is termed

    mesolimbic by Kandel and colleagues, who dont use the triune tripartition. In the

    triune brain conception, it is more properly a reptilian subsystem (so in fig. II. 5 d we

    have emphasized this pathway).

    Serotonin (or 5-Hydroxytryptamine or 5-HT) is a compound produced in some

    nervous centers starting from the amino acid tryptophan, and it exerts its effects by acting

    as a classic neurotransmitter or as a neuromodulator (these effects are due to postsynaptic

    receptors: 5-HT receptors are many and diversify serotonins effects in the various

    encephalic regions. See Baumgarten & Gthert 1997). Serotoninergic neurons are present

    in all animals that possess nervous systems, both Protostoma and Deuterostoma . In

    Vertebrates, nervous centers in which 5-HT is produced are raphe nuclei. In fig. II. 5 e the

    various mammalian raphe nuclei and their main projections are shown. According to ten

    Donkelaar (1998: 1417) In reptiles, only two raphe nuclei can be distinguished: i. e. the

    rostral, mainly small-celled nucleus raphes superior and the large caudal nucleus raphesinferior containing medium-sized to very large cells. The caudal part of the inferior

    raphes nucleus may correspond to the raphes pallidus of mammals, the rostral part to the

    mammalian nucleus raphes magnus. In the reptilian nucleus raphes superior may be

    hidden a primordium of the mammalian nucleus raphes dorsalis. It seems that the

    mammalian raphes obscurus has no homologue in the reptilian brain. Following

    MacLeans model, in the serotonergic system we can distinguish a reptilian part (i.e. of

    the R-complex) and a mammalian part (i.e. of the Limbic System), the latter being formed

    in particular by raphes obscurus and dorsal raphes nuclei. Ascending fibers extend from

    the pontine nuclei to several telencephalic regions, among which are basal ganglia.

    So, the serotonergic systems evolution is an example of the evolution of the brain

    according to MacLeans model. In fact we may distinguish in it a reptilian part which is a

    regulator system inherent to the R-complex itself and to the spinal cord; and a limbic part

    which greatly increases control over the R-complex and moreover has connections with

    the Neocortex, in particular with prefrontal cortex which, according to MacLean (1978),

    helps us to see deeply into other peoples feelings. Given its numerous projections, the

    serotonergic system provides the most powerful system of integration and coordination

    among MacLeans three brain types identified till now.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    13/65

    III. Brain and Mind, Modules and Operators.

    According to evolutionary psychologists, the human mind is organized into modules

    ormental organs, each with a specialized design that makes it an expert in one arena of

    interaction with the world. The modules basic logic is specified by our genetic program.

    Their operation was shaped by natural selection to solve the problems of the hunting and

    gathering life led by our ancestors in most of our evolutionary history (Pinker 1997, p.

    21). We obviously agree with the main lines of evolutionary psychology, going over our

    hunter and gathering ancestors past: in the evolutionary landscape we have depicted we

    search the biological bases of god concepts in steps, first in Reptiles, ancient Mammals

    and finally Primates.

    Considering the global functioning of the Brain (i. e. the Mind), d'Aquili and

    Newberg (1999) have considered its primary functional components, which they have

    referred to as cognitive operators, which have specific functions that are localized in

    specific regions of the brain and perform activities that underlie the capacities of the

    mind. Newberg and dAquili (2001) have described eight cognitive operators: the holistic,

    the reductionist, the causal, the abstractive, the binary, the quantitative, the emotional, and

    the existential ones.As these authors noticed (2001, p. 187), the cognitive operator concept is clearly

    similar to the concept of cognitive modules in that both are functions and are localizable

    to one or more specific areas of the brain. However Cognitive operators differ from

    cognitive modules, because cognitive modules represent more specific functions that are

    localized to particular brain structures, whereas cognitive operators refer to more

    generalized functions of many areas of the brain: modules are concerned more the

    anatomy of the brain, operators more the physiology of the brain (or of the encephalon).

    However brain functions also produce behaviors and unconscious knowledge. Let us

    extend the operator concept from cognitive ones to encephalic ones. Encephalic

    operators may be defined as behavioral, emotive or cognitive operators that are specific

    functions performed by specific parts of the encephalon.

    Proceeding along the evolutionary scheme of the triune brain, we may define, in the

    R-complex, some behavioral operators, such as:

    1) the specific operator, that allows the acquisition of species identity by the

    imprinting (i. e. a fast form of learning that occurs only during a critical period in

    the development of the organism);

    2) the sexualoperator (for male or female distinct behaviors): its primary component

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    14/65

    may be located in the hypothalamic medial preoptic nucleus, which produces

    gonadotropine releasing factors, that has different size in male and female

    individuals, and that is crucially involved in the consummatory phase of

    masculine copulatory behavior and also plays a prominent role in feminine

    sexual behavior (Voogd et al. 1998, p. 1871); another behavioral important

    component of this operator lies in the nucleus accumbens.

    3) The territorialoperator: animals mark their territory with smells or other signals

    of boundaries, warning other (conspecific) animals not to intrude;

    4) the isopraxic (or mimetic) operator: it allows the animals to act in a like manner

    and it is implicated in conspecific recognition and in most forms of

    communication involved in self-preservation and in the procreation of the

    species (MacLean 1990, p. 144) and, in the opposite sense, it serves to promote

    species isolation; it is evident when conspecific animals behave in the same way

    at the same time (group or mass isopraxis);

    5) the hierarchic operator, whose primary component most likely lies in the

    pallidum, and that is fundamental for engaging in various types of display.

    R- complex may be also produce cognitive operators such as:

    6) the space operator, that allows an animal to know its territory;

    7) the time operator, that permits the chronology of events;

    8) the sequence operator: from the interaction of the former two operators, an animalcan know that after an event a specific one follows or may follow;

    9) the semiotic operator: it allows the right understanding, often species-specific, of

    behaviors and non verbal communications between individuals inside the same

    species (and to some extent, interaction with different species). It moreover

    permits a symbolic codification of some behavior (for example, in Primates, the

    penis display made by a male towards another male is not a sexual presentation,

    but a challenge display), and let the animal receiving the message to decode and

    correctly interpret the behavior it sees (e.g., the male to whom penis display is

    directed must understand it as a challenge display and not as a sexual

    presentation). Summarizing, the semiotic operator allows the correct coordination

    of behavior operators actions listed above.

    The main site of these cognitive operators seems to be the dorsal striatum (Parent and

    Hazrati 1995).

    The Limbic System may be considered the main site of several emotional operators, as

    the follows:

    10) thefearoperator. It allows graded behavioral outputs in regard to safe, unsafe and

    life threatening environments. Its main nervous center is the amygdale, which acts

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    15/65

    in connection with the cortex, the hypothalamus, the parasympathetic (mainly the

    vagus nerve) and the sympathetic systems. Porges (2001) has fully discussed the

    connections among these nervous and endocrine organs. When sensations

    received by sensory cortex are judged by the amygdale as caused by a safe

    environment, the amygdale stimulates vagus action both directly and by

    hypothalamic oxytocin, which is released to the sensory and motor portions of the

    vagal complex and systemically to visceral organs. Oxytocin fosters a calm or

    anti-stress state, and the sympathetic nervous system is not stimulated. When

    the amygdale judges the environment as unsafe, it stimulates the hypothalamus to

    release the hormone vasopressin: the vagus is inhibited, the sympathetic system

    stimulated, and the animal is ready for fight or flight behaviors.

    A life-threatening environment may elicit immobilization (i.e., feigning death or

    passive avoidance) and fight-flight behaviors are inhibited. According to Porges

    polyvagal theory, the neural regulation of the autonomic nervous system is the

    result of three evolutionary stages, each with a connected behavioral strategy. The

    first stage is characterized by immobilization behaviors due to the action of a

    primitive unmyelinated vagus that responds to threat by depressing metabolic

    activity (and this causes immobilization). The second stage is characterized by the

    sympathetic nervous system that is capable of increasing metabolic outputs and

    inhibiting the visceral vagus to foster mobilization behaviors necessary for fight or

    flight. These two stages of the fear operator characterize the reptilian limbicsystem. The third stage, unique to mammals, is characterized by a myelinated

    vagus that can rapidly regulate cardiac output to foster engagement and

    disengagement with the environment. As a rule, mammals are active in the second

    and third stage of this evolutionary operator, but in threat environment may

    regress to the first stage, and subsequent immobilization.

    11) The aggression operator: it seems that we may distinguish various aggression

    operators, as, at first, between predatory or interspecific aggression and the

    intraspecific one. The last is linked to the hierarchic operator of the R-complex

    and has its limbic component mainly in the amygdala.

    12) Also the mimic judgmentoperator has its focus in the amygdalar complex. It is

    very important for those mammals that are able to change their face expression, as

    mainly primates do. Face expressions are linked to the Autonomic Nervous

    System, as Porges (2001, p. 123) has remarked: The neomammalian vagus is

    neuroanatomically linked to the cranial nerves that regulate social engagement via

    facial expression and vocalization. A primordium of the mimic judgment

    operator also exists in the brain of vertebrates that are each other stimulated by

    visual signals. In reptiles and birds emotional or intentional (as aggressive, sexual

    and other) signals are not given by facial muscles movements, but by skin color

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    16/65

    changes, ruffling up of feathers, shows of cutaneous appendixes (i. e. skin capes

    or fans as in the genus Amphibolurus). Humans employ plumages, mantles,

    headpieces and other various outfits to show their rank or their well or ill

    disposition towards other humans: the human R-complex unconsciously and

    immediately decodes and interprets these signals.

    13) The falling in love operator allows pair bonds establishing. The

    neurobiological bases of this behavior have been studied only recently: Pair bonds

    are not very frequent in mammals, but, in the species in which they exist, seem to

    be linked to the activity of the rhinencepha-lon, and to the each other smell

    recognition of the individuals.

    It seems that in the human pair bond instauration there is the involvement both of

    the serotonergic (Marazziti et al. 1999) and dopamine (Aron et al. 2005) systems.

    The initial serotonin tone deficiency (observed by Marazziti et al.) may have the

    role of paving the way to the reward function of the subsequent dopaminergic

    action (observed by Aron et al.).

    14) The call operator activity is triggered when the pup feels to be neglected by its

    parents and in turn triggers in them:

    15) the nursing operator activity, that, as we have said above, depends on the

    oxytocin release. The call and nursing operators are not in action only, or towards,

    infancy: in some emotional circumstances they are active even and towards adults

    (all the more reason it happens in Homo sapiens that is a neothenic species, inwhich infancy features and behaviors persist or could reappear along the life).

    16) The attachment operator allows the establishment of a peculiar bond between

    pups and parents. It does not operate from birth in all the species, and this makes

    possible the adoption of a forsake (or an orphan) pup by other adults. Its action

    becomes evident when the pup is able to distinguish one another its neighbors,

    preferring some of them and fearing others.

    17) the play operator allows pups to learn their specific behaviors according to the

    various circumstances. So behaviors of Mammals are less fixed and stereotyped

    than behaviors of most Reptiles and Birds. (Ethologists have observed play-like

    behaviors in some birds). Acquired behaviors and knowledge are, as a rule,

    proportional to the playing age period, which varies among the species, and within

    a species, among individuals (in the human species, experimental scientists may

    be considered boys that are playing despite their adult feature).

    The Limbic System may be considered the primarily seat of a cognitive operator, the

    existential one.

    18) The existential (or ontological) operator assigns a sense of existence or non

    existence to the sensory information processed by the brain. It likely gives also the

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    17/65

    sense of existence to the thoughts elaborated by other parts of the encephalon,

    such as the R-complex or the Neocortex. In other terms, this operator gives a

    sense of reality to beliefs, regardless of whether they might be non contradictory

    or contradictory, or counterintuitive, according to Neocortical operators.

    Emotional and existential operators may be assumed as generally functioning in

    Mammals. In humans, the existential operator, that is cognitive but not necessarily

    rational, is linked to limbic emotional operators. MacLean wrote (1973 b, p. 123)

    concerning the feelings associated with knowledge of fundamental truths: It seems that

    the ancient limbic system provides the ingredients for the strong affective feeling or

    conviction that we attach to our beliefs, regardless of whether they are true or false!

    The Neocortex is the main site of most cognitive operators.

    The following list summarizes data from dAquili and Newberg (1999) and Newberg et

    al. (2001) for human mind operators, from the holistic to the quantitative one. To these

    we add the linguistic, the insight, and the metacognition operators.

    19) the holistic operator enables the organism to view reality as a whole (gestalt). It

    likely rises from the activity of the parietal lobe in the non dominant hemisphere

    (i. e., in most humans, the right one);

    20) the reductionist operator allows the mind to see the whole broken down into its

    component parts; it functions in the opposite manner to that of the holistic

    operator, and it resides primarily in the dominant parietal lobe (i. e., in mosthumans, the left one), that so is involved in reductionist and analytical processes,

    while the non dominant parietal lobe is involved in holistic and synthetic ones.

    The corpus callosum allows connection between the two parietal lobes, permitting

    the mind to combine the holistic and the reductionistic approaches.

    21) The causaloperator enables the mind to interpret all of reality as a sequence of

    specific causes and effects. It is believed to result from the connections between

    the left frontal lobe and the left orientation association area; it spontaneously and

    actively tends to impart a sense of causality on all the events. This function of the

    mind is genetically hardwired in all humans, and may be called as the causal

    imperative, or, according to Levy-Bruhl, the need of explanation.

    22) The abstractive operator permits the organism to form general concepts. These

    abstract concepts are constructed from the perception of individual facts and

    various individual objects, derived from inductive functions. These operators

    likely reside in the inferior portion of the parietal lobe. V. Ramachandran has

    shown experimental evidence that the angular gyrus, belonging to the inferior

    parietal lobule, plays a major role in metaphoric understanding.

    23) The binary operator organizes reality by reducing the most complicated

    relationships to simple pairs of opposites. According to d'Aquili and Newberg it

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    18/65

    resides in the inferior parietal lobe on the dominant side. However, as its activity

    appears as a stereotyped one, this operator may have its unconscious origin in the

    R-complex, likely in the caudate nucleus, which has numerous links with

    neocortical association areas, so it may become conscious in the parietal lobe.

    24) The quantitative operator permits the abstraction of quantity from the perception

    of various elements, and allows mathematical operations but also the estimation of

    time and distance, the awareness of amounts of things as food or enemies

    approaching, and the need to order objects or sequences of events by some

    numerical system. It likely resides in the general area of the inferior parietal lobe.

    25) The linguistic operator has two neocortical components: the Brocas area in the

    frontal lobe and the Wernickes area in the superior temporal gyrus, near the

    inferior parietal lobule. The former area permits to speak correctly, the latter

    allows understanding of the meanings of words or sentences that have been

    heard. We may note that this area is contiguous with abstractive (and metaphoric)

    area. The linguistic operator may be considered the cortical correspondent and

    extension of the reptilian semiotic operator.

    26) The insightoperator let us to succeed in problem solving in a sudden way, after

    and by an unconscious elaboration and not by attempts and errors: the solution

    reach the consciousness suddenly, often while the subject is thinking or doing

    other matters. Remembering the exclamation shouted by Archimedes when the

    solution of a problem suddenly rose to his consciousness while he was in the bath,this operator may be termed eureka (I found!) operator.

    27) The metacognition (orempathy) operator permits the awareness of ones own

    mental states and relates behaviors to social or environmental circumstances; it

    allows the development of a theory of mind, i. e. an awareness of other peoples

    minds content. It likely resides in frontal lobes, mostly in the right hemisphere

    (Stuss et al. 2001), and mirror neurons (Gallese 2005) may contribute to this

    operator. Theory of mind, metarepresentation, meta-cognition, mind

    reading and mental state attribution are terms that refer to the awareness of

    ones own mental states, beliefs, attitude and experiences, the relationship

    between these and external events, and also of the mental states of others and the

    implications for their motives and intentions, as summarized by Stuss et al.

    (2001, p. 279). They note that only humans and a few species of great apes are

    capable of attributing mental states to others, making us, as Kirkpatrick noted, all

    amateur psychologists (2005, p. 274).

    We may assume that some of these cognitive operators are functioning in Mammals

    based on both to neocortical development and to complexity of connections among

    cortical centers. The holistic, reductionist, abstractive, quantitative, insight and ethical

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    19/65

    operators may be assumed working in a variable way in the different Orders of

    Mammalia, presumably more in Proboscidea (elephants), Cetacea (dolphins) and

    Primates (apes). The binary operator seems to be typically human, as well as the

    quantitative operator and its connections with other operators (as the linguistic and

    abstractive ones) to allow symbolic mathematics rising (but see Hauser et al. 2002 with

    regard to animal cognitive abilities and quantitative and linguistic functions).

    The causal operator, as it is endowed with an high abstractive ability, is produced

    thanks to human neocortical complexity: on the grounds of our knowledge, it seems to

    result from the inclusion and development of a mammalian neocortical operator (which

    has more or less abilities according to the various species), and that may be termed as

    agency detection operator: it attributes an intentionality as the concrete causes of

    phenomena.

    A first description of an animal behavior that we may refer to this operator has been

    reported by Darwin (1871, p. 67): my dog was lying on the lawn during a hot and still

    day; but at a little distance a slight breeze occasionally moved an open parasol As it

    was, every time the dog growled fiercely and barked. He must, I think, have reasoned

    to himself in a rapid and unconscious manner that movement without any apparent cause

    indicate the presence of some strange living agent, and no stranger had a right to be in his

    territory.

    Agency operator evolved in Mammals, by means of natural selection, not only for

    intruder detection, but also for predator detection: Barrett (2000) has termed this brainfunction as agent detection device. As exposed by Boyer for humans (2001, p. 145), Our

    evolutionary heritage is that of organisms that must deal with both predators and prey. In

    either situation, it is far more advantageous to overdetect agency than to underdetect it.

    The expense of false positive (seeing agents where there are none) is minimal, if we can

    abandon these misguided intuitions quickly. In contrast, the cost of not detecting agents

    when they are actually around (either predator or prey) could be very high. Thanks to

    this over-developed neocortex,Homo sapiens can be said to possess a developed agency

    detection operator, termed by J. Barrett (2000) as Hyperactive Agent-Detection Device

    (HADD), extended also in a more abstract form, in the causal operator.

    In Reptiles and Birds, R-complex has the ability of recording temporal sequences of

    events. Such sequences in order of time do the function of sequences in order of cause:

    this leads to routine activities, in conformity with precedent situations, ritualistic

    behaviors or associations between events and behaviors that may involve both Central

    and Peripheral Nervous Systems. Pavlov's conditioning experiments have shown that an

    animal may unconsciously and automatically put in connection stimuli and answers that

    normally have no link.

    Natural selection has favored survival and reproduction of those animals that,

    provided with a rudimentary forebrain, succeeded in linking and recording events that

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    20/65

    happened in a temporal sequence. Individual provided with such an ability had more

    fitness because they could anticipate favorable events, repeat positive and avoid negative

    experiences. Indeed, if it is true that not all the events that happen in a temporal order are

    also linked by a casual order, it also true that all the events that have a causal connection

    have also a temporal sequence: temporal relations are redundant in respect to causal ones,

    but for animal surviving it is better overloading than lacking. This pseudo causal

    association made by the R-complex is the fundamental, and almost the only, link among

    events Reptiles and Birds may think. It is basic also for Mammals, but they can

    elaborate sensorial or recorded data by means of the Neocortex. A mammal, in

    comparison with a reptile, more easily forgets a connection if it does not happen

    afterwards: this ability is linked to brain complexity.

    In Mammals, the R-complex is connected with the Neocortex in a more complicated

    way than in Reptiles and Birds. In Primates, as shown by Parent et al. (1995, p. 135),

    inputs from the frontal lobe (motor cortex) terminate mostly in the lateral putamen to

    constitute the sensorimotor striatal region (SMSR in fig. III. 1). Inputs from the

    hippocampal and parahippocampal gyri, the cingulated cortex and the amygdala,

    terminate in the nucleus accumbens and part of the olfactory tubercle to constitute the

    limbic striatal region (LSR). Inputs from the prefrontal cortex as well as from associative

    areas of the parietal and temporal lobes terminate in the head of the caudate nucleus and

    the rostral part of the putamen to constitute the associative striatal region (ASR). So in the

    mammalian R-complex we may locate at least four components: 1) a behavioral one,made up by globus pallidus and nucleus accumbens, that is the main seat of behavioral

    operators as the specific, sexual, territorial, isopraxic, and hierarchic ones; 2) a senso-

    motor component (which shows its weight in the Parkinsons disease); 3) a limbic

    component and, 4), an associative one, as above described.

    In Mammals the R-complex maintains the ability of fixing pseudo causal connections

    among phenomena (post hoc, propter hoc). In the associative component of the mammal

    R-complex may occur both the link among real events and the association, mostly

    unconsciously, of ideas, memories or emotions originated in other brain areas such as the

    Limbic System or the Neocortex. Particularly in humans, the associative striatum may

    link not only real events, but also events imagined by Neocortex, Moreover, the pseudo

    causal sequence may be reversed: not only from the cause to the effect, but given an

    event then connect it with a cause. These free associations, unconscious and

    reasonless, made by humans in ways that are structural similarly, but whose contents are

    various in each being, had been discovered by psychoanalysis long time before their

    cerebral origin had been located.

    The thought processed by the reverberating circuit Neocortex R-complex

    Neocortex etc., is called magical thought and it may be expressed by language.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    21/65

    Magical thoughts, as that events which have been observed to occur simultaneously or to

    follow a particular sequence will continue to follow the same pattern or that like

    produces like (Vernon 1962, p. 65), may lead the development of ritualistic actions, also

    up to obsessive and compulsive behaviors.

    The Limbic System, particularly its serotonergic component, aims to inhibit the magic

    and ritualized activity the associative striatum tends spontaneously to generate. But the

    Limbic System, if it fails in doing so, then adds an emotive component to ritualistic or

    compulsive behaviors, producing a released condition when they are completed, and an

    anxious condition when they are brusquely interrupted or changed.

    The Neocortex, as we have seen, has induction and causal operators, so that from

    the particular observations that different phenomena are effects of certain causes, it infers

    that every phenomenon is the effect of a cause. The abstractive operator lies in the

    inferior portion of the parietal lobe, and the causal operator results from the connection of

    the left association area /in the parietal lobe) with the frontal lobe: the parietal part of the

    causal operator is close to the abstractive operator, and it seems that, for its activity, the

    causal operator utilizes previous or almost contemporary investigations done by the

    abstractive operator.

    Therefore, the Neocortex, by means of its cognitive operators, allows humans some

    rational way of interpreting, or explaining, the reality: it is able to do this more or less

    well, according to its knowledge.

    Summarizing, the link among phenomena elaborated by the whole brain may be :

    a) a rational and verifiable one, if the Neocortex can find the cause-effect nexus;

    b)a magic one, that is not verifiable, that is characterized by more or less high levels

    of fancy, and that is held by faith. Magical thought has its unconscious origin in the

    R-complex, and is made conscious by the Neocortex, which tries to give it an

    appearance of rationality.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    22/65

    IV. Dominance-Submission Behavior in Religion, and the Principles of

    Uniformatism and of Correlation.

    In 1821 F. Schleiermacher wrote that religion is the feeling of sheer dependence on

    God (quoted by Burkert, 1996, p. 80), who afterwards adds: religion is generally

    accepted as a system of rank, implying dependence, subordination and submission to

    unseen superiors. The awareness of rank and dependence in religion is particularly clear

    in all the ancient religions. God means power, rule, and honors due. (p. 81). As noted by

    Morris (1967; 1994 p. 121) ... in a behavioral sense, religious activities consist of the

    coming together of large groups of people to perform repeated and prolonged submissive

    displays to appease a dominant individual. The dominant individual concerned takes

    many forms in different cultures, but always has the common factor of immense power.

    This notion of more powerful beings who determine the fate of humans is a widespread

    tendency, if not the nucleus of religion.

    Utilizing data from sciences as different as Neurobiology, Paleontology, and

    Compared Ethology, we apply the Principle of Uniformitarianism, that, in its broader

    form, affirms natural laws we observe in the present world are the same that operated in

    the past and the same will operate in the future. In general terms, natural laws are

    invariable both in space and in time. We seek to therefore understand the source of our

    Supreme Power Being in the evolutionary pre-history of dominance and submission

    relations in the animal kingdom.

    The Principle of Correlation proceeds from the principle of uniformitarianism: if

    particular phenomena, or peculiar features, of living beings regards their environment, or

    regards themselves, are in correlation in the present world, we may infer that they were in

    correlation also in the past. It implies that the knowledge of some documented by fossils

    features allows us to infer the presence, in those beings of the past, of other non fossil, but

    with the previous ones correlated, features.

    However, the correlation principle must be used with caution, as two phenomena or

    two features may show themselves together by chance. So we can correctly utilize the

    correlation principle either when we know the causal relation which determines the

    correlation, or when the statistical analysis allows us to exclude a more chance

    correlation.

    The hypothesis that religious behavior, religious thought and the origin of the

    Powerful Being idea are natural may be substantiated by evidence of the roots of such

    behavior among Hominids and non-human Primates. For confirmation of the validity of

    this hypothesis we find the existence of a dominant chief figure, potential or in action,

    to whom others show submission behavior.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    23/65

    V. Dominance-Submission Behavior in Primates

    In Primates strong links have been established between sexual organ or behaviors and

    territoriality and dominance-submission behaviors, so that male genitals and sexual

    behavior signify dominance on a territory or on a social group, while female genitals and

    behavior become symbols of subordination. Wickler (1967) has described the so called

    sentinel behaviorin baboons: one, or more, male dominant individuals sit at lookout sites

    thighs spread and a display of partial erection. This display is regarded as an optical

    marker of boundaries, warning other monkeys not to intrude (fig. V. 1 a). The male

    squirrel monkey, seeing another male or its own image on a mirror, flexes the head to

    one side, retracts the corners of the mouth while making high-pitched, peeping

    vocalizations, spread one or both thighs, and makes thrusting movements with the fully

    erect phallus (MacLean 1990, p. 171) (fig. V. 1 b).

    The rostral two-thirds of the medial segment of the globus pallidus seems to be the

    main site involved in mirror display and therefore in aggressive intraspecific behavior, or,

    in other words, this part of the pallidum seems to be the main site of the hierarchic

    operator.

    Such valences of sexual behavior and genitals are also evident in Homo sapiens.MacLean (1990, p. 233) refers that Gajdusek (1970) has called attention to the parallel

    between the display behavior of squirrel monkeys and certain rituals of Melanesian

    tribes, or that some New Guinea groups when frightened, excited or surprised meet the

    event by a penile display dance. This dance is similarly used to express both dominance

    and aggression. At the present time the rugby team All Blacks displays the Maori haka

    dance before any game. In many cultures in the world, house guards (stone or wooden

    monuments showing an erect phallus) have been used to mark territorial boundaries (fig.

    V. 1 c). In ancient Athens stylized statues of Hermes, endowed with an erect phallus,

    were posed at street corners. (Fig. V. 1 d). The erect phallus, symbolized by some means

    as, for example, the middle finger, is an human challenge display utilized by male or

    female individuals (fig. V. 1 f).

    For more than two centuries, Occidental men have found a discrete form of phallic

    display: the neck-tie. Perhaps it is superfluous as a masculine signal, since bearded men

    more easily give up tie in habitual dressing.

    Returning to Primates, the mounting threat is a form of rank demonstration, and it

    can be performed both by a male towards another male (fig. V. 2 b) and by a female

    towards a lower rank female (fig V. 2 c). Female sexual presentation can be used by low

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    24/65

    rank individuals in order to acquire group chiefs support (fig V. 2 d). Primate female

    sexual presentation may be observed in some human profane (fig. V. 2 e) or religious (fig.

    V. 2 f) submissive gestures. Sometimes female sexual presentations are utilized as

    challenge displays: doing so, the displayer means that he/she considers his/her opponent

    as impotent.

    VI Primate social behaviors and sexual dimorphism.

    Primate societies may be grouped according to breeding systems as follows:

    a) Monogamous breeding systems (Mo);

    b) Single male breeding systems (SM);

    c) Multi-male breeding systems (MM).

    Several scholars (as Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1977; Leutenegger and Kelly 1977;

    Harvey et al. 1978; Pickford and Chiarelli 1986; Hinde 1987) have attempted to correlate

    these systems with male/female sexual dimorphism in body size and in canine tooth size.

    These studies have shown that, at first view, monogamous species show a very low

    dimorphism, while single-male and multi-male species show a variable degree: as Buss

    (1999, p. 286) has summarized these studies the greater the effective polygyny the more

    pronounced the sexual dimorphism. However, results from in-dept studies have shownless linearity: after a long series of studies (as Plavcan et al 1992, 1997), Plavcan (2001)

    has made clear that sexual dimorphism is more related to male-male competition levels

    than to breeding systems.

    Plavcan and van Schaik (1992) classified primate species into four competition

    levels, labeled from 1 to 4, on the basis of the intensity and potential frequency of

    male-male competition.

    Competition level 1 contains low-intensity, low-frequency species.

    Competition level 2 contains low-intensity, high-frequency species.

    Competition level 3 contains high-intensity, low-frequency species.

    Competition level 4 contains high-intensity, high-frequency species.

    Low-frequency competition is typical of breeding groups that contain only a single,

    adult male: so monogamous species belong to level 1, while single-male species belong to

    level 3. But species in which single male breeding groups come together to form large

    groups daily, such as Papio hamadryas, Theropithecus gelada, and Nasalis larvatus,

    belong to level 4 because they are high frequency species. Multi-male species mainly

    belong to competition levels 2 and 4.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    25/65

    Primates breeding and social systems, some representative species and their

    competition levels, as estimated by Plavcan and van Schaik (1997, p. 347-348), are shown

    in table VI. 1.

    VII. Dominance-Submission Behavior in Hominin Phylogeny.

    Homo sapiens is an African ape, belonging to the order ofPrimates, infraorder

    Catarrhinae, that is now formed by two superfamiliae, Cercopithecoidea and

    Hominoidea. Evolution ofCatarrhinae occurred in Africa and Eurasia. The first family of

    catarrhins was Propliothecidae, whose main genera were Propliopithecus and

    Aegyptopithecus. This latter one, a cat size monkey, tail having and showing an evident

    canine size sexual dimorphism, is considered as the ancestor of both Cercopithecoidea

    andHominoidea, the first ones having a tail and quadrupedal walk, the second ones tail

    absence and the possibility of braching gait.Proconsul,Dryopithecus, Oreopithecus, and

    Ouranopithecus are the main genera of Miocene fossil Hominoidea. The 8 Myr ago

    common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees was chimplike forest dwelling and

    predominantly arboreal and fruit-eating (Wood & Brooks 1999, p. 219).

    The tectonic fracture series forming the African Rift Valley system caused, about 8-6

    Million years ago, a geographic barrier that divided Hominoids apes in two distinct

    populations, and the differentiation of the eastern environment respect the western one.This latter remained, and still does, an equatorial forest, while in the east side, owing to a

    growing dry environment, as a result of disturbances in atmospheric circulations caused

    by the Rift Valley, disperse woods succeeded in the first millennia, and then savannah

    developed.

    The eastern (future Hominini) and the western (future Panini) populations of

    Hominids were so each other isolated and became subject to increasingly different

    selective pressure, as Hominins faced at first disperse woods and subsequently the

    savanna, while Panins remained in equatorial forest, where they differed into two species,

    Pan throglodytes andP. paniscus.

    The most ancientHominini so far discovered, and dated back to 5.8 5.2 Mya, have

    been assigned to the species Ardipithecus kadabba (Haile-Selassie et al. 2004). Fossils

    happen chimplike and the environment in which they lived was still wood. The most

    likely competition level attributable to this hominin species is the second: A. kadabba

    most probably lived in multi male social and breeding groups, as presentPan species do.

    In subsequent Hominini, such as Ardipithecus ramidus (4.5 Mya),Australopithecus

    anamensis (4.2 4 Mya), and Australopithecus afarensis (4 2.5 Mya), we may note

    improvements in bipedalism and increase in sexual dimorphism.A. afarensis lived mostly

    in savanna.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    26/65

    Primate models of ancient hominin behaviors.

    Reconstructing hominin evolution and social behavior, some scholars (as De Waal

    2000), privileging genetic proximity, have chosen chimps as a species model; others,

    giving more consideration to environmental influences, have chosen baboon species. Both

    of these approaches have their strengths, but neither alone is a complete model for

    Hominins, who must be studied as Primates that, genetically and morphologically chimp-

    like, underwent an environmental selective pressure that might give them behavioral

    baboon-like traits, as we know that similar ecological pressures produce similar

    adaptations even in genetically different animals (convergent evolution) (e.g., see C. Jolly

    2001).

    As summarized by McFarland (1985, pp. 159-60), comparative studies have shown

    that in harsh environments baboon troops are dominated by a single male, whereas in rich

    environments the troops are multi-male.Papio hamadryas and Theropithecus gelada are

    typically SM, whilePapio cynocephalus lives in rich environments and has typical multi

    male troops. Papio anubis and P. ursinus (Chackma baboons) live in intermediate

    environments. They have multi-male troops and dominant males have privileged access to

    females. Often some dominant males form a central hierarchy and cooperate in

    maintaining their social position against rivals, in protecting mothers and infants and in

    defense against predators. Male anubis baboons have a smaller but evident cape, and

    show a marked sexual dimorphism in body and canine teeth sizes. Each male hamadryasbaboon that possess an harem of females does not try to mate with a female of another

    harem, while male anubis baboons try. From this and other behavioral differences, it

    follows that hamadryas males exhibit affiliative and mutually supportive behaviors in a

    variety of social contexts in which anubis males tend to be indifferent or hostile to each

    other (Kaplan et al. 1999, p. 518). The neurobiological basis of these different behaviors

    mostly lie in the serotonergic systems activity, that is lower in anubis than in hamadryas

    males, as shown by observations carried out in anubis and anubis-hamadryas hybrid

    males (Kaplan et al. 1999). It follows that, within the fourth level, hamadryas baboons are

    less competitive than anubis baboons.

    As stated by Plavcan and van Schalk, increasing sexual dimorphism in both canine

    tooth size and body weight is strongly correlated with increasing competition level

    (1997, p. 353). Such correlation allows us to assess competition level from sexual

    dimorphism, and vice-versa: we may control this statement in extant species and infer it

    for fossil species.

    As it lived in harsh environment, we may reasonable suppose that A. afarensis,

    which showed a marked body size sexual dimorphism, underwent a selective pressure that

    gave baboon-like its behavior and social organization. According to the correlation

    principle, we may chosePapio hamadryas orTheropithecus gelada as species that better

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    27/65

    may give us a model ofA. afarensis male behavior (high male male competition) and

    social/breeding organization (SM breeding groups that daily come together to form large

    groups). So the most likely competition level attributable to A. afarensis, and to the

    precedent speciesA. anamensis, which appears to have been very dimorphic too (Ward et

    a. 2001), is the fourth one. As noticed by Flinn et al. (2005, p. 26), Australopithecus

    body mass dimorphism suggests that these early hominins were polygynous, as

    significant mass dimorphism is not associated with monogamy in any extant primate

    (Plavcan 2001). So, we may likely suppose thatA. afarensis had an SM breeding system

    and an MM social system.

    Sexual selection in hamadryas and gelada baboons favors males with large hair

    capes: in harsh environment, males that seem to be bigger are more fit than males actually

    larger (that are more expensive in nutrition and easilier victims for predators).

    According to an hypothesis that has had Lovejoy among its first author (1980), it

    might have been that, in a hominin SM breeding system group (likely belonging to A.

    afarensis), it were born by chance a female having a concealed ovulation (i. e. without

    estrus or sexual swellings and perhaps with a little masculine appearance, having a little

    hair cape). In this case, the dominant male might have no close watch on her, and she

    could more easily mate with a subordinate, and so less dimorphic, male. From such mates

    it could be born both sons having little dimorphism and daughters having concealed

    ovulation and probably a continuous sexual receptiveness.

    These modifications very probably gave origin to a new species: Australopithecusgarhi is the fossil hominin most likely depict it. Male individuals of such a new species,

    being less dimorphic and less competitive each other, could easily co-operate in defense

    against predators and for food gaining. Moreover, ifA. afarensis males behaved asPapio

    hamadryas males do at present, they might exhibit supportive behaviors in various social

    contexts: this condition might be a pre-adaptive feature for more supportive behaviors in

    A. garhi. It seems thatA. garhi was gatherer and scavenger, rather than hunter, because its

    body was small and its legs were short.

    Fossils of the subsequent genus Homo are characterized by low body size

    dimorphism (competition levels 1 or 2), and absence of canine tooth size dimorphism,

    which may imply breeding and social systems unique among primates (Ruff 2002).

    Concealed ovulation and female continuous sexual receptivity formed two behavioral

    selective factors because of males that insisted in mating with the same female gained

    more reproductive success compared with males that tried to mate with all the females,

    risking impregnating no one. Such a situation lead to form some adaptations supporting

    long-term mating relationships and pair bonds, towards the success of monogamy in a

    primate species in which, owing to its basic genetic inheritance, both males and females

    were polygamous. Because of this complex interactions between towards monogamy

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    28/65

    adaptations and basic polygamous inheritance, we have chosen to term pseudo-Mo

    the human breeding system from early Homo to hunter-gatherer Homo sapiens.

    Subsequently, depending on environmental and cultural circumstances,Homo sapiens has

    produced various kind of marital systems: in some there has been a reappearance, at least

    in part, of an SM breeding system, with lawful or tolerated polygyny.

    As regards environmental adaptations, according to Bramble and Lieberman (2004,

    p. 345), endurance running, defined as running many kilometers over extended time

    periods using aerobic metabolism ... is unique to humans among primates. Fossil data

    suggest that this capability evolved in Homo habilis/rudolfensis for scavenging, and

    subsequently in Homo ergaster/erectus for hunting. Endurance running, as remarked by

    Bramble and Lieberman (2004, p.351), may have made possible a diet rich in fats and

    proteins thought to account for the unique human combination of large bodies, small guts,

    big brains and small teeth (see also Fialkowski 1986, 1987; Aiello & Wheleer 1995). Fig

    VII. 1 shows fossil species, which are thought to be inside the human phylogenetic tree,

    disposed according to their estimates of male-male competition levels. Scheme VII. 1

    correlates the human evolution with breeding systems.

    As regards social organization, combining data from body dimorphism, ecological

    environment and stone tools, there is a general consensus that humans lived as hunter -

    gatherers until a few thousands of years ago (that is, until the rising of agriculture, insome regions before, in other after, in other never), and that these social groups were

    egalitarian as regards the distribution of resources and reproduction, that is they had MM

    social system and pseudo-Mo breeding system.

    VIII. The Immense Power Being as a projection of primates group chief:

    Freuds and Morris hypotheses and criticism.

    Since religious displays are characterized by submission to a dominant individual,

    they may be the expression of an SM social system which has its neurological

    background in the hierarchy operator of the R-complex, as described in III. On the

    other hand, according to evolutionary psychology principles, a dominant individual may

    not be conceptualized by a brain which is not prepared to conceive individual relations as

    hierarchic relations. Therefore, the Immense Power Being concept must have been the

    result of the projection of the image of a groups dominant individual in a superhuman

    world: an example of such a model concerning the origin of religion is found in Freuds

    Totem und Taboo (1912/13).

    In this work, which aims to explain the origins of totemism and exogamy, rather than

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    29/65

    religion, Freud utilized the concept of the primitive horde hypothesized by Darwin in

    The Descent of Man (1871), and the concept of the sacramental sacrifice of the totemic

    animal described by W. R. Smith in Lectures on the Religion of Semites (1889).

    According to Freud, in the primitive horde the father forbade his sons to mate with their

    sisters, that he kept to himself: one day, the excluded brothers came together, and killed

    and devoured their father, putting an end to the paternal horde. Eating him, they fulfilled a

    sort of identification with him, and each of them took possession of a part of his power.

    But afterward, as Freud hypothesized inferring by means of psychoanalysis, they felt

    remorse for their action, and, as dead, the father became more powerful than he was as

    live: his sons, owing to the posthumous obedience, inferred from psychoanalysis,

    prohibited the killing of fathers substitute, the totem, and mating with their sisters (from

    which the origin of exogamy).

    In order to explain the persistence of totemism and exogamy in subsequent human

    societies, Freud assumed memories, that are acquired and individual characters, as

    genetically hereditary characters, while geneticists were showing acquired characters

    were not. For that reason, now we may consider Freuds tale only as a myth regarding the

    passage from SMhominin societies topseudo-Mo ones.

    In The Naked Ape (1967; 1994, p. 121-122), D. Morris has proposed a sort of

    biological updating of the Freudian hypothesis. He has wrote that before human ancestors

    evolved into cooperative hunters they lived in social groups dominated by a single male.But with the growth of the cooperative spirit so vital for successful group hunting, the

    application of the dominant individuals authority had to be severely limited if he was to

    retain the active, as opposed to passive, loyalty of the other group members. They had to

    want to help him instead of simply fear him. He had to become more one of them. The

    old-style monkey tyrant had to go, and in his place there arose a more tolerant, more

    cooperative naked ape leader. This change nevertheless left a gap. From our ancient

    background there remained a need for an all-powerful figure who could keep the group

    under control, and the vacancy was filled by the invention of a god. The influence of the

    invented god-figure could then operate as a force additional to the now more restricted

    influence of the group leader.

    We are in agreement with Morris about the basic point that the all-powerful being

    is a projection of the ancient old-style monkey tyrant, but Morris fast reconstruction

    presents some points that need explanations, so that it must be regarded as a further

    research stimulating model rather than a complete theory. Let us discuss two of this

    points:

    1) Morris does not state precisely the period of the human evolution in which the

    old-style tyrant was eliminated and the god-figure was invented: in his reconstruction

    these two events are regarded as or almost contemporary. But:

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    30/65

    a) we may reasonably assert that the ability of thinking abstract, superhuman and

    invisible beings was achieved only by hominins endowed with a large brain, presumably

    afterHomo sapiens speciation.

    b) We know, from the reconstruction of hominin social behavior based on fossil data

    dimorphism, and above reported, that the old-style tyrant was replaced by more

    cooperative leaders about two million of years ago.

    Therefore, if Morris postulated invention of god happened afterHomo sapiens

    speciation, his reconstruction does not hold in due consideration the very long time

    passed from the old-style tyrant cessation; vice versa, if it happened immediately after

    this cessation, it cannot be considered reasonable because the brains of two millions years

    ago hominins could not think superhuman and invisible beings.

    2) A gap is not a stimulus, but the absence of a stimulus. It is necessary a stimulus

    that, as it is inexplicable, suggests a gap of power that must be filled.

    Returning to our reconstruction, from the evolution of social behavior we have

    depicted, it logically descents that, in groups of lacking dimorphism Hominini (especially

    from earlyHomo), there were no individual who, as dominant male in SM social groups,

    might act as a concrete example of an Immense Power Being. In other words we can

    reject the possibility that the divine being idea might have been originated by a projection

    of a dominant male actually living in those groups.

    How did it happen, therefore, that among human beings with a documented low

    male-male competition social system we can find a projection in the super-human world

    of a being with immense power, that should have to be associated with a high male-male

    competition social system, supported by a strong hierarchy operators activity?

    Although inhibited by the Limbic System, the human brain R-complex preserved

    (and still preserves) structures and hierarchy forming functions which give rise to

    conceive powerful leaders. The neocortical elaboration of the concept of an Immense

    Powerful Being can only be created, according to the triune brain model, after a proposal

    of the R-complex to the Neocortex. Consequently we can deduce that at some time in the

    course of human evolution such a nervous structure was set free by the inhibitory action

    of the Limbic System, presumably as a consequence of a strong external stimulus which,

    causing a psychic trauma, weakened the inhibitory action of the Limbic System.

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    31/65

    IX. Towards a Neurobiological and Evolutionary Psychology Explanation:

    a) The Traumatic and Triggering Stimulus.

    We could presume that this traumatic stimulus consisted in the acquisition of the

    awareness of own mortality. Indeed, as the human brain, presumably in Homo sapiens,

    achieved the complexity that allowed it to think over it happened in surrounding

    environment, human beings realized that whereas animals died because another animal

    killed them, human beings could die even if no animal killed them: For example, at a

    certain point of his life, with no apparent cause, an human being felt ill, also pain, and

    died. This event, in various ways, occurred always: Every human being had not a

    predators (or accidents) victim, invariably died anyway.

    The abstractive operator, from every single death event, inferred that every human

    being had to die, i. e. it allowed human beings to be aware of their own mortality.

    Thanks to the linguistic operator, this awareness of death became collective: Even

    human beings who had not arrived alone at the concept of the universality of death came

    to know of it by other humans.

    We can presume this awareness was very shocking.

    Most scholars agree that human are, and always were, troubled by their own

    mortality, and some of them had explained many cultural human features as derived from

    this awareness (Terror Management Theory: See, for example, Greenberg et al. 1997, or

    Salzman 2001). Moreover, as Hinde has noticed (1999, p. 59), among present-dayhumans, even if most people profess to accept the prospect of death, indirect methods

    indicate that unconscious fear is widespread (Beit- Hallahmi and Argyle 1997; Hood et al.

    1996).

    Ethnological research of the past two centuries has shown that preliterate people do

    not think death as a natural datum: On the contrary, all the narratives on the origin and

    cause of death have shown in evidence, without exceptions, the unnaturalness of death

    (Widengren 1969).

    So we may infer thatHomo sapiens is the only animal who knows he is mortal, and

    that he does not immediately recognize this fact as a natural datum: he may think so by

    means of a rational effort.Homo sapiens, as historical and ethnographic data show, rather

    and immediately feels that death is a violence he has to suffer: all the more reason it could

    have been considered so by the first Homo sapiens who were conscious of death. We

    cannot know when this awareness took place.

    McBrearty & Brooks, in their very expansive review of the origin of modern human

    behavior (2000), refer (p. 519) that earliest evidence for burial among H. sapiens is

    found in the Levant at the site of Qafzeh, that these are dated to ca. 120, - 90,000 years

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    32/65

    ago and that at least one may be associated with grave goods.

    b) A scheme of the Triggered Psychic/Neurobiological Mechanism.

    On the basis of our neurobiological knowledge, we can try to reconstruct the psychic

    mechanism triggered by first death awareness in ancient humans, roughly locating the

    involved brain structures using the triune model, as follows and as it is depicted in fig. IX.

    1.

    1) Homo sapiens perceived death.

    2) The Neocortex realized the awareness that all human beings had to die, and in the

    same time death was felt as a violence.

    3) Neocortical causal operator, as it spontaneously tends to impart a sense of

    causality on all the events (the causal imperative orneed of explanation), tried to

    find the cause of the mortal violence exerted over humans. But it obviously failed

    in finding an empirical cause.

    4) The Limbic System received the awareness of death and the anxiety derived from

    the lack of any empirical explanation, and, as it was shocked by them, turned

    them into a distressing stimulus affecting the Neocortex and R-complex; at the

    same time its inhibitory action on the R-complex decreased.

    5) According to R-complex way of thinking (due to its behavioral operators),violence must be performed by a dominant individual. To a similar conclusion

    ancient Homo sapiens R-complex had unconsciously to come owing to its

    internal neuronal circuits linking cognitive operators of the caudate nucleus (the

    thinking part of the R-complex and main site of the pseudo-causal operator)

    with behavioral operators as the hierarchic one lying in the pallidum.

    6) The result of such a reptilian elaboration was projected to Neocortex, where it

    became conscious. The Neocortical causal operator, as no other active and

    empirical cause he might find, had to recognize in the reptilian concept of a

    dominant individual the agent of the mortal violence inflicted on humanity. Such

    a recognition was not difficult to causal operator, because, as we have said above

    ( III), it includes an agent-detection device. Therefore:

    7) The Neocortex formulated the Immense Powerful Being concept.

    8) However it was still an indefinite concept. The neocortical association areas, in

    relation to the ecological, cultural and social environments (9 a), and with more

    or less relevant contributions by the caudate nucleus (thank to the reverberating

    circuit Neocortex Associative Striatal Regions Neocortex etc., as we have

    described in III), developed various systems of myths (9 b) in order to explain

    mankind-god relations (first the reason of mortal violence inflicted to humans)

  • 7/31/2019 ARC 25 Theopoiesis for ARC

    33/65

    and systems of rites allowing the establishment of some kind of communication

    between humans and the Powerful Being(s). Inside the neocortical association

    areas that tried to explain the reason of mortal violence inflicted by the Immense

    Powerful Being to the humans, an important role might be performed by the

    metacognition or theory of mind operator.

    Finally, but it is fundamental, it must be emphasized that every system of belief

    may be felt as true only if the Limbic Systems existential operator (9c) gives it a

    sense of reality. In other words, in the presence of equivalent fantastic tales

    (regards neocortical judgment) the existential operator chooses which are fancy

    products and which are sacred, or revealed (and so absolutely true), tales.

    Thanks to the activities of above mentioned operators, we may agree with Geertz

    characterization of religion, as well as a totalitarian ideology, as a system of symbols

    which acts to establish powerful, pervasive, and long-lasting moods and motivations in

    humans by formulating conceptions of a general order of existence and clothing these

    conceptions with such an aura of factuality that the moods and motivations seem uniquely

    realistic (Geertz 1966). Howev