Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in...

46
Karl Gunnarsson, M.S., BCBA Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving Individuals with Brain Injury

Transcript of Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in...

Page 1: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Karl Gunnarsson, M.S., BCBA

Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving Individuals with Brain Injury

Page 2: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Objectives

• Participants will have a basic understanding of applied behavioral economics.

• Participants will be able to define choice impulsivity.• Participants will have a basic understanding of delay and probability

discounting.• Participants will understand clinical implications of discounting

research.

Page 3: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 4: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 5: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Learning Objectives

Be able to define choice impulsivity.

Acquire a basic understanding of delay and probability discounting.

Understand clinical implications of discounting research.

Page 6: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Impulsivity

Page 7: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Choice ImpulsivityChoice impulsivity refers to likelihood that a person, who is impulsive, may forgo a larger more beneficial reward because its delivery is distant in time, and choose a smaller reward because it can be acquired immediately or with limited delay

From the New Yorker

Page 8: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Choice Impulsivity

Page 9: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 10: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 11: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 12: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Traditional measurements of impulsivity within the brain injury literature

Page 13: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Imagine that someone would offer you a choice between $950 now, or $1000 in one week from now. What would you choose?

How about $500 now or $1000 in three years from now?

Page 14: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Said differently!

Page 15: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Delay discounting is the functional relationship between time and subjective value of a reward—that is— As time increases the subjective value of a reward will decrease

Page 16: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Now imagine that someone would offer you a choice between $500 for sure, or a 95% of receiving $1000. What would you choose?

How about $500 for sure or 50% of receiving $1000?

Page 17: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Said differently!

Page 18: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Probability discounting is the functional relationship between probability of receiving a reward and subjective value of that reward—that is— As odds against increase the subjective value of a reward will decrease

Page 19: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

“At their heart, delay-discounting procedures are about finding the point at which two

rewards, one relatively immediate and one delayed, have approximately the same

value.” (Odom, 2011, p. 428)

Page 20: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Indifference point

$600

Now Later$1000$750$500$400

$800$950 $1000$1000$1000$1000$1000

Page 21: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Indifference point

$600

Now Later$1000

$750$500$400

$800$950 $1000$1000$1000$1000$1000

=775

Page 22: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Different methodsRachlin et al., 1991

Now$95…..$1

Johnson & Bickel, 2002 Du et al., 2002 Reynolds & Schiffbauer,

2004

Now$50

Later$100 Now

% + T Later% + TReal money

and waiting

Later$100

Page 23: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Slope

V = Ae-kDExponential Hyperbolic

V = A/(1 + kD) V = A/(1 + kD)sHyperboloid

Three methods

Page 24: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

V = Ae-kDExponential Hyperbolic

V = A/(1 + kD) V = A/(1 + kD)sHyperboloid

k = higher impulsivity

1 2 3

123

k = (( A/ V) − 1)/ D.

Page 25: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 26: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 27: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

V = Ae-hOExponential Hyperbolic

V = A/(1 + hO) V = A/(1 + hO)sHyperboloid

h = higher impulsivity

Page 28: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Do we discount everything the same?Amount

Page 29: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Do we discount everything the same?Type of reward

Page 30: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

The rate of discounting appears stable over time for many different clinical populations Kids who discount steeply become steep discounters laterCollege students (no clinical diagnosis) discount at same rate, up to a year later

Page 31: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How about TBI folks?

Page 32: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How about TBI folks?

Page 33: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open
Page 34: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?Trans magnetic stimulation

Working memoryReframing

Episodic future thinking Derived relational responding

Contingency management programs

Page 35: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?Contingency management programs

Derived relational training

Episodic future thinking Reframing

Working memory

Page 36: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?

Working memory

Page 37: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?

Reframing

Page 38: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?

Episodic future thinking

Page 39: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?

Derived relational responding

Page 40: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

How can we alter discounting?

Contingency management programs

Page 41: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

101

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 101–105 NUMBER 1 (SPRING 2004)

PREFERENCE FOR PROGRESSIVE DELAYS ANDCONCURRENT PHYSICAL THERAPY EXERCISE IN

AN ADULT WITH ACQUIRED BRAIN INJURY

MARK R. DIXON AND TERRY S. FALCOMATA

SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY

The purpose of this study was to increase self-control and engagement in a physicaltherapy task (head holding) for a man with acquired traumatic brain injury. Once im-pulsivity was observed (i.e., repeated impulsive choices), an experimental condition wasintroduced that consisted of choices between a small immediate reinforcer, a large fixed-delay reinforcer, and a large progressive-delay reinforcer. The participant showed a pref-erence for the progressive-delay option, even when the duration of the delay exceededthat of the fixed delay. The results have implications for establishing optimal choicemaking and teaching life-enhancing skills.

DESCRIPTORS: brain injury, impulsivity, physical therapy, self-control

One key component in a comprehensiverehabilitation program for persons withbrain injury is physical therapy. Unfortu-nately, many persons who need physicaltherapy refuse to participate in it, and as aresult, do not reach optimal levels of inde-pendence postinjury. Not participating inphysical therapy may be conceptualized asan impulsive choice whereby the person se-lects the small immediate reinforcer of es-cape from a demanding task over the largerdelayed reinforcer of greater independence.Vollmer, Borrero, Lalli, and Daniels (1999)have crafted a similar model conceptualizingthe choice between engaging in problem be-havior or actively participating in meaning-ful tasks. Thus, treatment strategies are

This project was completed in partial fulfillment ofthe master’s degree in behavior analysis by the secondauthor under the direction of the first author. Wethank the other members of the thesis committee, An-thony J. Cuvo and Paula K. Davis. We also extendour appreciation to the Center for ComprehensiveServices in Carbondale, Illinois, for allowing us toconduct this research project. Specifically, John Guer-cio’s and Debbie McMorrow’s assistance was invalu-able. Terry Falcomata is now affiliated with the Uni-versity of Iowa.

Address correspondence to Mark R. Dixon, BehaviorAnalysis and Therapy Program, Southern Illinois Uni-versity, Carbondale, Illinois 62901 (e-mail: [email protected]).

needed to increase cooperative participationin important, but difficult, tasks (e.g., phys-ical therapy).

To date, much of the self-control researchincorporating a fading-to-delay procedurehas compared obtained performances aftertraining to a preexisting baseline of impul-sive choices (Dixon & Cummings, 2001;Dixon & Holcomb, 2000). That is, duringbaseline and following the self-control train-ing procedure, the participants were given achoice between a small immediate reinforcerand a larger delayed reinforcer that was avail-able after the participant completed a taskfor a fixed duration of time. However, dur-ing the self-control training procedure, thesmaller reinforcer was available as in base-line, but the larger reinforcer was madeavailable after a progressively increasing de-lay (starting at 0 s and gradually increasingto the delay associated with the larger rein-forcer during baseline). In effect, these stud-ies might be conceptualized as pretest–post-test designs. No study has assessed the rela-tive changes in preference that may occurfrom the experience with a progressively de-layed alternative on a concurrently availablebaseline choice between a smaller immediatereinforcer and a large fixed-delay reinforcer.

371

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2003, 36, 371–374 NUMBER 3 (FALL 2003)

SELF-CONTROL AND THE PREFERENCE FORDELAYED REINFORCEMENT: AN EXAMPLE IN

BRAIN INJURY

MARK R. DIXON AND MOLLIE J. HORNER

SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY

AND

JOHN GUERCIO

CENTER FOR COMPREHENSIVE SERVICES

We investigated the effects of a concurrent physical therapy activity (keeping the handopen) during delays to reinforcement in an adult man with acquired brain injuries. Oncea relatively stable level of hand-open behavior was obtained, the participant was asked tochoose between a small immediate reinforcer and a larger delayed reinforcer contingenton keeping the hand open at greater-than-baseline duration. Afterwards, the participantwas asked to select between a larger delayed reinforcer with no hand-open requirementand the identical larger delayed reinforcer with a progressively increasing hand-open re-quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open requirement option.

DESCRIPTORS: self-control, impulsivity, physical therapy, delayed reinforcement

Self-control occurs when a participant se-lects a larger delayed reinforcer over a con-currently available smaller immediate rein-forcer. Persons with acquired brain injury aresometimes resistant to physical therapy, per-haps choosing the immediate reinforcementof escape from the aversiveness of the ther-apy over the delayed reinforcement of even-tually returning to a higher level of func-tioning.

One step is to increase the choice to en-gage in the therapy. Strategies that can beused to promote more optimal choice mak-

This project was completed in partial fulfillment ofthe MS degree in Behavior Analysis and Therapy atSouthern Illinois University by the second author un-der direction of the first. We thank the other membersof that thesis committee, Anthony J. Cuvo and RuthAnne Rehfeldt, for their shaping of this final product.Thanks are also extended to Debbie Braunling-McMorrow and Anita Daneshdoost of the Center forComprehensive Services of Carbondale, Illinois, fortheir support for this project.

Address all correspondence to Mark R. Dixon, Be-havior Analysis and Therapy Program, RehabilitationInstitute, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Il-linois 62901 (e-mail: [email protected]).

ing include the fading of delay duration(Dixon & Cummings, 2001) or the intro-duction of a concurrent activity to be emit-ted during the delay (Dixon, Rehfeldt, &Randich, 2003). Another way to alter pref-erence has been via the implementation of asignal or discriminative stimulus associatedwith one delayed alternative (Vollmer, Bor-rero, Lalli, & Daniel, 1999). Such stimulimay enhance preference for this option overa more immediate (and less optimal) alter-native by serving a conditioned reinforcingfunction (Stromer, McComas, & Rehfeldt,2000). Although Vollmer et al. demonstrat-ed that a signaling stimulus such as a cardor object can alter choice preference, it isunknown whether an actual low-probabilitybehavior emitted by the participant couldfunction similarly. Therefore, the purpose ofthe present study was to shift reinforcer pref-erences to those of more delayed options andassess the relative value of a concurrent phys-ical therapy activity during the delays to re-inforcement in a participant with acquiredbrain injuries.

Page 42: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

“If delay discounting moderates treatment effectiveness, it follows that information about an individual’ s propensity to discount delayed rewards may be quite useful when it comes to choosing intervention components.”

Page 43: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Future implications and utility for TBI:Effort research

AltruismNeuroeconomics

Page 44: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Future implications and utility for TBI:Mindfulness

Acceptance and Commitment TherapyProgressive delay

Brain plasticity research

Page 45: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Audrain-McGovern, J., Rodriguez, D., Epstein, L. H., Cuevas, J., Rodgers, K., & Wileyto, E. P. (2009). Does delay discounting play an etiological role in smoking or is it a consequence of smoking? Drug Alcohol Depend, 103(3), 99-106.

Bickel, W. K., Yi, R., Landes, R. D., Hill, P. F., & Baxter, C. (2011). Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological psychiatry, 69(3), 260-265.

Dixon, M. R., & Falcomata, T. S. (2004). Preference for progressive delays and concurrent physical therapy exercise in an adult with acquired brain injury. J Appl Behav Anal, 37(1), 101-105. doi:10.1901/jaba.2004.37-101

Dixon, M. R., Horner, M. J., & Guercio, J. (2003). Self-control and the preference for delayed reinforcement: An example in brain injury. J Appl Behav Anal, 36(3), 371-374. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-371

Dixon, M. R., Jacobs, E. A., Sanders, S., Guercio, J. M., Soldner, J., Parker-Singler, S., . . . Dillen, J. E. (2005). Impulsivity, self-control, and delay discounting in persons with acquired brain injury. Behavioral Interventions, 20(1), 101-120. doi:10.1002/bin.173

Du, W., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2002). Cross-cultural comparison of discounting of delayed and probabilistic rewards. The Psychological Record, 52, 479-492. Green, L., Myerson, J., & Vanderveldt, A. (2014). Delay and probability discounting. In F. K. McSweeney, E. S. Murphy, F. K. McSweeney, & E. S. Murphy (Eds.), The Wiley

Blackwell handbook of operant and classical conditioning. (pp. 307-337): Wiley-Blackwell.Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within‐subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. J Exp Anal Behav, 77(2), 129-146. Kaplan, B. A., Reed, D. D., & Jarmolowicz, D. P. (2016). Effects of episodic future thinking on discounting: Personalized age-progressed pictures improve risky long-term health

decisions. J Appl Behav Anal, 49(1), 148-169. doi:10.1002/jaba.277Kirby, K. N. (2009). One-year temporal stability of delay-discount rates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(16), 457-462. Kocka, A., & Gagnon, J. (2014). Definition of Impulsivity and Related Terms Following Traumatic Brain Injury: A Review of the Different Concepts and Measures Used to Assess

Impulsivity, Disinhibition and other Related Concepts. Behavioral Sciences, 4(4), 352. Malec, J. F. (2014). Comprehensive brain injury rehabilitation in post-hospital treatment settings. In M. Sherer, A. M. Sander, M. Sherer, & A. M. Sander (Eds.), Handbook on the

neuropsychology of traumatic brain injury. (pp. 283-307). New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media.McHugh, L., & Wood, R. L. (2008). Using a temporal discounting paradigm to measure decision-making and impulsivity following traumatic brain injury: A pilot study. Brain

injury, 22(9), 715-721. doi:10.1080/02699050802263027Merzenich, M. M., Van Vleet, T. M., & Nahum, M. (2015). Brain plasticity-based therapeutics. Neuroplasticity and Neurorehabilitation, 6. Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay Discounting: I'm a k, You're a k. J Exp Anal Behav, 96(3), 427-439. doi:10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2004). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: an experiential discounting task. Behav Processes, 67(3), 343-356. doi:http://

dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.06.003Sellitto, M., Ciaramelli, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2010). Myopic Discounting of Future Rewards after Medial Orbitofrontal Damage in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience,

30(49), 16429. Sellitto, M., Ciaramelli, E., Mattioli, F., & di Pellegrino, G. (2016). Reduced sensitivity to sooner reward during intertemporal decision-making following insula damage in humans.

Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00367Wood, R. L., & McHugh, L. (2013). Decision making after traumatic brain injury: A temporal discounting paradigm. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,

19(02), 181-188.

Page 46: Applied Behavioral Economics for Clinicians Serving ......quirement. Results suggest a shift in preference to larger delayed reinforcers and an even-tual preference for the hand-open

Send questions [email protected]

Thanks!