An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

download An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

of 14

Transcript of An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    1/14

    An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring: Links to MoralDisengagement and Unethical Business Decision Making

    BABATUNDE OGUNFOWORA1*, JOSHUA S. BOURDAGE

    2and BRENDA NGUYEN

    3

    1Goodman School of Business, Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada2 Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada3 Haskayne School of Business, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada

     Abstract: The majority of research on self-monitoring has focused on the positive aspects of this personality trait. The

    goal of the present research was to shed some light on the potential negative side of self-monitoring and resulting

    consequences in two independent studies. Study 1 demonstrated that, in addition to being higher on Extraversion, high

    self-monitors are also more likely to be low on Honesty-Humility, which is characterized by a tendency to be dishonest 

    and driven by self-gain. Study 2 was designed to investigate the consequences of this dishonest side of self-monitoring

    using two previously unexamined outcomes: moral disengagement and unethical business decision making. Results

    showed that high self-monitors are more likely to engage in unethical business decision making and that this relationship

    is mediated by the propensity to engage in moral disengagement. In addition, these negative effects of self-monitoring

    were found to be due to its low Honesty-Humility aspect, rather than its high Extraversion side. Further investigation

    showed similar effects for the Other-Directedness and Acting (but not Extraversion) self-monitoring subscales. These

     ndings provide valuable insight into previously unexamined negative consequences of self-monitoring and suggest im-

     portant directions for future research on self-monitoring. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

    Key words: self-monitoring; Honesty-Humility; Extraversion; moral disengagement; ethical business decision

    making; HEXACO model; personality

    Since its initial proposition (Snyder, 1974), the self-monitoring

    construct has been a topic of enthusiastic investigation in many

    research domains. Self-monitoring refers to the extent to which

    an individual monitors the public appearance of the self in

    social settings and interpersonal relationships (Snyder, 1987).

    High self-monitors1 are attuned to their social environment 

    and adjust their behaviours in order to control images that they

    present to others. Low self-monitors, on the other hand, are

    more consistent in their behaviours across contexts and are also

    consistent with their attitudes, emotions and dispositions

    (Snyder & Gangestad, 1986).

    A review of the literature shows strong evidence in support 

    of the positive effects of high self-monitoring, including higher 

    emotional intelligence (Tett, Fox, & Wang, 2005), greater non-verbal decoding skills (ability to recognize emotional displays

    in others) and behavioural sensitivity to the expectations of 

    others (see Gangestad & Snyder, 2000, for a review). Within

    the workplace context, high self-monitoring has been positively

    linked to promotions over a 5-year period (Kilduff & Day,

    1994), positive leadership behaviours, perceptions of leader 

    effectiveness (Day, Schleicher, Unckless, & Hiller, 2002),

    leadership emergence (Cronshaw & Ellis, 1991; Ellis, 1988),

    greater job involvement (Day et al., 2002) and higher evaluations

    on interpersonal aspects of job performance (Barrick, Parks, &

    Mount, 2005; Blakely, Andrews, & Fuller, 2003).

    Despite these positive effects, however, there appears to be

    a somewhat more negative side to self-monitoring. In their 

    seminal review, Gangestad and Snyder (2000) noted that 

    although constructs such as self-monitoring may be good

    natured and functional in terms of social interaction, there   “. . .

    may also be an essential component in illicit social activities

    as well, such as lying, concealing one’s true intentions, or 

    presenting an inauthentic self ”   (p. 530). Indeed, researchers

    have found that high self-monitors tend to condone lies when

    there is opportunity for self-gain and engage in deception inromantic relationships (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000; McLeod

    & Genereux, 2008). Thus, there is a growing body of 

    evidence indicating that there may be a more negative side

    to self-monitoring. The goal of the present research was to

    investigate the personality basis of this negative side and

    potential consequences in two independent studies. In the

    succeeding discussions, we provide an overview of the

    extant theoretical and empirical work on self-monitoring

    and how the present research   ts within this literature.

    Next, we present two empirical studies designed to extend the

    literature. In Study 1, we examined the personality correlates

    of self-monitoring in order to show the dual aspects of thisconstruct —that is, its   ‘positive’   (high Extraversion) and

    ‘negative’   (low Honesty-Humility) sides. In Study 2, we

    *Correspondence to: Babatunde Ogunfowora, Goodman School of Busi-ness, Brock University, St Catharines, Ontario, Canada.E-mail: [email protected] 1We use the term   ‘high self-monitor ’ as an abbreviation to describe individ-uals who score highly on self-monitoring scales. It is not our intent to usethis term as a diagnostic label.

     European Journal of Personality, Eur. J. Pers.  27: 532–544 (2013)

    Published online 11 July 2013 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)  DOI: 10.1002/per.1931

     Received 2 August 2012

     Revised 25 March 2013, Accepted 1 April 2013Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    2/14

    examined the consequences of the proposed negative side of 

    self-monitoring with respect to the propensity to engage in

    moral disengagement and to make unethical business

    decisions.

    THEORETICAL BACKGROUND

    Although Snyder (1974, 1987) initially conceptualized self-

    monitoring as uni-dimensional, the past few decades have

    witnessed extensive debate about its dimensionality. The major-

    ity of this debate stems from evaluations of the psychometric

    properties of Snyder ’s Self-Monitoring Scale (Snyder, 1974;

    Snyder & Gangestad, 1986). According to this body of 

    research, the Self-Monitoring Scale can be broken down into

    three subscales, namely Acting, Extraversion and Other-

    Directedness (Lennox & Wolfe, 1984; Snyder & Gangestad,

    1986). Acting describes the extent to which the high self-

    monitor is capable of playing or assuming the role of a different 

    persona or character (e.g. ‘I would probably make a good actor ’or   ‘I   nd it hard to imitate the behaviour of other people’—

    Reversed). Other-Directedness is concerned with the extent to

    which the individual portrays multiple façades to different 

    persons and in different contexts (e.g.   ‘In different situations

    and with different people, I often act like very different persons’

    and   ‘I’m not always the person I appear to be’). Last, the

    Extraversion subscale is composed of sociable and outgoing

    behaviours (e.g.   ‘At a party, I let others keep the jokes and

    stories going’—Reversed). On the basis of a comprehensive

    quantitative review of this literature, Gangestad and Snyder 

    (2000) reported evidence in support of a two-factor structure,

    which they termed Public Performing and Other-Directedness(see also Briggs & Cheek, 1988; Gangestad & Snyder, 1985).

    All three subscales were found to load on the Public Performing

    factor, which captures the majority of the variance of the scale.

    This factor, named the Self-Monitoring axis, was found to be

    very strongly closely associated with the Acting subscale

    (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000). Last, the Other-Directedness

    and Extraversion subscales loaded on the second dimension

    (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000).

    The prominence of Extraversion in Snyder ’s (1974)

    self-monitoring measure led to extensive investigations

    into the extent to which self-monitoring and its effects

    could be primarily accounted for by trait Extraversion

    and other related   ‘social’   traits (e.g. Briggs & Cheek,1988; John, Cheek, & Klohnen, 1996). Across a number 

    of studies, Extraversion has emerged as an important corre-

    late of self-monitoring, typically possessing positive rela-

    tionships in the .30s and .40s (Barrick et al., 2005; Bono

    & Vey, 2007; Briggs & Cheek, 1988). Gangestad and

    Snyder (2000) acknowledged that Extraversion enjoys a 

    dominant presence in the Self-Monitoring Scale and is a 

    nontrivial covariate of the self-monitoring axis. Moreover,

    the link between self-monitoring and trait Extraversion

    may also be explained by the Acting subscale, which to

    some extent requires social skills in order to be successful

    (i.e. to   ‘be an actor ’

      or to imitate the social behavioursof others). Research evidence, however, indicates that 

    self-monitoring and its effects cannot be solely explained

    by Extraversion or any other personality trait within the

    dominant Big Five factor structure (Barrick et al., 2005;

    Gangestad & Snyder, 2000). Thus, there appears to be a 

    distinct aspect to self-monitoring that, according to Gangestad

    and Snyder, is informed by phenomena that pertain to   “varia-

    tion in predispositions to engage in certain forms of impression

    management . . .categories that can best be characterized as

    concerning the active construction of public selves designed

    to achieve social ends” (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000, p. 546).

    A review of the emerging literature on broad personality

    dimensions suggests that this other aspect of Self-Monitoring

    may be better captured under an alternative personality frame-

    work known as the HEXACO Personality Model (Ashton &

    Lee, 2007; Lee & Ashton, 2005). The HEXACO model pro-

    poses that there are six, not ve, broad dimensions of personal-

    ity, namely: Honesty-Humility, Emotionality, eXtraversion,

    Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness to Experi-

    ence (Ashton & Lee, 2007). Although there are a few notable

    differences between the HEXACO model and the prominent 

    Big Five Model, the inclusion of Honesty-Humility, the sixthpersonality dimension, represents a key departure and signi-

    cant advantage of this model (Ashton & Lee, 2007; Lee,

    Ogunfowora, & Ashton, 2005). Honesty-Humility is described

    by adjectives such as honest, fair, sincere or loyal versus

    greedy, conceited, pretentious and sly (Ashton & Lee, 2007).

    The negative pole of Honesty-Humility highlights the ten-

    dency to be dishonest and driven by self-gain, the acquisition

    of material goods and willingness to condone fraudulent acts,

    including presenting a façade to gain something from others

    (Ashton & Lee, 2007). These descriptors, particularly the

    latter, share theoretical similarities with high self-monitoring.

    As noted earlier, Gangestad and Snyder (2000) argued that the Self-Monitoring axis reects a predisposition to engage in

    impression management and image management behaviours.

    Similarly, individuals who are low on Honesty-Humility have

    been shown to engage in impression management and

    impression-management-motivated behaviours (Bourdage,

    Lee, & Ogunfowora, 2009; Bourdage, Lee, Lee, & Shin,

    2012). This suggests a negative link between self-monitoring

    and Honesty-Humility. In particular, low Honesty-Humility

    should be associated with the Other-Directedness subscale.

    This is because they both share a tendency to be disingenuous

    in interpersonal relations, specically, with respect to

    presenting an inconsistent self to others in order to gain some-

    thing from them (Ashton & Lee, 2007; Gangestad & Snyder,2000). Moreover, low Honesty-Humility may also be posi-

    tively related to the Acting subscale, which to some degree

    likely relies on an ability to ‘deceive’—that is, to the extent that 

    Acting entails assuming and convincingly portraying the per-

    sona of someone else to observers (Gangestad & Snyder,

    2000). Indeed, the previously described associations of high

    self-monitoring to the tendency to lie and deceive others

    (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000; Mcleod & Genereux, 2008) are

    consistent with low levels of Honesty-Humility (Ashton &

    Lee, 2007). Not surprising, therefore, researchers have found

    a negative link (r =.42) between Honesty-Humility and

    self-monitoring—

      measured using Snyder ’s (1974) Self-

    Monitoring Scale (Ashton & Lee, 2005). More recently,

    Grieve (2011) found partial support for this relationship by

    Self-Monitoring and Honesty-Humility 533

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    3/14

    using a different measure of self-monitoring (Lennox &

    Wolfe, 1984). Grieve found that Honesty-Humility was neg-

    atively related to the   ‘ability to monitor self-presentation’

    subscale of self-monitoring but not the ‘ sensitivity to expres-

    sive behaviour of others’ subscale. Thus, in addition to Ex-

    traversion, low Honesty-Humility appears to be an

    important personality basis of self-monitoring.

    The proposed dual nature of self-monitoring may also

    be understood within the context of a prominent theoretical

    model in the self-monitoring literature—Arkin’s (1981)

    theory of    acquisitive   and   protective   self-presentation

    styles. According to this theory, the way people present 

    themselves to the world can be captured by two motiva-

    tional processes. Acquisitive self-monitoring is concerned

    with self-presentation driven by a desire for social gain

    and approval (Arkin, 1981; Wolfe, Lennox, & Cutler,

    1986). This corresponds to the desire of high self-

    monitors to   ‘get ahead’   by actively managing how they

    are perceived by others in an effort to garner social success

    and rewards (Rauthmann, 2011). This is also conceptuallysimilar to low Honesty-Humility, which is driven by the

    acquisition of material goods and willingness to condone

    fraudulent acts for self-gain (Ashton & Lee, 2007).

    Honesty-Humility also appears to share some conceptual

    links with protective self-monitoring. Ashton and Lee

    (2007) suggested that Honesty-Humility can be theoreti-

    cally interpreted as altruism-driven—a motive that pushes

    one who is high on Honesty-Humility to deal genuinely

    with others   ‘even when one might exploit them without 

    suffering retaliation’   (p. 156). In contrast, the individual

    who is low on Honesty-Humility is driven by selsh mo-

    tives. Protective self-monitoring also appears to have anunderlying self-serving motive; that is, to the extent that 

    the high protective self-monitor is motivated to get along

    with others only to   ‘protect ’   the self from social rejection

    (Arkin, 1981; Wolfe et al., 1986).

    In support of these postulations, Rauthmann (2011) recently

    demonstrated that the protective and acquisitive aspects of self-

    monitoring are both related to the so-called Dark Triad—

    narcissism, psychopathy and Machiavellianism (Paulhus &

    Williams, 2002)—a group of traits that have in common a ten-

    dency to manipulate and exploit others for the pursuit of selsh

    gains. Interestingly, Lee and Ashton (2005) found that Honesty-

    Humility relates substantially with each of the Dark Triad traits,

    with correlations ranging from  

    .53 (Machiavellianism) to.72 (psychopathy). More recently, Lee, Ashton, Wiltshire,

    Bourdage, Visser, and Gallucci (2012) demonstrated that the

    core variance of the Dark Triad variables can be accounted

    for by low Honesty-Humility.

    The positive association of self-monitoring with Extraver-

    sion may also be understood in terms of the protective and ac-

    quisitive self-presentation styles. High protective self-monitors

    attempt to minimize the negative repercussions of social rejec-

    tion by putting up façades in social situations in order to facil-

    itate acceptance. They are likely to be successful at this to the

    extent that they possess extraverted qualities (i.e. outgoing,

    talkative, energetic, and enthusiastic; Costa & McCrae, 1991)that enable them to acutely manoeuvre social situations in or-

    der to stand out and incur social approval (rather than

    rejection). Moreover, Rauthmann (2011) has proposed that 

    the link with Extraversion can also be understood from an ac-

    quisitive perspective to the extent that Extraversion results in

    feelings of social reward and success when the high self-

    monitor effectively presents a favourable image of himself or 

    herself in social situations.

    In sum, extant theoretical and empirical research to date

    strongly indicates that Extraversion and low Honesty-Humility

    are important personality bases of self-monitoring. A deeper un-

    derstanding of this dual nature of self-monitoring is important 

    for a number of practical reasons. Although self-monitoring is

    positively related to many indicators of success in life, the

    preliminary evidence showing a negative co-variation with

    Honesty-Humility is a signicant cause for concern. This is

    because research indicates that low levels of Honesty-Humility

    are associated with delinquency and antisocial behaviours

    (Dunlop, Morrison, Koenig, & Silcox, 2012; Lee, Ashton, &

    Shin, 2005), sexual promiscuity and sexual harassment (Ashton

    & Lee, 2008; Bourdage, Lee, Ashton, & Perry, 2007; Lee,

    Gizzarone, & Ashton, 2003), counterproductive work behav-iours (Lee, Ashton, & De Vries, 2005; Marcus, Lee, & Ashton,

    2007) and unethical business decision making (Lee, Ashton,

    Morrison, Cordery, & Dunlop, 2008). This suggests that high

    self-monitoring may also be associated with these negative out-

    comes. To date, however, there have been no further investiga-

    tions into the low Honesty-Humility aspect of self-monitoring

    following Ashton and Lee’s (2005) initial research. The issue

    of the dual nature of self-monitoring was not the focus of past 

    research (Ashton & Lee, 2005; Grieve, 2011) and, as such, has

    received minimal attention to date.

    The present research

    The present multi-study research sought to shed further light 

    on the proposed dual aspect of self-monitoring. Study 1 was

    designed to test the proposition that Extraversion and

    Honesty-Humility are the two most important broad personal-

    ity factors associated with self-monitoring. This investigation

    was conducted in two separate samples and contexts. Study 2

    was designed to investigate the consequences of the proposed

    dual nature of self-monitoring using two previously

    unexamined outcomes: moral disengagement and ethical busi-

    ness decision making.

    STUDY 1

    We began our investigation by exploring the most important 

    broad personality correlates of self-monitoring in an attempt 

    to corroborate and extend Lee and Ashton’s (2005) initial

    ndings. These relationships were tested using multiple

    sources (self-reports and peer reports) of self-monitoring

    and HEXACO personality traits in an undergraduate student 

    sample. At a general level, demonstrating that the   ndings

    converge across multiple methods of measurement of the

    same traits is pertinent for illustrating the robustness of the

    proposed dual nature of self-monitoring (Campbell & Fiske,1959; Oh, Wang, & Mount, 2011). This is in line with

    Hofstee’s (1994) recommendation that researchers provide

    534 B. Ogunfowora  et al .

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    4/14

    evidence beyond self-reports when attempting to establish

    relationships among personality constructs. Moreover, sole

    reliance on self-reports may limit the robustness of conclu-

    sions, as observer reports of personality may also capture

    important unique variance in understanding the relationship

    among constructs (Oh, Lee, Ashton, & De Vries, 2011).

    Thus, we employed both self-ratings and observer ratings

    in the   rst sample. In line with past research, we expect 

    that Honesty-Humility and Extraversion will demonstrate the

    strongest and most consistent associations with self-monitoring

    across rating sources.

     Hypothesis 1: Among the major personality dimensions, Extra-version and Honesty-Humility will emerge as the strongest andmost consistent correlates of self-monitoring.

    We further tested the proposed relationships between

    self-monitoring, Extraversion and Honesty-Humility in an

    employment context. This allowed for a test of the generaliz-

    ability of the proposed relationships in a sample of adults in

    an employment setting. Although only self-reports wereavailable, this is important because a substantive body of 

    research on self-monitoring has been conducted in the work 

    domain (e.g. Barrick et al., 2005; Blakely et al., 2003; Bono

    & Vey, 2007; Day et al., 2002; Kilduff & Day, 1994).

    Method

    Participants and procedure

    Sample 1.   The   rst sample consisted of a total of 258

    undergraduate students from a mid-sized Canadian university.

    Of these, 65% were female, and the average age was

    20.63 years (SD = 3.96). Participants arrived in dyads, withclose friends attending together. Each person completed a 

    self-report questionnaire and a peer-report questionnaire. In

    order to help participants feel comfortable answering honestly,

    we ensured that participants sat at separate tables, and if this

    was not possible, they were separated by a large divider. The

    participants were awarded research credits or paid $20 for 

    participating in the study.

    The peers consisted of 63% female participants, with an

    average age of 20.69 years (SD = 4.25). Participants

    reported knowing their peers an average of 4.75 years

    (SD = 4.31), and this corresponded with the estimates pro-

    vided by the peers. We also obtained subjective ratingsof how well participants reported knowing each other 

    using a 10-point rating scale and found an average rating

    of 7.75 (SD = 1.59).

    Sample 2.   Participants from the second sample were

    employees in the Services department of a not-for-prot 

    human services organization in Canada. The total organization

    size was 358 employees, 226 of whom worked in the Services

    department. One hundred and ten Services employees

    participated in the study, indicating a response rate of about 

    49%. The average age of respondents was 41.52 years

    (SD = 11.19). Approximately 78% were women, and the

    average tenure with the organization of 38.26 months(SD = 60.35 months). The present data were collected as part 

    of a larger validation study at the organization. The authors

    attended team meetings during which employees were

    asked to complete a number of questionnaires that included

    personality scales.

     Measures

    Personality.   In both samples, personality was measuredusing the HEXACO Personality Inventory Revised

    (HEXACO PI-R; Lee & Ashton, 2004). This scale assesses

    six personality factors: Honesty-Humility, Emotionality,

    Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and

    Openness to Experience. The items are scored on a 5-point 

    scale (1= strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree).

    In Sample 1, the 100-item HEXACO PI-R was adminis-

    tered, and self-reports and peer reports were obtained. The

    internal consistency reliabilities for the self-report scales

    were acceptable: Honesty-Humility (a = .80), Emotionality

    (a= .83), Extraversion (a = .85), Agreeableness (a= .85),

    Conscientiousness (a = .82) and Openness to Experience

    (a= .82). The reliability estimates for the peer-report scales

    were also acceptable: Honesty-Humility (a= .85), Emotional-

    ity (a= .88), Extraversion (a= .87), Agreeableness (a= .89),

    Conscientiousness (a = .86) and Openness to Experience

    (a= .84). In Sample 2, because of pressures from management 

    to keep the survey short, a limited number of items were se-

    lected to measure each personality trait. Honesty-Humility

    was assessed using 12 items (a = .74) from the HEXACO Fair-

    ness, Sincerity and Modesty subscales, whereas Extraversion

    was assessed using eight items (a = .66) obtained from the

    HEXACO Liveliness and Social Boldness subscales.

    Self-monitoring.   In Sample 1, self-monitoring was assessed

    using self-reports and peer reports on the 18-item Self-

    Monitoring Scale (Snyder & Gangestad, 1986). Responses

    were assessed on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree

    to 5 = strongly agree). The internal consistency

    reliabilities for the self-reports and peer-report measures

    were .73 and .72, respectively. We also calculated

    scores for the three subscales of Acting, Extraversion

    and Other-Directedness. The reliability estimates (self-

    reports and peer reports, respectively) were as follows:

    Acting (a= .74; .67), Extraversion (a = .63; .65) and

    Other-directedness (a= .45; .54) subscales. These somewhat 

    lower values are quite similar to those reported in past research. For instance, Hoyle and Lennox’s (1991)

    comprehensive study found reliability estimates of .70

    for the total 18-item self-report scale, and .64, .64 and

    .46 for the Acting, Extraversion and Other-Directedness

    subscales, respectively.

    In Sample 2, self-monitoring was assessed using self-

    reports on the 10-item Self-Monitoring Scale from the Inter-

    national Personality Item Pool (IPIP; Goldberg et al., 2006).

    Responses were assessed on a 5-point Likert scale

    (1 = strongly disagree   t o 5= strongly agree). The obtained

    reliability of this measure was .79. This IPIP scale (Gold-

    berg et al., 2006) was developed to closely reect self-monitoring as measured by Snyder (1974), but was not 

    designed to measure the three subscales.

    Self-Monitoring and Honesty-Humility 535

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    5/14

    Results

    The means, standard deviations and correlations among

    study variables can be found in Table 1. In examining the

    correlations among self-ratings and peer ratings of personal-

    ity and self-monitoring, we used a multi-trait-multi-method

    approach (Campbell & Fiske, 1959), looking at the correla-

    tions between the following: (i) self-rated HEXACO traitsand self-rated self-monitoring; (ii) self-rated HEXACO

    traits and peer-rated self-monitoring; (iii) peer-rated

    HEXACO traits and self-rated self-monitoring; and (iv)

    peer-rated HEXACO traits and peer-rated self-monitoring.2

    In terms of overall self-monitoring, across all four sets of correla-

    tions, the links between self-monitoring, Extraversion and

    Honesty-Humility were replicated. Consistent with past research,

    Extraversion was strongly and signicantly correlated with self-

    monitoring across sources (range = .34 to .47; average r = .40).3

    Moreover, as expected, Honesty-Humility emerged as a statisti-

    cally signicant correlate of self-monitoring across sources

    (range =.15 to .42; average r =.30). In comparison, open-

    ness to experience—the next trait most consistently related to

    self-monitoring—demonstrated relatively modest associations

    (range = .15 to .27; average r = .19). All other traits demonstrated

    weakand/or inconsistent associations with self-monitoringacross

    source (thenext strongest averagecorrelation was Agreeableness,

    r =.15). Taken together, the results obtained from both samples

    provide strong support for hypothesis 1. Last, we found a 

    moderate correlation of .42 between self-ratings and peer-ratings

    of self-monitoring, indicating that students can judge the self-

    monitoring of their peers to some extent.

    Table 1 also shows the correlations with the self-monitoring

    subscales. Across sources, Honesty-Humility was moderately

    correlated with Other-directedness (range=

    .11 to

    .54, aver-age   r =.31) and Acting (range =.16 to   .31, average

    r =.24) but relatively weaker with the Extraversion subscale

    (range =.01 to .16, average r =.09). In contrast, Extraver-

    sion was most strongly associated with the Extraversion

    subscale (range = .40 to .64, average  r = .54), moderately with

    Acting (range = .31 to .42, average   r = .37) and weakly with

    Other-Directedness (range=.21 to .03, average  r =.09).

    The means, standard deviations and inter-correlations

    among the study variables for the adult work sample are

    presented at the bottom of Table 1. The results show that 

    overall self-monitoring is signicantly correlated with Extra-

    version (r = .42,   p< .001) and Honesty-Humility (r =.38,

     p< .001). These results replicate the pattern of correlations

    obtained in the student sample in an employment context.

    Brief discussion

    The results of Study 1 provide support for the dual nature of self-

    monitoring. Our results corroborated past   ndings that have

    found the primary personality correlate of self-monitoring to

    be Extraversion. The magnitude of this correlation across differ-

    ent sources was similar to that reported in previous studies

    (Barrick et al., 2005; Bono & Vey, 2007; Briggs & Cheek,

    1988). Study 1 also demonstrated that, across different sources,

    high self-monitoring is also associated with low Honesty-

    Humility. Moreover, we demonstrate that these  ndings gener-

    alized to an employed adult sample. Past research has shown

    that high self-monitors may be more prone towards engaging

    in deceit, manipulation and status-driven motivation (e.g.Gangestad & Snyder, 2000). Similarly, low Honesty-Humility

    has at its core the tendency to be duplicitous and status-

    oriented, and correlates highly with the Dark Triad personality

    traits (Lee & Ashton, 2005; Lee et al., 2012). The fact that the

    negative association between these two traits generalized across

    both self-reports and peer-reports indicates that these correla-

    tions are not solely due to same source effects. As such, these

    features strengthen our condence in the conclusions drawn.

    However, we do note that a possible limitation of our study de-

    sign is that participants were allowed to choose which of their 

    friends to bring along to the study. This may have introduced

    some degree of selection bias as participants may have invitedonly good friends that are more likely to endorse a view of the

    individual that is similar to the participant ’s self-views.4

    At the facet level, Honesty-Humility appears to be mod-

    erately associated with Other-Directedness and, to a slightly

    lesser degree, the Acting subscale of the Self-Monitoring

    Scale. The stronger association with the Other-

    Directedness subscale is in line with Grieve’s (2011)  nding

    that Honesty-Humility was most strongly associated with

    the component of Lennox and Wolfe’s (1984) self-

    monitoring measure that pertains to one’s ability to modify

    behaviour in response to social cues (rather than a sensitivity

    or ability to detect these cues). The association with the

    Extraversion subscale was much smaller in comparison. Incontrast, HEXACO Extraversion was most strongly associ-

    ated with the Extraversion subscale, followed by the Acting

    subscale. The observed pattern of correlations therefore sug-

    gests that Honesty-Humility is most closely associated with

    Other-Directedness; HEXACO Extraversion is most aligned

    with the Extraversion subscale; and both Honesty-Humility

    and Extraversion are implicated in the Acting subscale.

    These differential patterns of relationships indicate that it 

    may useful to explore the consequences of the dual nature

    of the self-monitoring construct by using the subscales of 

    the Self-Monitoring Scale, in addition to analyses using the

    overall scale.

    2We also tested the extent to which the cross-source correlations were af-

    fected by the length of time that participants had known each other and/or how well they reported knowing each other. The results showed that acrossall sets of analyses, the length of time or  ‘how well known’ generally did not signicantly moderate the relationships between (i) self-monitoring andHonesty-Humility, (ii) self-monitoring and Extraversion and (iii) self-report self-monitoring and peer-report self-monitoring. The only exceptionwas that the relationship between self-ratings of self-monitoring and peer-ratings of Extraversion was moderated by length of time known (but not how well known):   b =.04,   t = 2.15,   p< .05,   Δ R2 = .02. This relationshipwas stronger for those who had known their peers for a longer period—that is, one standard deviation above the mean (b = .62,   p< .01)—versus a shorter (one standard deviation below the mean) duration (b= .25,  p< .05).However, given that this is the only relationship observed out of 10 tested,we do not place emphasis on this result because of the potential of capitaliz-ing on chance.3For all of the   ‘average correlations’ reported for Study 1, at the suggestionof a reviewer, averages were computed by transforming each correlationusing Fisher ’s  z-transformation,   nding the average and then reversing thetransformation for the new mean. This approach is described in detail byThorndike (2007).   4We thank an anonymous reviewer for bringing this issue to our attention.

    536 B. Ogunfowora  et al .

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    6/14

    STUDY 2

    Study 1 established that self-monitoring can be understood in

    terms of high Extraversion and low Honesty-Humility, at 

    least among broad personality dimensions. Building on this

    research, an important line of inquiry that warrants further at-

    tention pertains to the potential implications of the low

    Honesty-Humility correlate of self-monitoring. To this end,

    Study 2 was designed to test the effects of self-monitoring

    on outcomes that are likely to be the result of this dishonest 

    side. Specically, we examined the extent to which self-

    monitoring relates to unethical business decision making,as well as the potential mediating effect of moral disengage-

    ment (Bandura, 1986) in understanding this association.

    Last, we tested the proposition that these relationships can

    be explained primarily by low Honesty-Humility but not 

    high Extraversion.

    Self-monitoring and unethical decision making

    A potential negative consequence of the low Honesty-

    Humility aspect of self-monitoring is the propensity to make

    unethical business decisions. Gangestad & Snyder (2000)

    have suggested that high self-monitoring is driven in part 

    by the motivation for self-gain and the need to acquire social

    status and success. This is also in line with the theoreticalinterpretation of acquisitive self-monitoring, which high-

    lights a desire for social gain that is driven by a   ‘get ahead’

    mentality (Wolfe et al., 1986). We propose that such a 

    motive may increase the probability that a high self-

    monitor will make unethical business decisions, particularly

    those judged to benet the self and which allows the high

    self-monitor to get ahead in life. A similar drive for self-

    gain and social status is also inherent in low Honesty-

    Humility. Not surprisingly, research studies have found a 

    positive association between low Honesty-Humility and

    unethical business decision making (e.g. Lee et al., 2008;

    Perugini & Leone, 2009; Van Gelder & De Vries, 2012). A

    high self-monitor may further reason that an unethical deci-

    sion that brings about  nancial success may help to elevatehis or her social status and ultimately foster social acceptance

    rather than rejection by others (i.e. protective self-

    monitoring). These arguments indirectly suggest that high

    self-monitors may be likely to make unethical business

    decisions. Moreover, self-monitoring has also been linked

    to discrete forms of unethical intentions, such as willing-

    ness to condoning lies and a tendency to deceive and

    manipulate others (Grieve, 2011; Mcleod & Genereux,

    2008; Gangestad & Snyder, 2000). Hence, we propose that 

    self-monitoring will be associated with unethical business

    decision making.

     Hypothesis 2: Self-monitoring will be positively related tounethical decision making.

    Table 1. Means, standard deviations and correlations among the variables in Study 1 (student sample and adult working sample)

     M SD

    Self-report self-monitoring Peer-report self-monitoring

    SM Total Acting Other-D Extra SM Total Acting Other-D Extra  

    Student sample M    3.04 2.91 2.97 3.31 2.93 2.82 2.75 3.27

    SD   .47 .70 .58 .61 .44 .57 .62 .66HEXACOSelf-report Honesty-Humility 3.19 0.57   .35**   .21**   .42**   .16**   .15*   .16**   .11*† .01Emotionality 3.46 0.59   .09   .15** .08   .09   .03   .05 .00   .02Extraversion 3.50 0.55 .41** .40**   .21** .62** .38** .35** .03 .40**Agreeableness 2.92 0.59   .18**   .16**   .16**   .03   .05   .05   .02   .03Conscientiousness 3.49 0.54   .03   .05   .10 .14*   .01   .02   .05 .04Openness 3.35 0.61 .27** .32** .03 .14* .15* .22** .01 .04Peer-report Honesty-Humility 3.19 0.60   .27**   .26**   .12*† .13*   .42**   .31**   .54**   .05Emotionality 3.31 0.63   .09   .13* .01   .02 .02   .05 .11 .00Extraversion 3.52 0.57 .34** .31**   .07 .45** .47** .42**   .11 .64**Agreeableness 3.10 0.65   .19**   .22** .00   .11   .17**   .13   .17**   .06Conscientiousness 3.49 0.57   .09   .16* .06 .01   .11   .09   .26** .10

    Openness 3.08 0.60 .15* .18** .04 .09 .17** .23** .05 .03Peer-report SM Total .42** .42** .03 .34**Acting .45** .48** .03 .33**Other-D .14* .13* .09 .03Extra .23** .21**   .06 .33**Adult work sampleSelf-report SM Total 2.47 0.58   —Honesty-Humility 4.05 0.51   .38***Extraversion 3.52 0.52 .42***

     Note: Student sample:  N = 258; adult work sample:  N = 110. The mean and standard deviation values in parentheses are those obtained for the International

    Personality Item Pool Self-Monitoring Scale used in the adult work sample. Other-D, Other-Directedness; Extra, Extraversion; SM, Self-Monitoring; SM Total,

    total Self-Monitoring Scale score. * p< .05; ** p< .01; *** p< .001. †

    One-tailed.

    Self-Monitoring and Honesty-Humility 537

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    7/14

    The mediating effects of moral disengagement 

    One potential explanation for the proposed link between

    self-monitoring and unethical decision making may be the

    propensity to morally disengage (Bandura, 1986). Bandura 

    (1986) developed the concept of   moral disengagement   to

    explain how people come to engage in detrimental conducts

    that are otherwise incongruent with their moral standards.

    Social cognitive theory proposes that self-management 

    of detrimental conducts occur through cognitive, self-

    regulatory mechanisms (Bandura, 1999). According to

    Bandura and colleagues, people evade self-imposed

    sanctions for engaging in negative conducts by using a num-

    ber of   ‘sociocognitive manoeuvres’   that deactivate normal

    self-regulatory processes, thereby  disengaging   their moral

    standards from detrimental conduct (Bandura, Caprara,

    Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, & Regalia, 2001). Bandura (1999)

    has described eight such mechanisms of moral disengage-

    ment that ultimately enable a person to engage in detrimental

    conduct with little guilt and self-censure. They include

    moral justication (cognitively misconstruing harm toothers so as to seem morally justiable), euphemistic

    labelling (using morally neutral diction to make unethical

    conduct seem less threatening), advantageous comparison

    (making an unethical conduct seem less harmful by compar-

    ing it with more injurious unethical actions), displacement of 

    responsibility (passing culpability for one’s unethical

    conduct to the directives of authority  gures), diffusion of 

    responsibility (abating personal liability for unethical

    conduct when such actions are taken as part of a larger group

    or collective), disregarding or distorting the consequences

    (minimizing, forgetting or even actively discrediting

    evidence of harm caused to others), dehumanization(sanctioning immoral acts by casting victims as subhuman

    or lacking human qualities) and attribution of blame

    (blaming the recipients of harmful behaviours for instigating

    such actions) (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli,

    1996; Detert, Treviño, & Sweitzer, 2008).

    Detert et al. (2008) recently argued that moral disen-

    gagement mechanisms also disassociate   contemplated 

    actions from the guilt or self-censure that would otherwise

    prevent it (p. 378). In particular, moral disengagement 

    mechanisms may operate to minimize guilt feelings associ-

    ated with thinking about engaging in unethical behaviours.

    In support of this, Detert et al. found that moral disengage-

    ment was positively related to unethical decision making.We propose that moral disengagement can help to explain

    the relationship between self-monitoring and unethical

    decision making. The high self-monitor who contemplates

    an unethical course of action in order to gain social success

    may eliminate any guilt or self-censure through moral

    disengagement mechanisms. For instance, the high self-

    monitor may justify the need to make an unethical business

    decision by reasoning that the resulting   nancial success

    will help to achieve a favourable public image, which

    is necessary in order to get ahead in an otherwise

    dif cult and harsh world (i.e. serving the acquisitive self-

    monitoring motive). The high self-monitor may also reasonthat the resulting   nancial success and social status will

    bring about acceptance from others who may otherwise

    reject him or her from their social circles (i.e. serving the

    protective self-monitoring motive). These are examples of 

    the  moral justi cation  mechanism (for instance, a sample

    moral justication item is as follows:   ‘Playing dirty is

    sometimes necessary in order to achieve noble ends’;

    Moore, Detert, Treviño, Baker, & Mayer, 2012). As another 

    example, the high self-monitor may engage in euphemistic

    language strategies to make their unethical intentions sound

    less threatening. For instance, making unethical decisions

    that result in social or   nancial gain may be construed as

    ‘being politically savvy’  or   ‘playing the game’   rather than

    unethical per se.

     Hypothesis 3: The relationship between self-monitoring andunethical decision making will be mediated by moraldisengagement.

    Last, in light of the preceding arguments, we posit that 

    the negative relations of self-monitoring to unethical decisionmaking, and the potential mediating effects of moral disen-

    gagement, will be explained by its low Honesty-Humility

    side but not the extraverted side.

     Hypothesis 4: The link between self-monitoring and unethicaldecision making, and through moral disengagement, will beexplained by Honesty-Humility but not Extraversion.

    Method

    Participants and procedureA total of 215 undergraduate Canadian students enrolled in

    multiple sections of an introductory business course in orga-

    nizational behaviour were recruited as part of a larger project 

    on individual and team characteristics. About 48% of respon-

    dents were female, and the average age was 21.8 years old

    (SD = 3.95). Students were randomly assigned into teams of 

    four to six members and worked on a major class project that 

    accounted for 40% of their  nal class grade. At the start of 

    the semester, students were invited to voluntarily participate

    in a multi-wave research study (at approximately 1-month

    intervals) in exchange for partial course credits. Personality

    questionnaires were administered at the start of the semester 

    (time 1), followed by the moral disengagement measure at time 2. At time 3, participants completed the ethical business

    decision-making scale.

     Measures

    Personality.   As in Study 1, Honesty-Humility and

    Extraversion were measured using the 100-item version of the

    HEXACO PI (Lee & Ashton, 2004). The observed internal

    consistency reliabilities were .82 and .79 for Honesty-Humility

    and Extraversion, respectively.

    Self-monitoring.   Self-monitoring was assessed again using

    the 18-item Self-Monitoring Scale (Snyder & Gangestad,1986)—see the Method section in Study 1. The reliability

    of the total Self-Monitoring Scale was .74; and for the

    538 B. Ogunfowora  et al .

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    8/14

    Acting, Extraversion and Other-Directedness subscales, the

    reliabilities were .69, .49 and .61, respectively.

     Moral disengagement.   Moral disengagement was measured

    using the modied version by Detert et al. (2008) of the 32-

    item scale originally developed by Bandura et al. (1996) for 

    use with children and adolescents. This scale was modied

    for use with an older population. The resulting 24-item scale assesses the eight subcomponents of moral

    disengagement and was shown to possess acceptable

    overall scale reliability (a= .87) and strong initial construct 

    and predictive validity evidence (Detert et al., 2008).

    Responses to the questions were made on a 5-point Likert 

    scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) t o 5 (strongly

    agree). Interested readers may refer to Detert et al. for a 

    complete list of items in this scale.

     Ethical decision making.   Participants completed a revised

    version of the Business Ethical Decision Making Dilemma 

    scale (Lee et al., 2008). This scale contains 10 questions

    that require participants to estimate how likely (1 = veryunlikely   t o 4= very likely) they are to choose a course of 

    action that would yield personal or organizational prots,

    even though it may potentially cause harm to other 

    individuals or to society at large (Lee et al., 2008). The

    scale was scored such that high scores indicated a higher 

    likelihood to make   unethical   decisions. The reliability of 

    this measure was .79.

     Analyses

    The hypotheses were tested using the  PROCESS  procedure

    in SPSS (Hayes, 2013). PROCESS incorporates traditional

    regression analysis tests (Hypotheses 1–2), as well asbootstrapping methodologies for testing indirect effects

    (Hypotheses 3–4). In contrast to traditional signicance test-

    ing, mediation is supported to the extent that the condence

    interval (CI) around the estimated mediated effect does not 

    contain zero (Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007). In the

    present study, 95% bias-corrected CIs were calculated on

    the basis of 10 000 bootstrapped samples. Hypothesis 4 was

    tested using two models. In the   rst model, Extraversion

    was entered as a control variable in the model testing the

    indirect effects of self-monitoring on unethical decision mak-

    ing through moral disengagement. A second model was then

    specied with Honesty-Humility as the control. This allowed

    us to determine which of the two personality traits was

    responsible for the observed effects.

    Results

    Table 2 shows the means, standard deviations and inter-

    correlations among the study variables. The bivariate correla-tions among the hypothesized variables were generally in the

    predicted direction and were statistically signicant. Consistent 

    with the results from Study 1, the two strongest correlates of 

    overall self-monitoring were Extraversion, r = .42, p< .01, and

    Honesty-Humility,   r =.38,   p< .01. In addition, similar to

    Study 1, we found that Honesty-Humility was moderately neg-

    atively related to the Other-Directedness subscale,   r =.32,

     p< .001, but weakly to the Extraversion subscale,   r =.15,

     p< .05, whereas HEXACO Extraversion was strongly and

    positively related to the Extraversion self-monitoring subscale,

    r = .62,   p< .001, but weakly to the Other-Directedness

    subscale, r =

    .11, p>

    .05. Both Extraversion, r = .39, p<

    .001,and Honesty-Humility,   r =.31,  p< .001, were related to the

    Acting subscale. These trends clearly replicate the ndings from 

    Study 1. In support of our hypothesized relationships, self-

    monitoring positively correlated with moral disengagement,

    r = .19, p< .01, and unethical decision making, r = .26, p< .01,

    providing initial support for Hypotheses 1 and 2. The relation-

    ship between moral disengagement and unethical decision

    making was moderate and signicant, r = .47, p< .01.

    Table 3 provides the results obtained from the regression

    and bootstrapping analyses. The regression results showed

    that overall self-monitoring was positively related to moral

    disengagement,   b = .15,   p< .05, providing support for 

    Hypothesis 1. In addition, overall self-monitoring had botha direct positive effect on unethical business decision mak-

    ing,   b = .21,   p< .01, and an indirect effect through moral

    disengagement,   r = 0.08 (CI = 0.002 to 0.16). Hence,

    Hypothesis 2 and 3 were supported. In order to test Hypoth-

    esis 4, the proposed mediation was tested controlling for 

    HEXACO Extraversion. The results showed that self-

    monitoring retained a signicant direct effect on unethical

    decision making, b = .17, p< .05, as well as an indirect effect 

    through moral disengagement,   r = 0.13, CI = 0.05 to 0.23.

    This indicates that the direct and indirect effects of self-

    monitoring on unethical business decision making cannot 

    be explained by HEXACO Extraversion. These analyses

    Table 2. Means, standard deviations and correlations among the study variables (Study 2)

     Mean SD   1 2 3 4 5 6 7

    1 Honesty-Humility 3.25 0.572 Extraversion 3.51 0.45   .123 Moral disengagement 2.22 0.40   .40**   .094 Unethical DM 1.94 0.49   .47** .12 .47**5 SM Acting 2.91 0.63   .31** .39** .19** .21**6 SM Other-Direct 2.95 0.65   .32**   .11 .22** .15* .25**7 SM Extraversion 3.32 0.56   .15* .62**   .06 .21** .41** .048 SM Total 3.03 0.45   .38** .42** .19** .26** .87** .58** .62**

     Note: N = 215. SM, Self-monitoring; SM Total, total Self-Monitoring Scale score; Other Direct, Other-Directedness; Unethical DM, Unethical business decision

    making. * p< .05; ** p< .01.

    Self-Monitoring and Honesty-Humility 539

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    9/14

    were repeated with Honesty-Humility as the control variable.

    The results showed that self-monitoring no longer demon-

    strated a direct effect on unethical decision making,  b = .11,

     p> .05, nor did it have an indirect effect through moral

    disengagement,   r =0.01, CI=

    0.07 to 0.08. These resultssuggest that the direct and indirect effects of self-monitoring

    on unethical business decision making can be explained by

    the low Honesty-Humility aspect of self-monitoring.

    The aforementioned analyses were repeated for each of theself-monitoring subscales. The Acting, Other-Directednessand Extraversion aspects of self-monitoring were all positivelycorrelated with unethical business decision making,   r s=.21,.15 and .21, ps< .05, respectively. However, Acting, r = .19, p.01, and Other-Directedness,  r = .22, p< .01, were related tomoral disengagement but not Extraversion,  r =.06,  p> .05.The direct and indirect effects of these subscales on unethicaldecision making were also tested. The results showed that 

    moral disengagement mediated the effects of Other-Directedness,   r = 0.07, CI= 0.02 to 0.13, and Acting,r = 0.06, CI = 0.01 to 0.11, on unethical business decisionmaking; this mediation effect was, however, not supportedfor the Extraversion subscale,  r =0.07, CI =0.26 to 0.09.In these analyses, Acting,  b = .11, p< .05, and Extraversion,b = .20, p< .01, also demonstrated direct effects on unethicaldecision making, but not Other-Directedness, b = .05, p> .05.This suggests that the effect of Other-Directedness (but not theother subscales) on unethical decision making occurs primarilythrough moral disengagement.

    Similar to the results obtained for the overall Self-

    Monitoring Scale, the direct and indirect effects of Other-

    Directedness and Acting on unethical business decisionmaking through moral disengagement remained unchanged

    after controlling for HEXACO Extraversion. However,

    these effects were no longer supported after controlling for 

    Honesty-Humility. In contrast, the Extraversion subscale

    retained a direct positive effect (b = .16,   p< .01) on

    unethical business decision making, after controlling for 

    Honesty-Humility (even though there was no support for an indirect effect through moral disengagement). This sug-

    gests that the Extraversion subscale shares a portion of its

    variance with unethical business decision making that can-

    not be explained by low Honesty-Humility.

    Brief discussion

    Research studies have found that self-monitoring relates to a 

    number of positive outcomes (e.g. Day et al., 2002;

    Gangestad & Snyder, 2000). In comparison, fewer studies

    have examined the potential detrimental aspects of this con-

    struct. In light of the evidence from Study 1 indicating that 

    self-monitoring has a low Honesty-Humility side, Study 2was carried out to investigate the potential repercussions of 

    this association. The results demonstrated that high self-

    monitors are more likely to make unethical business

    decisions. We also found that the tendency to use moral

    disengagement mechanisms as a way of minimizing feelings

    of guilt associated with unethical decisions is one explana-

    tion for the link between self-monitoring and unethical deci-

    sion making. To date, no research has linked self-monitoring

    to these important ethics-related consequences. Last, the

    results supported our thesis that these negative, ethics-

    related outcomes can be explained by the low Honesty-

    Humility correlate of self-monitoring, rather than its Extra-version correlate. In other words, low Honesty-Humility

    is pertinent to understanding the extent to which high

    Table 3. Direct and indirect effects of self-monitoring on unethical business decision making (Study 2)

    Moral disengagement Unethical business decision making

    b   SE    b   SE    r   Low CI High CI

    Model 1: The direct and indirect effects of self-monitoringSelf-monitoring .15* 0.07 .21** 0.07

    Moral disengagement   — —

      .55*** 0.08 R2 .03* .27***Bootstrap resultsMediator: Moral disengagement    0.08 0.002 0.16Model 2: The direct and indirect effects of self-monitoring: Controlling for ExtraversionSelf-monitoring .22** 0.07 .17* 0.08Extraversion   .20** 0.07 .12 0.08Moral disengagement    — —   .58*** 0.08 R

    2 .07** .28***Bootstrap resultsMediator: Moral disengagement    0.13 0.05 0.23Model 3: The direct and indirect effects of self-monitoring: Controlling for Honesty-HumilitySelf-monitoring .01 0.06 .11 0.07Honesty-Humility   .29*** 0.05   .26*** 0.06Moral disengagement    — —   .42*** 0.08

     R

    2

    .17*** .33***Bootstrap resultsMediator: Moral disengagement 0.01   0.07 0.08

     Note: N = 183. r  represents the mediation effect. CI, condence interval (95% bias-corrected and accelerated) based on 10,000 bootstrap samples. Boldface type

    highlights a signicant effect as determined by the exclusion of zero from the 95% CI. * p< .05; ** p< .01, *** p< .001.

    540 B. Ogunfowora  et al .

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    10/14

    self-monitors engage in morally questionable cognitive

    processes and decisions.5

    Further investigations into the self-monitoring subscales

    provided additional support for our propositions. The results

    revealed similar mediatory effects of moral disengagement 

    on the links between Acting and Other-Directedness and

    unethical business decision making. This was, however, not 

    the case for the Extraversion subscale. Although the Extra-

    version subscale was related to unethical decision making,

    most likely because of its shared variance with the other 

    components of self-monitoring, the use of moral disengage-

    ment tactics may be more readily attributable to those behav-

    iours that fall within the Acting and Other-Directedness

    domain. Specically, compared with the Extraversion

    subscale, the Acting and Other-Directedness behaviours

    may be more closely associated with a tendency towards

    deception through image control (particularly Other-

    Directedness) and, as such, may be more likely serviced by

    moral disengagement tactics. It is therefore not surprising

    that low Honesty-Humility was again implicated inexplaining the links between these self-monitoring subscales

    and unethical business decision making through moral

    disengagement. In sum, analyses of the self-monitoring

    subscales revealed differential patterns of relationships and

    support the notion that these subscales should be investigated

    independently. However, we encourage readers to exercise

    caution in the interpretation of these  ndings given the lower 

    levels of reliability associated with these subscales. This

    issue is not unique to the present study, however, as past 

    studies have found similarly low reliabilities for these

    subscales (e.g. Hoyle & Lennox, 1991).

    GENERAL DISCUSSION

    The present multi-study research was designed to shed light 

    on the dual nature of the self-monitoring construct. On the

    basis of past research, we proposed that self-monitoring re-

    lates primarily and most consistently to two important major 

    personality dimensions: Extraversion and (low) Honesty-

    Humility.6 In light of the extensive research that has been

    conducted on Extraversion as a correlate of self-monitoring,

    this paper sought to establish the consequences of the low

    Honesty-Humility side of self-monitoring. A number of 

    important   ndings emerged from this investigation. First,

    across multiple samples, we found consistent evidence that 

    Extraversion was the strongest correlate of self-monitoring

    (as measured by the 18-item Self-Monitoring Scale;

    Snyder & Gangestad, 1986), followed closely by low

    Honesty-Humility. This association generalized across dif-

    ferent rating sources and different samples, including an

    adult work sample. These   ndings replicate Ashton and

    Lee’s (2005) initial observation of a negative association

    between Honesty-Humility and self-monitoring and

    provide strong evidence of the dual nature of self-

    monitoring. Second, Study 2 showed that self-monitoring

    and its three subscales were positively related to unethical

    business decision making. In addition, overall self-

    monitoring—as well as the Acting and Other-Directedness

    components—also demonstrated an indirect effect on

    unethical business decision making through moral disen-gagement. Additionally, we showed that these negative

    outcomes of self-monitoring were primarily due to the

    low Honesty-Humility side of self-monitoring but not 

    the extraverted side. As noted earlier, past research has

    found that high self-monitors are more likely to engage

    in impression management, to condone deceit in order to

    gain favours, as well as engage in deception in romantic

    relationships (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000; Mcleod &

    Genereux, 2008; Turnley & Bolino, 2001). More recently,

    Grieves (2011) found that high self-monitoring was

    positively associated with self-reported tendencies to engage

    in emotional manipulation. Not surprisingly, these same im-pression management and emotional manipulation tactics have

    been associated with low Honesty-Humility (e.g. Bourdage

    et al., 2012, 2009; Grieves, 2011). Taken together, the

    results of the present research more clearly and directly dem-

    onstrate the role of low Honesty-Humility in understanding

    the negative effects of self-monitoring on two previously

    unexamined ethics-related outcomes.

    Implications and future research

    High self-monitoring has been associated with success in

    different domains of social life and, as such, is often treated

    as an adaptive construct. The present research, however,shows that high self-monitors are also likely to entertain

    unethical cognitions and decisions. This may be problematic

    in the workplace, for instance, where high self-monitors are

    likely to be viewed positively by others and promoted to po-

    sitions of authority (Day et al., 2002). Moreover, there are

    likely to be other unfavourable outcomes that may result 

    from high self-monitors’  propensities to morally disengage.

    Bandura et al. (1996) have also shown that children and ad-

    olescents who engage in moral disengagement are less likely

    to engage in prosocial behaviours and more likely to be

    prone to aggression. A recent multi-study investigation

    (Moore et al., 2012) found that individuals who have a higher propensity to morally disengage are more likely to

    engage in a number of unethical behaviours, including

    5As noted by a reviewer, it is also possible that the relationship between self-

    monitoring and these negative outcomes is   moderated   by an individual’slevel of Honesty-Humility (or Extraversion). We however found no empiri-cal support for the moderating effects of Honesty-Humility or Extraversionon the relationships between self-monitoring (or its subscales) and unethicalbusiness decision making and moral disengagement. We also tested a morecomprehensive moderated mediation model (using PROCESS) where the di-rect and indirect effects of self-monitoring on unethical decision making,through moral disengagement, were conditional on the Honesty-Humility(or Extraversion) levels of the participants. Again, the results did not yieldsupport for these models.6At the suggestion of a reviewer, we investigated whether investigatingHEXACO personality factors at the facet level in association with self-monitoring could give any further insight as to the nature of these relation-ships. Across six sets of correlations (four within Study 1 Sample 1, onewithin Study 1 Sample 2, and one for the Study 2 Sample), facet level anal-yses indicated that all four H-H facets related similarly to self-monitoring.On the other hand, whereas all Extraversion facets had moderate relation-ships with self-monitoring, the facet of Social Boldness (which assessesone’s comfort or condence within a variety of social situations) had theconsistently strongest relationship with self-monitoring.

    Self-Monitoring and Honesty-Humility 541

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    11/14

    stealing, lying and cheating. These individuals showed a 

    greater tendency to engage in fraudulent activity and made

    more self-serving decisions that place others at jeopardy.

    Moreover, self-monitoring also demonstrated a direct 

    effect on ethical business decision making. This suggests that 

    there may be other explanatory mechanisms for understand-

    ing why high self-monitors make unethical decisions that 

    should be explored in the future. In addition, future studies

    should also be devoted to investigating other implications

    of the low Honesty-Humility nature of self-monitoring. For 

    instance, Honesty-Humility is a strong predictor of delin-

    quency, antisocial behaviours, sexual harassment proclivity

    and cheating behaviours (Hersheld, Cohen, & Thompson,

    2012; Lee, Ashton, & De Vries, 2005; Lee et al., 2003;

    O’Neill, Lewis, & Carswell, 2011). As such, future studies

    should examine the potential links between self-monitoring

    and similar negative outcomes.

    Similar to past research (Cheek, 1982; Wymer & Penner,

    1985), we observed a moderate relationship between self-

    ratings and peer ratings of self-monitoring in Study 1. Past research has further shown that the magnitude of this self-

    peer agreement is roughly the same for high self-monitors

    as it is for low self-monitors (Gangestad & Snyder,

    2000). As noted by a reviewer, this observation is some-

    what disconcerting because if those who rate themselves

    as high self-monitors are readily judged by their peers as

    high self-monitors, then the objective of self-monitoring

    (convincingly presenting differing   ‘favourable’   images to

    others) would have failed. This raises the question of 

    whether there are individual differences in the ability to

    be an effective high self-monitor. For instance, past 

    research has shown that the effective use of impressionmanagement tactics—a set of behaviours related to self-

    monitoring—depends on the political skills of the impres-

    sion manager (e.g. Harris, Kacmar, Zivnuska, & Shaw,

    2007). A review of the self-monitoring subscales, however,

    showed moderate correlations between self-ratings and

    peer ratings of Extraversion (r = .33) and Acting (r = .48),

    but not Other-Directedness (r = .09). This suggests that,

    unlike the other sub-facets, participants are unable to judge

    the Other-Directedness of their peers. This is worth noting

    because Other-Directedness embodies the fundamental,

    chameleon-like attribute of the high self-monitor —that is,

    it describes the extent to which the individual puts on

    different favourable facades to different people. The resultsindicate that perhaps peers are not capable of judging this

    pertinent aspect of high self-monitors after all. Future

    research is, however, necessary to replicate these   ndings

    before substantive conclusions are drawn, particularly given

    the lower reliabilities of the self-monitoring subscales in the

    present study.

    Limitations

    The present research has some limitations that warrant 

    consideration. First, we generally observed low internal

    consistency reliability estimates for the subscales of theSelf-Monitoring Scale. Although these reliability estimates

    are similar to those that have been reported in past research

    (e.g. Hoyle & Lennox, 1991), the low reliability of these

    subscales may have had negative effects on the observed

    relationships reported in this study. Another limitation is that 

    the   ndings in Study 2 were based on same source data,

    which may limit the inferences made from this study. To

    minimize the effects of same source biases, Study 2 incorpo-

    rated a lagged design that separated measures of the indepen-

    dent, mediator and outcome variables by time (i.e. across

    three periods). This does not, however, alleviate concerns

    regarding the external validity of the   ndings obtained in

    Study 2. In particular, this study found a link between self-

    monitoring and unethical business decision making by using

    a sample of undergraduate business students. The generaliz-

    ability of this association to decision making in the work-

    place, however, needs to be established. Last, we should

    note that although we focused on two major personality

    factors that appear to underlie self-monitoring (Extraversion

    and low Honesty-Humility), the self-monitoring construct is

    likely more than just a combination of these two personality

    variables. For instance, we conducted post hoc regressionanalyses relating the HEXACO personality variables as a 

    set to the overall Self-Monitoring Scale. The results indicate

    that, as a whole, the HEXACO model of personality does not 

    fully account for the variance in self-monitoring (for Study 1,

    the average   R   between the six HEXACO factors and self-

    monitoring is .52). Thus, although self-monitoring cannot 

    be completely understood within the HEXACO framework,

    the HEXACO model does provide an avenue for understand-

    ing the dishonest side of this important construct.

    Conclusion

    In sum, the present research illustrates that the self-monitoring

    construct has both high Extraversion and low Honesty-

    Humility aspects. An exploration of the low Honesty-Humility

    side provided valuable insight into previously unexamined,

    negative consequences of self-monitoring. It is our hope that this

    research will encourage future work on the implications of the

    dishonest side of self-monitoring across different domains of 

    social life.

    REFERENCES

    Arkin, R. M. (1981). Self-presentation styles. In J. T. Tedeschi(Ed.),   Impression management theory and social psychological theory  (pp. 311–333). New York: Academic Press.

    Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-Humility, the Big Five, andthe  ve-factor model.  Journal of Personality,  73(5), 1321–1353.

    Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practi-cal advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure.Personality and Social Psychology Review,  11, 150–166.

    Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2008). The prediction of Honesty-Humility-related criteria by the HEXACO and  ve-factor modelsof personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1216–1228.

    Bandura, A. (1986).   Social foundations of thought and action: Asocial cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhu-manities.  Personality & Social Psychology Review, 3, 193–209.

    Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, B., & Pastorelli, C. (1996).Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moralagency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 364–374.

    542 B. Ogunfowora  et al .

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    12/14

    Bandura, A., Caprara, B., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & Regalia, C.(2001). Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing trans-gressive behavior.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,80, 125–135.

    Barrick, M. R., Parks, L., & Mount, M. K. (2005). Self-monitoringas a moderator of the relationships between personality traits andperformance. Personnel Psychology,  58, 745–767.

    Blakely, G. L., Andrews, M. C., & Fuller, J. (2003). Are chame-

    leons good citizens? A longitudinal study of the relationshipbetween self-monitoring and organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Business and Psychology,  18(2), 131–144.

    Bono, J. E., & Vey, M. A. (2007). Personality and emotional perfor-mance: Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Self-Monitoring.  Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,  12(2), 177–192.

    Bourdage, J. S., Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., & Perry, A. (2007). BigFive and HEXACO model personality correlates of sexuality.Personality and Individual Differences,  43, 1506–1516.

    Bourdage, J. S., Lee, J., Lee, K., & Shin, H. (2012). Motives for organizational citizenship behavior: Personality correlates andco-worker ratings of OCB.   Human Performance,  25, 179–200.

    Bourdage, J., Lee, K., & Ogunfowora, B. (2009).  Antecedents and consequences of impression management: A   eld study. 24thAnnual Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology

    (SIOP) conference. New Orleans, Louisiana: United States.Briggs, S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1988). On the nature of self-

    monitoring: Problems with assessment, problems with validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  54, 663–678.

    Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discrimi-nant validation by the multitrait –multimethod matrix. Psycholog-ical Bulletin,  56 , 81–105.

    Cheek, J. M. (1982). Aggregation, moderator variables, and thevalidity of personality tests: A peer rating study.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  43, 1254–1269.

    Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1991).  NEO ve-factor inventory.Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Cronshaw, S. F., & Ellis, R. J. (1991). A process investigation of self-monitoring and leader emergence.   Small Group Research,22(4), 403–420.

    Day, D. V., Schleicher, D. J., Unckless, A. L., & Hiller, N. J. (2002).Self-monitoring personality at work: A meta-analytic investigationof construct validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87 , 390–401.

    Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., & Sweitzer, V. L. (2008). Moral disen-gagement in ethical decision making: A study of antecedents andoutcomes.  Journal of Applied Psychology,  93, 374–391.

    Dunlop, P. D., Morrison, D. L., Koenig, J., & Silcox, B. (2012). Compar-ing the Eysenck and HEXACO model of personality in the predictionof adult delinquency. European Journal of Personality, 26 , 194–202.

    Ellis, R. J. (1988). Self-monitoring and leadership emergence ingroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 681–693.

    Gangestad, S., & Snyder, M. (1985).   “To carve nature at its joints”:On the existence of discrete classes in personality.   Psychological  Review,  92(3), 317–349.

    Gangestad, S. W., & Snyder, M. (2000). Self-monitoring: Appraisal

    and reappraisal.  Psychological Bulletin,  126 , 530–555.Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M.

    C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The InternationalPersonality Item Pool and the future of public-domain personalitymeasures.  Journal of Research in Personality,  40, 84–96.

    Grieve, R. (2011). Mirror mirror: The role of self-monitoring andsincerity in emotional manipulation.  Personality and Individual  Differences,  51, 981–985.

    Harris, K. J., Kacmar, K. M., Zivnuska, S., & Shaw, J. D. (2007).The impact of political skill on impression management effective-ness.  Journal of Applied Psychology,  92, 278–285.

    Hayes, A. F. (2013).   Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach.New York: Guilford Press.

    Hersheld, H. E., Cohen, T. R., & Thompson, L. (2012). Short horizons

    and tempting situations: Lack of continuity to our future selves leadsto unethical decision making and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117 , 298–310.

    Higgins, C. A., & Judge, T. A. (2004). The effect of applicant inuence tactics on recruiter perceptions of   t and hiring rec-ommendations: A   eld study.  Journal of Applied Psychology,89, 622–632.

    Hofstee, W. K. (1994). Who should own the denition of personal-ity? European Journal of Personality,  8(3), 149–162.

    Hoyle, R. H., & Lennox, R. D. (1991). Latent structure of self-monitoring. Multivariate Behavioral Research,  26 , 511–540.

    John, O. P., Cheek, J. M., & Klohnen, E. C. (1996). On the nature of self-monitoring: Construct explication via Q-sort ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  71, 763–776.

    Kilduf, M., & Day, D. V. (1994). Do chameleons get ahead? Theeffects of self-monitoring on managerial careers.   Academy of  Management Journal ,  37 (4), 1047–1061.

    Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of theHEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research,39, 329–358.

    Lee, K., & Ashton, M.C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism,and narcissism in the  ve-factor model and HEXACO model of Personality Structure.   Personality and Individual Differences,38, 1571–1582.

    Lee, K., Ashton, M., & De Vries, R. E. (2005). Predicting workplacedelinquency and integrity with the HEXACO andve-factor modelsof personality structure.  Human Performance, 18(2), 179–197.

    Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Morrison, D. L., Cordery, J., & Dunlop, P.D. (2008). Predicting integrity with the HEXACO personalitymodel: Use of self- and observer reports.   Journal of Occupa-tional and Organizational Psychology,  81, 147–167.

    Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., & Shin, K. H. (2005). Personality correlatesof workplace anti-social behavior.  Applied Psychology: An Inter-national Review,  54(1), 81–98.

    Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Wiltshire, J., Bourdage, J. S., Visser, B. A.,& Gallucci, A. (2012). Sex, power, and money: Prediction from the Dark Triad and Honesty–Humility.   European Journal of Personality,  27 , 169–184. DOI: 10.1002/per.1860

    Lee, K., Gizzarone, M., & Ashton, M. C. (2003). Personality andthe likelihood to sexually harass.  Sex Roles,  49, 59–69.

    Lee, K., Ogunfowora, B., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Personality traitsbeyond the Big Five: Are they within the HEXACO space? Journal of Personality,  73, 1437–1463.

    Lennox, R. D., & Wolfe, R. N. (1984). Revision of the self-monitoringscale.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,   46 ,1349–1364.

    Marcus, B., Lee, K., & Ashton, M. (2007). Personality dimensionsexplaining relationships between integrity tests and counterpro-ductive behavior: Big Five, or one in addition?   Personnel Psychology,  60, 1–34.

    McLeod, B. A., & Genereux, R. L. (2008). Predicting the accept-ability and likelihood of lying: The interaction of personality withtype of lie.  Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 591–596.

    Moore, C., Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., Baker, V. L., & Mayer, D. M.(2012). Why employees do bad things: Moral disengagement and

    unethical organizational behavior. Personnel Psychology, 65, 1–

    48.Oh, I.-S., Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., & De Vries, R. E. (2011). Are

    dishonest extraverts more harmful than dishonest introverts?The interaction effects of Honesty-Humility and Extraversion inpredicting workplace deviance.  Applied Psychology: An International  Review, 60, 496–516.

    Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer ratings of the   ve-factor model of personality traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology,  96 , 762–773.

    O’Neill, T. A., Lewis, R. J., & Carswell, J. J. (2011). Employeepersonality, justice perceptions, and the prediction of work-place deviance.   Personality and Individual Differences,   51,595–600.

    Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. (2002). The Dark Triad of personal-ity: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy.  Journal of 

     Research in Personality,  36 , 556–568.Perugini, M., & Leone, L. (2009). Implicit self-concept and moral

    action. Journal of Research in Personality,  43, 747–754.

    Self-Monitoring and Honesty-Humility 543

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    13/14

    Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressingmoderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescrip-tions. Multivariate Behavioral Research,  42, 185–227.

    Rauthmann, J. F. (2011). Acquisitive or protective self-presentation of dark personalities? Associations among the Dark Triad and self-monitoring.  Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 502–508.

    Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  30, 526–537.

    Snyder, M. (1987).   Public appearances, private realities: The psychology of self-monitoring. New York: Freeman.

    Snyder, M., & Gangestad, S. (1986). On the nature of self-monitoring:Matters of assessment, matters of validity.  Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 51, 125–139.

    Tett, R. P., Fox, K. E., & Wang, A. (2005). Development and valida-tion of a self-report measure of emotional intelligence as a multidimensional trait domain.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1–30.

    Thorndike, R. (2007). Fisher ’s z transformation. In N. J. Salkind (Ed.), Encylopedia of measurement and statistics: Vol 1  (pp. 361–365).Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.

    Turnley, W. H., & Bolino, M. C. (2001). Achieving desired imageswhile avoiding undesired images: Exploring the role of self-monitoring in impression management.   Journal of Applied Psychology,  86 , 351–360.

    Van Gelder J. L., & De Vries, R. E. (2012). Traits and states:

    Integrating personality and affect into a model of criminaldecision making.   Criminology,  50, 637–671.

    Wolfe, R. N., Lennox, R. D., & Cutler, B. L. (1986). Getting alongand getting ahead: Empirical support for a theory of protectiveand acquisitive self-presentation.   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  50(2), 356–361.

    Wymer, W. E., & Penner, L. A. (1985). Moderator variables anddifferent types of predictability: Do you have a match?   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,  49, 1002–1015.

    544 B. Ogunfowora  et al .

    Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.   Eur. J. Pers. 27: 532–544 (2013)

    DOI: 10.1002/per 

  • 8/17/2019 An Exploration of the Dishonest Side of Self-Monitoring Links to Moral disengagement

    14/14

    C o p y r i g h t o f E u r o p e a n J o u r n a l o f P e r s o n a l i t y i s t h e p r o p e r t y o f J o h n W i l e y & S o n s , I n c . a n d      

    i t s c o n t e n t m a y n o t b e c o p i e d o r e m a i l e d t o m u l t i p l e s i t e s o r p o s t e d t o a l i s t s e r v w i t h o u t t h e      

    c o p y r i g h t h o l d e r ' s e x p r e s s w r i t t e n p e r m i s s i o n . H o w e v e r , u s e r s m a y p r i n t , d o w n l o a d , o r e m a i l    

    a r t i c l e s f o r i n d i v i d u a l u s e .