AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

282
MODELING CONSUMER RESPONSES TO NEGATIVE DISCONFIRMATION OF EXPECTATIONS: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE THEORY BASED MEASURES by JAGDIP SINGH, B. Tech. A DISSERTATION IN BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Texas Tech Univereity in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION ^ppro^^d Accepted August, 1965

Transcript of AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

Page 1: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

MODELING CONSUMER RESPONSES TO NEGATIVE

DISCONFIRMATION OF EXPECTATIONS:

AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING

ITEM RESPONSE THEORY

BASED MEASURES

by

JAGDIP SINGH, B. Tech.

A DISSERTATION

IN

BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION

Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Texas Tech Univereity in

Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

the Degree of

DOCTOR OF BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION

^ppro^^d

Accepted

August, 1965

Page 2: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

C.e>p, ^ - ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

A dissertation is a mosiac of contributions from

several people; without their cooperation and

unhesitating assistance the task would be impossible.

This acknowledgment commits to paper the writer's

gratitude and debt to all those who assisted in the

research.

The writer especially acknowledges the guidance and

encouragement of Dr. Roy D. Howell, Chairman of the

Committee. Dr. Howell remained a source of inspiration

not only during the dissertation process, but throughout

the doctoral program. To him the author owes a debt of

gratitude which can never be repaid.

The financial and moral support of Mr. Jan

Freiderich, Chief Executive Officer, Mr. Edward M.

Markham, Director of Management Information Systems, Axel

Hopp, and Kathy Komoll, all of Furrs Supermarkets

Incorporated, is sincerely appreciated. In particular,

the writer thanks Mr. Markham for his encouragement and

the many hours he patiently spent in providing valuable

managerial insights.

Special acknowledgments are also due to Dr. Danny

Bellenger, Dr. Robert Wilkes, Dr. James Wilcox and other

members of the marketing faculty. The author deeply

appreciates the valuable time that they unhesitatingly

ii

Page 3: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

devoted in guiding this research.

A very special acknowledgment is due to the writer's

family, Neena and Tashi. Their love and affection made

this task much more meaningful and easier. It is to them

that this work is dedicated.

Finally, the author also wishes to recognize the

unfailing support and friendship of doctoral colleagues

especially, Phil Goodell and Gary Rhoads.

iii

Page 4: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ii

LIST OF FIGURES vii

LIST OF TABLES ix

1. INTRODUCTION 1

Substantive Contribution 3

Methodological Contribution 6

The Choice of Service Industries 8

Outline of the Dissertation 9

2. A REVIEW OF THE CONCEPTUAL AND EMPIRICAL

RESEARCH 11

Conceptual Review 11

Empirical Review 30

3. THE CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF A HOLISTIC

MODEL OF CCB 46

Conceptual Foundations of the Model. . . . 46

Partial Formalization of the Model . . . . 54

4. 0PERATI0NALI2ATI0N AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF

KEY HYPOTHESES 66

Operationalizations of Key Constructs. . . 68

Key Hypotheses 7d

iv

Page 5: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

5. SURVEY PROCEDURES AND METHODS 92

The Two Phases of Research 92

Phase 1 93

Phase II 97

6. PHASE I RESULTS 105

Measurement Properties Using Traditional Methods 106

Structural Relationships Using Path Analysis 108

Measurement Properties Using IRT Based Procedures 110

Structural Relationships With

Reduced Scales 118

7. PHASE II RESULTS 137

Measurement Properties 137

Empirical Investigation of the Typology for the Predominant Predictor of CCB Intentions (Hypotheses H6-H8) . . . . 141

Empirical Investigation of the Framework for Predicting Specific CCB Responses (Hypotheses H14-H17). . . . 147

Process Model Versus Naive Model (Hypothesis H13) 155

Empirical Investigation of the Process Model (Hypotheses H4, H5 and H9-H12) 159

Empirical Investigation of Expectancy Value Judgments in the Four Industries (Hypothesis H18) 172

Page 6: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

a. SUMMARY, IMPLICATIONS AND LIMITATIONS 197

An Overview of the Dissertation 197

Managerial Implications 207

Public Policy Implications 214

Theoretical Implications 216

Limitations 218

LIST OF REFERENCES 226

APPENDICES 237

vi

Page 7: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

LIST OF FIGURES

2.1 Two Competing Conceptualizations of Consumer Complaint Behavior 43

2.2 A Process Model for Post-Purchase Phenomena . . 44

2.3 A Classification Schema for Consumer Complaints 45

3.1 A Holistic Model of Consumer Complaining Behavior 63

3.2 A Typology for the Nature of CCB Processes 64

3.3 A Model for Predicting Specific Consumer Complaint Actions 65

6.1 The Empirical Model Proposed to be Tested in Phase I 124

6.2 Eigenvalue Structure for the 35 Item Alienation Scale 125

6. 3 Eigenvalue Structure of the 82 Item Discontent Scale 126

6. 4 Path Analytical Diagram for Complete Scales 127

6. 5 Eigenvalue Structure for the Combined Pool of Discontent and Alienation Items . . . . 128

6.6 Scale Information Curves for Phase I Complete Scales 129

6.7 Scale Information Curves for Phase I

Shortened Scales 130

6.8 Plot of "b" Parameters from Two Studies . . . . 131

6.9 Scale Information Curves for Phase II Scales 132

6.10 Path Analytical Diagram Using Shortened Scales 133

vxi

Page 8: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

6. 11 Path Analytical Diagram Based on Automotive Repair Data 134

6. 12 Path Analytical Diagram for the Discontent Group 135

6. 13 Path Analytical Diagram for the

Alienated Group 136

7. 1 A Naive Model of CCB Intentions 191

7. 2 A Process Model of CCB Intentions 192

7. 3 The Estimated Model of CCB Intentions for Grocery Data 193

7. 4 The Estimated Model of CCB Intentions for Automotive Repair Data 194

7. 5 The Estimated Model of CCB Intentions for Medical Care Data 195

7.6 The Estimated Model of CCB Intentions for Financial Data 196

viii

Page 9: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

LIST OF TABLES

2.1 A Summary of Empirical Findings in the CCB Area 40

4.1 Operationalization of Key Constructs

in the Holistic Model 91

5. 1 Response Rates for Phase II Surveys 102

5.2 Chi-square Test for "No Problem" Respondents 103

6.1 Correlation Matrix for Discontent, Alienation and Attitude 122

6.2 "A" and "B" Parameters for the Discontent

and Alienation Scales 123

7. 1 Alpha Reliabilities of All Constructs 176

7.2 Partial Correlation Table for Grocery Data 177

7. 3 Partial Correlation Table for Automotive Repair Data 178

7.4 Partial Correlation Table for Medical Care Data 179

7.5 Partial Correlation Table for Financial

Data 180

7.6 Cell Means for "VOICE" Intentions 181

7.7 Cell Means for "W-O-M" Intentions 182

7.8 Cell Means for "FORMAL" Intentions 183 7.9 A Comparison of Naive and Process Models

for Grocery Data 184

7.10 A Comparison of the Naive and Process Models for Automotive Repair Data 185

7.11 A Comparison of Naive and Process Models for Medical Care Data 186

ix

Page 10: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

7. 12 A Comparison of Naive and Process Models for Financial Data 187

7.13 Estimated Parameters for the Process Model: Maximum Likelihood Structural Parameters 188

7.14 Estimated Parameters for the Process Model: Standardized Measurement Parameters . . . . 189

7. 15 A Comparison of Expectancy Value Judgments Across the Four Industries 190

8.1 A Summary of the Various Hypotheses Tested in Phase 1 220

8.2 A Summary of the Various Hypothesis Tested in Phase II 221

8.3 Typical Verbatim Responses 223

Page 11: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Much of the research in consumer behavior is focused

on pre-purchase processes (e.g., information search,

beliefs, formation of attitudes, purchase preferences and

intentions). More recently, the importance of post-

purchase processes and their influence on consumer beha­

vior in general and repurchase activity in particular has

been recognized. For example, the annual Conference on

Consumer Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction (CS/D) first

held in 1976, has now been broadened to include Consumer

Complaining Behavior (CCB) as well.

Post-purchase activity often involves a series of

steps in which consumers evaluate the perceived perfor­

mance of a product against an expected level (or norm) of

performance and then act in a way influenced by the

resulting congruence or discrepancy (Gilly and Gelb 1982;

Woodruff et al. 1983; Bearden and Teel 1983; Oliver

1980). Several theoretical models have been proposed to

guide empirical investigation of the post-purchase

process (Andreasen 1977; Day 1984). In all models, the

overall feeling of satisfaction or dissatisfaction that

results from the post-purchase process is hypothesized to

affect repurchase beliefs, attitudes, intentions and

Page 12: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

loyalty (Andreasen 1977; Engel and Blackwell 1982). From

a theoretical standpoint, the study of post-purchase

process provides increased understanding of the role of

prior experiences (previous satisfactions or dissatisfac­

tions) in future purchase decisions. Additionally, it

affords the manager a richer understanding of the psycho­

logical processes leading to brand loyalty over repeated

purchases.

The major objective of this dissertation is to

develop a theoretical framework for consumer dissatisfac­

tion and complaint processes, and to empirically test a

portion of that framework. It is expected that a model

for the explanation and prediction of these post-purchase

processes which is supported by empirical observations

would provide a contribution to theoretical understanding

of consumer behavior as well as provide guidelines to

practicing managers for retaining the loyalty of their

customers. Further, empirical investigation is conducted

in four separate industries in order to test the validity

of the model in different situations and for a range of

products or services. Specifically, the proposed model

is tested for dissatisfactions involving grocery

shopping, automotive repair, medical care and financial

services. In order to delimit the scope and extent of

this dissertation, the specific substantive and methodo­

logical contributions as well as the reasons behind the

Page 13: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

choice of the four industries (for empirical investi­

gation) are discussed.

Substantive Contribution

Research in the area of post-purchase processes has

followed two somewhat different directions. One is

widely referred to as the confirmation/disconfirmation of

expectations paradigm for explaining consumer satisfac­

tion/dissatisfaction (CS/D). This paradigm asserts that:

Prior to purchase and use of a brand, the consumer forms expectations of its performance in a particular use situation. These expectations are predictions of the nature and level of performance the user will receive. After using the brand, the consumer compares perceived actual performance with expected performance. Confirmation results when the two performances match. A mis-match will cause a positive (perceived performance exceeds expectations) or a negative (perceived performance falls below expectations) disconfirmation. In turn, confirmation/disconfirmation leads to an emotional reaction called satisfaction/dissatisfaction (Woodruff et al. 1983).

Many theoretical frameworks have been suggested to

explain the process of confirmation/disconfirmation and

its relationship to CS/D. Early researchers in marketing

employed the cognitive dissonance theory suggesting that

consumers seek to reduce dissonance between expectations

and performance (Howard and Sheth 1969; Olson and Dover

1976). Lack of empirical support for this theory led

researchers to adopt the assimilation contrast theory as

a possible mechanism for the CS/D process (Anderson 1973;

Olshavsky and Miller 1972). Using the notion of latitude

Page 14: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

of acceptance, this theory predicted an interaction

between the level of expectations and degree of discon-

firmation. These predicted effects, however, have had

little empirical support (Oliver 1980). Current research

tends to postulate independent effects of expectation

level and disconfirmation on the level of consumer satis­

faction. Such a conceptualization is grounded in the

theories of comparison level (Latour and Peat 1979) and

adaptation level (Oliver 1980a). Each of the above

frameworks assumes that confirmation or positive discon-

firmation of the performance increases, while negative

disconfirmation of the performance decreases, the likeli­

hood of brand repurchase.

The second stream of research, often referred to as

the Consumer Complaining Behavior (CCB), has sought to

understand, explain and predict actions following

unsatisfactory purchase or use experiences. Though much

of this work is descriptive, several studies show that

disconfirmation of expectations triggers a complaint

process (Bearden and Teel 1983; Richins 1982; Jacoby and

Jaccard 1981).

Several theoretical frameworks have been proposed to

explain the consumer complaint process--that is, when

would dissatisfied consumers seek redress, register

complaints, change future behavior (e.g., by withdrawing

patronage to the brand) or do nothing at all? Richins

Page 15: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

(1979) suggests a psychological framework of

attitudes >intentions >behavior as the basis for

explaining the complaint process. Krishnan and Valle

(1979) propose an attributional theory perspective based

on attributions of dissatisfaction as the foundation for

understanding the CCB. Another framework based on

economic theory purports that consumers are rational

processors of information and specific complaint actions

are determined by expectancy-value Judgments that are

attached to each alternative course of complaint action

(Hirschman 1970). Althogh a clear understanding concern­

ing the appropriate perspective in any given situation or

a framework that ties together the three theoretical

perspectives is lacking (Day et al. 1931), it seems clear

that retailer/manufacturer responses to specific consumer

complaint actions result in a final or overall feeling of

satisfaction/dissatisfaction with a purchase incident.

The CCB paradigm thus proposes that consumer's

satisfaction/dissatisfaction with the handling of his/her

complaint actions influences the likelihood of repur­

chase. The disconfirmation of performance expectations

is, according to this paradigm, a necessary but not

sufficient condition for determining repurchase inten­

tions (Andreasen 1977).

If these two dimensions of the post-purchase pheno­

mena can be treated within a single framework, the

Page 16: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

resulting synthesis would encourage and guide theoretical

as well as empirical research in the area. The consid­

eration of how CS/D and CCB can be integrated into a

holistic model of post-purchase processes forms the major

objective of this dissertation. A research design is

presented which empirically tests a part of the proposed

holistic model of the post-purchase process in four key

service industries.

Methodological Contribution

From a methodological standpoint, this dissertation

research is one of the few attempts in the marketing

literature to use Item Response Theory (IRT) based

techniques as the basic tool for estimation of construct

reliability and validity. IRT is a measurement theory

(of. classical test theory), in that it sets down rules

for converting empirical observations into corresponding

theoretical constructs. IRT belongs to the latent trait

class of theories which explicitly specify the form of

the response curve for each item in the scale using

"item" as well as "person" parameters. IRT based tech­

niques use this response specification in assessing

measurements of underlying constructs that are both

"sample-free" (not affected by the specific set of

respondents to whom the questionnaire is administered) as

well as "test free" (not affected by the specific set of

items used to measure the construct). This is in

Page 17: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

contrast to classical test (or true score) theory which

guarantees only "sample-free" measurement and does not

account for "test" effects. IRT is well grounded in

statistical theory and has been rigorously developed by

researchers in the area of psychology and educational

psychology (Lord 1980; Brinbaum 1968; Hulin, Drasgow and

Miller 1983). IRT has not been embraced by marketing and

the classical test theory remains, despite its well docu­

mented limitations, as the predominant paradigm of meas­

urement in the marketing literature.

In this research, IRT is used to provide estimates

of construct reliability in terms of the standard error

of measurement defined for every level of the underlying

construct. This is a substantial improvement over the

global estimates of the lower bound of reliability, inde­

pendent of the level of the construct being measured,

provided by the classical test theory (example: coeffi­

cient alpha). Further, IRT provides item and test infor­

mation curves as a function of the level of the under­

lying construct. These information curves are proposed

to be used in the present research to achieve reliability

and parsimony in the various scales used as operational

measures of the constructs in the holistic model of post-

purchase process. Next, the specific service industries

selected for empirical investigation are discussed.

Page 18: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

8

The Choice of Service Industries

This research investigates dissatisfaction and

complaint behavior in four industries: (a) auto repair,

(b) health services, (c) banks and financial services,

and (d) the grocery retail industry. Best and Andreasen

(1977) and Day and Bodur (1977) report that these indus­

tries are characterized by high levels of consumer dis­

satisfaction and complaints. For instance. Day and Bodur

(1977) report that the percentage of users dissatisfied

with auto-repair services is as high as 49.2X, with

health services in hospitals as much as 24.IX and with

banks and trust companies over 18X. Yet, only 61.IX of

these dissatisfied users, in the case of auto-repair,

take some action (e.g., complaint). This proportion

drops to 36.3X in health service industry, and is less

than 30X for grocery products (Best and Andreasen 1975).

This selection of industry groups, then, provides a wide

variation in post-purchase processes, level and nature of

dissatisfaction, and nature of complaints.

From a macro-marketing standpoint, Andreasen (1983)

predicts a rather gloomy picture of service industries

that are characterized by "loose monopolies." Hirschman

(1970) defines "loose monopolies" as industries, such as

health services where there are many suppliers but where

the consumer's freedom of "exit," that is, his/her abil­

ity to switch suppliers, is restricted. Andreasen (1983)

Page 19: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

predicts that when service industries function as loose

monopolies, they become increasingly inefficient and

unresponsive to consumer dissatisfaction. Ironically,

despite this rise in dissatisfaction, the nature of the

industry is such that consumers are discouraged from

seeking other suppliers. Such consequences entail great

cost, both for the individual consumer and the society as

a whole (Hirschman 1970).

This combination of increasing competition and

declining consumer satisfaction raises some serious

concerns for managers of consumer services. If a

systematic investigation of the processes, antecedents

and possible causes of dissatisfaction and complaints in

specific sex /ice industries can be carried out, it may

provide managers with guidelines for addressing

complaints satisfactorily and suggest programs for respo­

nding to future dissatisfactions. Such a program could

lead to a higher level of consumer satisfaction which in

turn encourages loyalty over repeated purchases (Engel

and Blackwell 1982).

Outline of the Dissertation

The dissertation is organized as follows: (a) a

review of the theoretical and empirical work undertaken

in the CS/D and CCB area; (b) the development of a

holistic model that serves as the basis for the present

research; (c) the operationalization of the holistic

Page 20: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

10

model and the development of key hypotheses; (d) the

survey procedures and methods adopted to collect data;

(e) the analysis of data using IRT and other traditional

methods to implement empirical investigation of these

hypotheses; and (f) a discussion of the implications,

contributions and limitations of the research.

Page 21: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 2

A REVIEW OF THE CONCEPTUAL AND EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Conceptual Review

Terms and Concepts

Most of the research in CS/D and CCB concerns pro­

ducts. Key terms in the area, such as perceived perfor­

mance, expectation level, use experience, are defined in

terms of products rather than services. Since terms

merely represent theoretical concepts and concepts are

invariant across subjects (i.e., products or services),

these terms can be redefined for services. For instance,

the performance of a product is conceptually equivalent

to the benefits of a service, the use experience of a

product to the consumption experience of a service, and

so on. Rather than define a host of new terms, this

dissertation retains the usual terminology of the area

(performance, use, etc.) but implicitly assumes that

these terms represent concepts that are consistent with

the notion of services.

Theories of CS/D

The central concept in the various approaches to the

study of CS/D is the notion of discrepancy. It is implied

that satisfaction or dissatisfaction results from a

11

Page 22: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

12

discrepancy, which in turn is a function of the conscious

comparison between "a cognitive state prior to the event

and a subsequent cognition state, usually realized after

the event is experienced" (Oliver 1980, p. 206). These

events could be product use experiences, the consumption

of services or other related consumption phenomena. The

various theories of CS/D are consistent in suggesting

that a feeling of satisfaction results when the subse­

quent cognitive state "exceeds" the prior cognitive

state. Dissatisfaction results when the former "falls

short" of the latter (Woodruff et al. 1983). These

theories differ in the theoretical meaning and appro­

priate operationalizations of these cognitive states, and

in the underlying mechanism of the comparison between

these cognitive states.

Cognitive Dissonance Theory

Early writers in marketing, including Engel, Koliat

and Blackwell (1968, pp. 512-15) and Howard and Sheth

(1969, pp. 145-50) proposed a cognitive dissonance theory

(Festinger 1957) as the underlying framework for CS/D.

They suggested that prior cognitions were expectations of

product or service performance and based on shopping

effort involved, and that subsequent cognitions pertained

to actual product performance. Thus satisfaction would

increase as the ratio of performance to expectations

increased (Cardozo 1965; Woodside 1972). The cognitive

Page 23: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

13

dissonance theory predicted that consumers would be moti­

vated to reduce the dissonance between expectations and

performance, yet empirical research in the laboratory and

the field showed little support for this framework and

suggested that CS/D may be a much more complex process

(Olson and Dover 1976, 1979; Oliver 1977; Swan 1977).

Assimilation-Contrast Theory

The Assimilation-Contrast theory (Sherif and Hovland

1961) framework was proposed by Anderson (1973) as a

possible mechanism for CS/D process. He posited that

consumers have a "Just noticeable difference" (Jnd) for

the magnitude of discrepancy. That is, there exists a

range of acceptable deviations around one's expectation

level which is not perceived as discrepant. This range

forms a "latitude of acceptance" which produces the

assimilation effect. Alternatively, deviations falling

outside this latitude become psychologically magnified so

that the product is perceived as much better or worse

than it actually is: the contrast effect. Empirical work

based on this framework has yielded equivocal findings

(Olshavsky and Miller 1972; Olson and Dover 1979). Oliver

(1980) suggests that the failure of the assimilation-con­

trast theory may be because:

emerging research across different products and con­texts shows that expectations and disconfirmation are uncorrelated Crather than be interacting as suggested by assimilation-contrast theory] and

Page 24: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

14

satisfaction is an additive function of the two (p. 207). ^

Comparison Level Theory

LaTour and Peat (1979) suggest Thibaut and Kelly's

(1959) comparison level theory as another promising

research direction. An extension of this theory to CS/D

phenomenon implies that a consumer has a desired expecta­

tion level for each attribute of the product or the

service, which is essentially subjective and is based on

personal experience, significant other's experience and

the unique nature of the situation. This subjective

expectation level and its comparison with the net of

positive and/or negative disconfirmations of perceived

attribute levels for the brand (service) determine the

degree of satisfaction with the brand (service).

The major contribution of this framework is in its

definition of expectation levels that results from affec­

tive evaluation of physical attributes as well as other

subjective considerations. This is in contrast to the

assimilation-contrast theory and the cognitive dissonance

theory, both of which conceptualize expectations as

dependent on objective features (attributes) of the

product or service. Though this framework appears inter­

esting, empirical work in this area has suffered due to

the lack of rigorous conceptual development of the

comparison level theory as applied to consumer satisfac-

Page 25: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

15

tion or dissatisfaction area (Oliver 1980, pp. 207-8).

Adaptation Level Theory

Recent work by Woodruff et al. (1983) and Oliver

(1980a) has attempted to model the additive character­

istic of expectations and disconfirmations in determining

CS/D by using Helson's Adaptation Level theory (1948,

1959). The theoretical meaning of expectations in this

framework is very similar to that of its definition in

the comparison level theory (LaTour and Peat 1979). That

is, expectations are based on any number of factors

including prior experiences, word-of-mouth, manufac­

turer's reputation, advertising, etc. Disconfirmation is

the perceived discrepancy obtained by comparing the

perceived performance against the expectation level. The

consumers' discrepancy ratings are hypothesized to be

normally distributed around their expectation level. The

consumer's net response of satisfaction or dissatisfac­

tion is then given by:

Satisfaction = F (expectations, disconfirmation) (1)

The function "F" is an additive function of uncorre­

lated factors (Oliver 1980a). Further, Oliver (1980a)

suggests that satisfaction experiences influence future

purchase intentions as well as post-purchase attitude,

say at time (t-^1). In other words, a dissatisfying

product or service use experience would decrease one's

Page 26: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

16

inclination to re-purchase. Thus post-purchase attitude

at time (t+l) can be written as:

Attitude(t*l) = F (attitude(t), satisfaction) (2)

In all of the above, which Oliver (1980) calls as

the "over-theorizing" of CS/D phenomenon, one key

question has received comparatively much less attention:

What is the appropriate conceptualization of satisfac­

tion, the dependent phenomenon? Is satisfaction a

cognition, an attitude, an emotion, a feeling or a

motivating force? Recent research in CS/D is now begin­

ning to explore this issue seriously; this research focus

has served to bring together the CS/D and the CCB areas

of research on post-purchase process. The issue of the

conceptualization of satisfaction is now addressed.

Conceptualization of Satisfaction

Can satisfaction be conceptualized as an attitude

which results from disconfirmed expectation? In order to

qualify as an attitude the phenomena must be persistent

and fairly stable over time (Day 1984). Since satisfac­

tion is the result of a particular consumption event, it

can neither exist before the event occurs nor necessarily

affect outcomes of future consumption events. This is

particularly true for frequently purchased services and

products. Thus, satisfaction does not seem to fit the

Page 27: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

17

definition of an attitude (Day 1984).

How then should satisfaction be viewed? Recent

studies support the conceptualization of CS/D as an

emotional feeling (Westbrook 1983; Woodruff et al. 1983).

Emotion is defined as "a state of arousal which is mani­

fested in conscious feelings, neurological processes and

observable expressions and behaviors" (Day 1984). Thus

emotions (satisfactions or dissatisfactions) may quickly

subside when the triggering stimulus (consumption event)

is removed or the situation changes (engaging in a new

consumption event). Westbrook (1983) presents the

results of an empirical study and Woodruff et al. (1983)

provide a theoretical framework incorporating an emo­

tional conceptualization of CS/D.

Initial or Final Reaction

Andreasen (1977) further clarified the CS/D concept

by suggesting that there are two critical points in the

post-purchase process at which one can define CS/D. The

first or the "initial" reaction is S/D resulting from the

consumer's disconfirmation of expectations with product

or service performance. The second or "final" reaction

is the consumer's perceived S/D with the manner in which

complaints are handled. While the study of initial

reaction of S/D may predict that a "dissatisfying product

purchase should decrease one's inclination to repurchase"

(Oliver 1980) and support a satisfaction >post-attitude

Page 28: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

18

link, the study of final S/D may suggest that this may

not necessarily be the sequence of events. In partic­

ular, the final S/D reaction may predict that the

consumers who have the source of their initial dissatis­

faction resolved by sellers' complaint handling

mechanisms, may end up with enhanced positive post­

attitude and post-intentions of repurchase (Day 1981;

Andreasen 1977). Thus a CS/D can be conceived of as an

emotion that occurs both initially and again, after

sellers respond to reactions of dissatisfaction, with an

intervening complaint process.

Conceptualization of CCB

At least two competing conceptualizations of CCB

have been proposed in the literature (see Figure 2.1).

Bearden and Teel (1983) suggest that CCB is an action

resulting from emotions of dissatisfaction. Such a

conceptualization assumes no intervening variables

between CS/D and CCB. Much of the empirical work with

this conceptualization suggests a very weak relationship

between CS/D and CCB, with typically only 15% of the

variance explained (Day 1984). These findings strongly

suggest a misspecification of the relationship between

CS/D and CCB.

The alternative conceptualization posits that

complaining behavior is logically subsequent to dissatisfaction and is a distinct set of activities

Page 29: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

19

which are influenced by a variety of situational and personal factors which appear to be unrelated to the intensity of dissatisfaction (Day 1984).

This implies that CS/D is an emotional state which will

under some circumstances motivate consumers to engage in

a complaining/non-complaining decision process, the out­

come of which is the specific CCB. In other words,

dissatisfaction motivates the consumer to undergo a

subsequent decision making process which depends not so

much on how strong the emotions of dissatisfaction are

but on the consumer's perception of the attribution of

dissatisfaction, expectancy and value of outcomes, costs

involved, product importance, etc. This conceptual­

ization is consistent with many empirical findings that

show that a large number of dissatisfied consumers do not

complain (Best and Andreasen 1975; Day and Ash 1979;

Warland et al. 1975).

What do these conceptualizations of CS/D and CCB

suggest for the relationship between post-purchase proc­

esses and post-attitudes? If proper distinctions are made

between satisfaction from disconfirmation of expectations

and the satisfaction with resolution of subsequent

complaints, the following equations can be written:

Attitude(tl)=F(expectations) (3)

Satisfaction(i)=F(expectations,disconfirmation) (4)

Complaint actions=F(personality, attributions, costs and benefits, attitudes,

product importance) (5)

Page 30: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

20

given: riigsatisfaction

Satisfaction(f)=F(complaint actions,resolution) (6)

Attitudes(t2)=F(satisfaction(i), satisfaction(f), attitude(tl)) (7)

where the subscript "i" refers to initial and "f"

refers to final.

In instances when no dissatisfaction takes place,

the post-attitude at time t2 is a function of (a) atti­

tudes at earlier time tl and (b) satisfaction with the

product performance. However, when CCB is triggered by

performance dissatisfaction, satisfaction with the reso­

lution of these complaints significantiy influences atti­

tudes at time t2. Post-purchase intentions at times

subsequent to t2 are a function of attitudes at time t2.

The process described by equations 3-7 is presented in

Figure 2.2.

What is the specific nature of this CCB process?

The theories of CCB are reviewed in an attempt to answer

this question.

Theories of CCB

It is suggested that a better understanding and

evaluation of theories can be achieved when the phenom­

enon of interest is properly defined and its taxonomy (or

classification) satisfactorily developed (Hunt 1983).

Therefore, first the issue of a definition for CCB is

Page 31: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

21

addressed and a comprehensive classification system for

CCB is proposed. Then the proposed classification system

is used as the framework for evaluating the various

theories of CCB.

Definition and Taxonomy

Landon (1980) states that complaint behavior has not

been thoroughly described and further work would be

helpful on a global definition and taxonomy of complaint

actions. Thus the development of a comprehensive

classification system is a desirable contribution to the

area.

What ought to be the properties of such a

classification system? Hunt (1983) suggests that classi­

fication schema should: (a) adequately specify the

phenomena, (b) adequately specify the criteria of classi­

fication, (c) provide categories that are mutually

exclusive, and (d) collectively exhaustive, and finally

(e) be useful.

In accordance with Hunt's first guideline, the

phenomenon must be adequately specified. A widely

accepted definition of CCB states (Day 1980; Fornell and

Didow 1980; Jacoby and Jaccard 1981):

The consumer complaint behavior or response is the

set of all non-behavioral and behavioral responses

which are triggered by dissatisfaction and involve

communicating something negative regarding a

Page 32: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

22

purchase episode including the product or service.

Day and Landon (1977) proposed a two level hierar­

chical classification of CCB. The first level distin­

guishes between action and no action, while the second

distinguishes public actions from private actions.

Public actions include seeking redress or refund from the

seller as well as complaining to a consumer organization.

Private actions include word-of-mouth communication to

friends and relatives and the consumer's decision to stop

patronizing the product or service.

Day (1980) proposed another classification schema

for complaint behavior using the criterion of consumer

motive and the distinction between seeking redress and

the decision to change future behavior. He proposed

three responses based on this criterion:

(1) Seeking Redress: seeking a specific remedy.

(2) Complaining: communicating dissatisfaction for

reasons other than seeking remedy.

(3) Personal Boycott: discontinue purchase of

offending product or service.

Although this taxonomy addresses a deficiency in the

Day and Landon (1977) classification, it fails to

incorporate the distinction in private versus public

actions. However, Day (1980) suggests that his taxonomy

could be combined with that of Day and Landon (1977).

Research by Fornell and Westbrook (1983), Day and

Page 33: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

23

Bodur (1977) and Day and Ash (1979) shows that there are

significant differences between consumers who complain to

sellers directly and those who take their complaints to

consumer organizations. Therefore, the distinction

between dyadic and third party complaint actions is,

probably, another useful criterion in the understanding

of CCB. A "dyadic" CCB response is limited to the

dissatisfied consumer and the seller whose product/-

service caused the dissatisfaction. "Third party" CCB

response involves consumer organizations, public

agencies, friends, relatives, etc.

Thus an exhaustive and complete classification of

CCB responses is achieved by incorporating within a

single taxonomy the distinctions between action/no

action, public action/private action, redress/future

behavior, and dyadic/third party. This taxonomy is in

part hierarchical since the last three distinctions are

meaningful only if some action is taken. For instances

of no action, further classification is meaningless.

A taxonomical framework using the above distinctions

is shown in Figure 2.3. Interpretation of the various

cells is fairly straightforward. For instance, voice

implies public actions for seeking redress that are

directed at the sellers. Similarly, exit results when

private actions for changing future patronage of the

specific seller/product/service are taken.

Page 34: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

24

This classification schema provides a useful

grouping of the whole range of phenomena into mutually

exhaustive classes with well defined characteristics. An

interesting property of such a logical partitioning or

classification is that it reveals some "empty cells" that

are under-researched (cells 1 and 3 ) . Landon (1980)

previously noted that the CCB literature has completely

ignored informal actions taken occasionally for the

purpose of seeking redress from sellers.

This classification schema is the basic building

block in the theory building process and will be employed

in subsequent evaluation and empirical investigation of

CCB process.

Conceptual Frameworks of CCB

Phenomenological Model. Landon (1977) made one of the

first attempts in the marketing literature to develop a

theoretical model of CCB process. He proposed a phenom­

enological model that restricts attention to the

consumer's perceptions of the relevant variables and

therefore does not directly incorporate the salient

characteristics of the environment. He suggested that

CCB process can be modeled by the equation:

Complaint behavior=F(dissatisfaction, importance, benefit from complaining, personality characteristics) (8)

Page 35: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

25

Landon (1977) defined dissatisfaction as the func­

tion of the discrepancy between performance and expecta­

tion, importance of the product/service as a function of

cost, the search time involved in its purchase, the

possibility of physical harm that could result from

dissatisfaction and the ego involvement, benefit from

complaining as a function of the perceived payoffs and

costs of complaining, and personality as a sum of

concepts such as discontent, locus of control, attribu­

tions, etc. Landon (1977) proposed personality to be

only a mediator in the process of CCB.

Landon's model integrated various research findings

in the CCB area and triggered a large number of studies

(Clabaugh et al. 1979; Day 1981; Bearden and Mason 1983;

Barnes and Kelloway 1980; Day and Ash 1979). However,

the model's lack of specificity allows neither a critical

assessment of empirical findings nor a clear under­

standing of the process underlying the CCB responses.

Attribution Theory. Another approach suggested for

studying CCB is based on attribution theory (Krishnan and

Valle 1979; Richins 1983; Valle and Waliendorf 1977;

Foikes 1984). This theory suggests that people are

rational information processors whose actions are influ­

enced by causal inferences (Foikes 1984). In other

words, when people are dissatisfied with a service, they

Page 36: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

26

try to determine the cause of dissatisfaction and assign

responsibility.

Weiner (1980) suggested analyzing attributions in

terms of stability (temporary or permanent), locus (in

the consumer or in the seller), and controllability

(volitional or non-volitional). These attributions in

turn influence the type of action taken by the consumer

in response to a dissatisfying experience.

Empirical findings generally support the basic

tenets of the attributional theory. That is, the more

external, the more stable and the more controllable the

attribution, the greater the likelihood of engaging in

the VOICE responses. For instance, Foikes (1984) shows

that consumers who attribute the failure to the manu­

facturer or store tend to engage more in seeking refunds.

However, Landon's model (1977) indicates that

attributions constitute but one of many determinants of

the CCB process. For instance, consumers may make exter­

nal and volitional attributions of their dissatisfac­

tions, but may decide not to complain because "it is not

worth the effort" or "the retailer wouldn't care anyway"

(Foikes 1984). Thus the explanation and prediction of

CCB based entirely on attributions of consumer dissatis­

faction remain limited. Perhaps attributions are

antecedents to beliefs or expectancies regarding various

courses of action, which in turn affect the CCB process

Page 37: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

27

(Landon 1977; Foikes 1984).

Economic Perspective. A far more promising theoretical

framework is proposed by Hirschman (1970) in the area of

economics. He suggested that the likelihood of CCB is

determined by two factors:

(1) an evaluation of the probability that taking a

particular action would have a desired result, e. g. ,

redress, refund, etc. This concept is similar to the

notion of expectancy of an outcome from a course of

action.

(2) a Judgment that the end result of the action is

worthwhile. This is the value Judgment of the

outcome.

Hirschman (1970) focused on macro-economic effects

of consumer responses to dissatisfaction. He proposed

that industries which are highly competitive would elicit

responses of EXIT from dissatisfied consumers. This is

so because many alternative products or services are

available and the cost of VOICE is high compared to the

easiness of EXIT. On the contrary, in monopolistic

industries, the consumers are "locked" in with the seller

and the avenue of EXIT is blocked. Hence, dissatisfied

consumers are more prone to VOICE their feelings of

dissatisfaction.

Fornell and Didow (1980) and Fornell and Robinson

(1983) present two empirical studies based on Hirschman's

Page 38: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

28

theory. By modeling the effects of structural variables

(availability of alternative substitutes, expectancy of

outcomes, the number of competing firms in the industry

and distribution breadth), 63X of the variance in CCB

responses could be explained.

Andreasen (1983) has attempted to elaborate on the

economic framework of Hirschman (1970) as applied to the

CS/D and CCB processes in loose monopolies. He cites the

health industry as an example of a loose monopoly where

it is possible for the consumers (patients) to exit but

where exiting is unlikely for a host of reasons (restric­

ted information about other doctors, ignorance of the

patient, psychological inhibitions against changing a

doctor). In such industries, the consumer's ability to

EXIT as well as the ability to VOICE is restricted. Thus

overall quality and welfare suffers. This particular

extension of the economic framework promises to be very

useful for the study of CCB, but as yet no empirical test

of this theory has been undertaken.

Psychological Perspective. The psychological approach to

the study of CCB process has gained considerable atten­

tion from many recent researchers (Jacoby and Jaccard

1981; Richins 1979, 1982; Day 1980, 1984; Westbrook

1980). This framework suggests that the consumer's psyc­

hological evaluations of the costs and benefits of

Page 39: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

29

complaining actions and the resulting reaction of favor-

ableness or unfavorableness is a strong determinant of

CCB. In other words, attitude towards the act of

complaining is a causal antecedent of CCB and this

relationship can be modeled by the usual attitude >

intentions >behavior framework (Richins 1982; Day

1984). However, researchers in this tradition generally

allow that other variables (rational analysis of the

costs and benefits, attributions, etc.) probably act

along with attitudes to determine CCB. Though some

empirical studies have been undertaken using this frame­

work, the actual form of the relationship is unknown.

Empirical research corroborates a strong relation­

ship between attitudes and intentions of complaining,

though the relationship between attitudes and behavior is

generally poor (Richins 1982). The latter finding is a

reflection of the general lack of attitude-behavior

consistency found in the consumer behavior literature and

is probably due to many situational variables (e.g.,

frequency of patronizing, product cost, etc.) that

intervene in the attitude-behavior relationship (Richins

1982).

Other studies have investigated the effects of

generalized and stable affective influences, e.g.,

consumer discontent and alienation from the market place,

as a determinant of CCB (Westbrook 1980; Bearden and

Page 40: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

30

Mason 1983). Generally, the explanatory power of these

generalized affective influences is weak (Bearden and

Mason 1983). This is perhaps due to the high level of

generality in consumer discontent and alienation from the

market place. CCB is a specific response to a specific

dissatisfaction.

The usefulness of a conceptual framework lies in the

extent of empirical research and support that it entails.

Therefore, a proper evaluation of these various concep­

tual frameworks of CCB process should be in the light of

empirical findings in the area. Based on the review of

the empirical findings, an attempt will be made in the

next chapter to bring together the various conceptual

frameworks into a holistic model of CCB process.

Empirical Review

A recent review of the empirical findings in the CCB

literature was published by Robinson (1979) in the 1978

proceedings of the CS/D and CCB conference. This was one

of the first attempts in the CCB literature to summarize

previous research findings, identify shortcomings and

list areas of future inquiry. Since 1978, there has been

a steady growth in the body of knowledge about consumers

who complain when confronted by consumer problems: who

they are, what they complain about, to whom they

complain, how they are treated when they complain, and

how they differ from other consumers.

Page 41: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

31

In the present dissertation, empirical findings are

discussed in light of Robinson's review of literature and

on empirical findings published since 1978. Table 2.1

shows a summary of key research findings including some

major empirical studies of 1977 (not covered in

Robinson's review). The criterion used for including

studies in table 2.1 are:

(a) adequate coverage of the large variety of

variables investigated as predictors of CCB,

and

(b) inclusion of studies which have generalizable

findings.

Validity of Robinson's Conclusions

Many of the conclusions stated by Robinson (1979)

regarding the state of research in CCB are still valid

for the research undertaken since then. Many empirical

investigations are based on recall information of a past

dissatisfaction and complaint behavior. The use of

scenarios is rare (see for exceptions Langmeyer and

Langmeyer 1979; Foikes 1983). Robinson (1979, p. 41) has

identified a specific problem with this method of re­

search: "large samples sizes are required in most studies

(using recall data) in order to develop an acceptable

number of respondents who had experienced problems with a

product or service." Though this problem is reduced with

Page 42: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

32

the use of scenarios (since each respondent is assumed to

face a given problem). The use of scenarios is not very

common in the CCB literature, perhaps due to the limited

generalizability of its research findings.

Robinson (1979) also reported that most studies

found low complaining rates for those consumers who did

experience a problem. Subsequent studies suggest a

similar conclusion. Day (1981) reported the number of

non-complainers to be between 22X to 46X for different

products, Richins (1983) found them to be as high as

32.2X and Bearden and Mason (1984) reported their number

to be as much as 61X to 76.6X. However, NO ACTION is a

legitimate complaint response (see classification of CCB)

and ought to be investigated as a part of the complete

range of complaint phenomena. Robinson (1979) points out

in his review that most studies focus on complainers,

ignoring non-complainers completely. Subsequent research

studies have attempted to address this deficiency. For

instance, Bearden and Mason (1984) and Richins (1982)

attempted to explain non-complainers based on their

attitudes towards complaining, Gronhaug and Zaltman

(1981) used "market place activity" as the explanatory

variable while Krishnan and Valle (1979) have attempted

to explain no-action consumers using attribution theory.

It was also pointed out in the previous review that

much of the research in the area has been limited to

Page 43: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

33

analysis of demographic correlates. This emphasis has

changed considerably since the review was published

(1979). Many different theoretical streams have been

explored empirically. Perhaps one of the most active

research streams is the psychological perspective which

posits that attitudes toward the act of complaining are

predictors of CCB. Other theoretical frameworks

considered for empirical studies are attribution theory

(Foikes 1984) and Hirschman's economic model incorpo­

rating availability of alternatives and structural

constraints.

Robinson (1979) had also concluded that much of CCB

research has oversimplified the concept of complaint

actions. Most researchers classify complaint actions

into a dichotomy of action/no action but the phenomena

has a range of possibilities, from legal action to word-

of -mouth communication. However, Day (1984) and Bearden

and Teel (1983) among others have attempted to concep­

tualize complaint actions as several possible alterna­

tives which can be ordered according to the extent of

effort involved.

In conclusion, Robinson's (1979) suggestion relative

to sample size remains valid. However, other reported

deficiencies, such as (a) the neglect of non-complainers,

(b) the over-simplification of complaint behavior, and

(c) limitation of the research to demographic correlates,

Page 44: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

34

appear much less severe in current research studies. The

next section summarizes the empirical findings and the

key independent variables investigated as predictors of

CCB.

Key Predictors of CCB

Personality and Demographic Characteristics

Robinson (1979) reports six personality variables

that have been investigated as predictors of CCB;

dogmatism, internal-external locus of control, gener­

alized self confidence, powerlessness, social isolation,

and political efficacy. Subsequent research studies have

in addition investigated aggressiveness and assertiveness

(Richins 1983) as predictors of CCB.

Among the demographic variables, Robinson (1979)

suggests age, income, education, and occupation as key

predictors found across many studies. Complainers are

usually younger, more educated, have higher incomes, and

have a greater tendency to hold managerial and profes­

sional Jobs. In addition, Villareal-Camacho (1983) has

suggested that race and cultural differences could also

be related to complaint actions. However, Gronhaug and

Zaltman (1981) show that these demographic and person­

ality correlates are, perhaps, artifacts of the extent of

market place activity. They show empirically that the

correlation between demographic and personality corre-

Page 45: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

35

lates and CCB becomes insignificant when the effect of

market place activity is partialled out.

Attitudes and Affective Variables

There has been considerable investigation of the

effects of many attitudinal and affective variables on

complaint actions. While Robinson (1979) reports much

research on the concepts of consumer discontent

(Lundstrom and Lament 1976) and alienation from the

market place (Allison 1978), generally the explanatory

power of these generalized affective influences is weak

(Bearden and Mason 1983). This is probably due to the

high level of generality of these affective influences.

CCB is a specific response to a specific dissatisfaction.

Subsequent research has attempted to operationalize

the construct of attitude toward the act of complaining,

and to use it as predictor of intention to engage in

complaint actions (Richins 1980; Bearden, Teel and

Crockett 1980; Bearden and Mason 1984). These findings

corroborate a strong relationship between attitudes and

intentions of complaining though the relationship between

attitudes and actual behavior is generally poor (Richins

1982). The latter finding is a reflection of the general

lack of attitude-behavior consistency found in the

consumer behavior, and is probably due to many situa­

tional variables (i.e., product cost, importance,

frequency of patronizing the store) that intervene the

Page 46: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

36

attitude-behavior relationship (Richins 1982).

Evaluation of Alternatives

There is also a growing concern that the cost and

benefit evaluation of each alternative complaint action

should be incorporated within CCB model (Day 1984).

Richins (1980) has empirically shown the predictive

validity of this concept in explaining complaint actions.

Fornell and Didow (1980) show that consumer's perception

of possible alternatives available in a given situation

is a useful predictor in the case of complaint actions

with a range of products and services. Finally Richins

(1983) shows that consumers' evaluation of retailer

responsiveness when a specific complaint action is taken,

is a key determinant of the complaint behavior across two

product groups.

Attribution Variables

Other variables that have been investigated as

predictors of CCB include notions of external versus

internal attributions for the "cause" of dissatisfaction

(Foikes 1984), and the severity of the dissatisfaction or

the problem (Richins 1983). Each of these variables have

been examined empirically as antecedent to CCB.

Empirical findings generally support the basic tenets of

the attribution theory. That is, the more external, the

more stable, and the more controllable the attribution.

Page 47: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

37

the greater the likelihood of engaging in VOICE

responses. For instance, Foikes (1984) shows that

consumers who attribute the product failure to the

manufacturer or store tend to engage more in seeking

refunds than to take NO ACTION. In addition, the

severity of the dissatisfaction seems to affect the

amount of effort that is expended in the CCB process

though not necessarily the specific complaint action

taken (Richins 1983).

Structural Variables

Fornell and Robinson (1983) and Fornell and Didow

(1980) present two empirical studies based on Hirschman's

theory. Their research indicates that structural

constraints such as the nature of industry (for example

competitive, monopolistic, etc.), and the distribution

breadth (for example widely distributed, etc.) seems to

affect the consumer's feelings of which complaint action

would be fruitful, in other words, the "expectancy" of

complaint actions. By modeling the effects of structural

variables (availability of alternative substitutes,

expectancy of outcomes, the number of competing firms in

the industry and distribution breadth), they explained

63X of the variance in the CCB responses.

Discussion

This chapter summarized and reviewed the literature

Page 48: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

38

in CCB, discussed conceptual frameworks currently

employed and analyzed empirical findings in this area.

Two conclusions can be drawn. One, that the CCB area is

enriched by considerable theoretical contribution pur­

porting to explain the process underlying the complaint

actions. Each of these theoretical frameworks has

received empirical corroboration for its hypotheses

(though limited in some cases) indicating that there may

be more than one route for explaining the CCB process.

This conclusion provides the basis for the second key

finding. The many different and sometimes contrasting

theoretical frameworks for explaining the CCB process

appear to give an impression of a relatively fragmented

structure of the research in the area. The empirical

corroboration of these perspectives suggests that each of

these frameworks may be valid under different conditions

and situations. Further research that investigates these

conditions and identifies the "valid zone" for the indi­

vidual theories would be very useful in increasing our

understanding of the CCB process.

Further, a comprehensive conceptual model that

incorporates different streams of past research findings

and at the same time provides programmatic research

directions, would address a key deficiency in the area.

Richins (1979) states in the same vein:

Few if any studies (in the CCB area) have followed

Page 49: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

39

or been embedded within a conceptual or theoretical framework. This deficit is an important one, and a conceptual model is needed to fully understand the nature of consumer complaining behavior.

Similarly, Foikes (1984) affirms that a theoretical

model is needed to "map out relationships between

specific thoughts about product failure and specific

complaining behavior."

Some recent attempts have been made to address these

issues. Day (1984) proposed a conceptual model that

incorporated the psychological perspective and the

rational decision making perspective of the economic

theory. However, the model is not formalized to allow

empirical testing of derived hypotheses. For instance,

the nature and operationalization of the construct of

"analysis of alternatives," which is a direct determinant

of CCB is not suggested. Thus the objective of the

following chapter is to develop and partially formalize a

holistic model of consumer complaining behavior--a model

that would incorporate the different theoretical streams

of thought in a single framework. The partial formaliza­

tion of this model is undertaken to clearly define the

assumptions, axioms and law-like statements contained in

the development of the model.

Page 50: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

4 0

(N

U

CD <

cn CD z M Q Z

-J < < u u M OC

cc <

a. oQ E U UI u u u X X

u. z O M

>•

< z: c D en

C-lO t / l

c o •O (J

c

5 ^ 'oJ

Q .

f c c O 0)

c o o

>> <fl * J

C 0 0 a; o. o . t . -o Q . 0 )

c ,— iQ a>

O) >< X ) r -3 ^

•«-> O •.- a; 4-> (_

< - O 1 -

«- cy>

c a; o • ^ irt

a> o •— n ^ .

•M 4- - D •»- «J 3 «/) > 4-> C - 1 -01 U 4->

O ^ <« ( - Q . Q . ID 4->

t - O 0) O) Ol c o >«-a; E * > 0) <«

• J - D X C C 0^

•o 0) *-> <c o>

•^ 4->

«/> 0 1

> c

• • ^

0 0

o

(/) "O (. <a s o

4 . )

m 0)

•o s

' M

•^ 4 J 4 ^ « t

l/> - . -C IT] 0 ) 1 — Q. O. O E t . O

Q. O

• . a> c c •!-

• ^ *c c .— •

• > - Q . 10 IQ E c

.— O O Q . O • > -

o o o O 4-> «

u

o

"S

o a; Q .

(O I . C 0)

o 1 -t- - c a.

OJ 0) f

O; (/) X c o o

CT>

3

"S

OJ

o

0."? •c

I/O ^-^ O Ol=> 01 'M C t . ^ c o c ^ •—

<-> O 4 J «" - ^ O 10

4-> C O) 0) le - ^

- C (J O O ) •M • - - T ) 0)

C ^J

§ 3 O tJ

E 4-> (_

u

<0 r— « - Q .

CO c

O) ^ 0) •4J CT>

0) c f 0) en

•^ o

u lO

4-> a>

c .— <o o a. S <o

0) ro

u c

U 3 (O 0 ) 0 0 ; «- E ^

"O (•- 4.> O IQ

10 I .— «/) "C fl< 0) ( . ( .

"i c

« (t3 1 -

.^ Qi Ut OJ • • - - O 0) — C lo Q . 0) O 3 I -

I— «rt <4-

(-0) E 3 «/) C O «J

c o E

"S (O .

i-s o; c

JC 10 4-> O

• I - o <o - 1 -Q . > . Ol L. 01 ( - o; 4->

io 0) o i—

• M • © * J 3 C 3

^ <0 O

to OJ

p —

^ (C

c 3

.> (/) 04 ^ •O XI

1 "TJ

S8

UI 0) u •^ > (. Ol «/)

t / l

(. 0)

t . lO

o; to 0)

Of

. o

• t o

•o . ^ u to O ( . C-

« o. oe

• o «/) < c

• ^ v^

f CVJ U GO

f - CTN

QC ^

• ^ i ^

o; o;

t—

• c c <o

c o;

•o (-l O

« oc

St T

« fo oc O l

.-

CVJ

4->

o;

OC

*»22 la 9 i

O r-i

CO

o; u O 0) o;

I .

0)

«o • a; • c to to o (/I t_ • . -O) Ol 4-> «- E o Q . 3 le 3 to I -to c OJ

O 4->

to o c

f- s »-• « - > • . - 0 * <o < . - .— C 1/1 r - . U • . - OJ OJ 4-> I/I .

4-> IQ I • >— to ( . <a to o)

— >>• «»- - D 3 O ^ .

C > > - ^ O)

4 ^ ^ •^ V) •»-> r r- 0»

OJ

u

« o c -.- •.-

<0 C O) — OJ 3 • • - "O o -lO C 01 > OJ t -

< 4 J *•-

c f— IQ

(o a. 1/1 g OJ o

t o T 3 C C O lO . 1 -

4 J C O

o <o • -4-> 4-> O C (O - i -

<*- <o </> 1 —

• • - Q .

IQ 5 t<0 O

.« t o c o •^ 4-) ( J l O

0) 4>> R>

> •»" (_ o .

. 10 •o <D c <a u

T3 C m

U I

=>

. lO

•o <o c le

o

4.J C

<o

Q .

§ U

— 'OJ o

o >

lO I/I OJ 4 J O ( J f -3 >

T3 »-O OJ L. to Q .

1—I HJ

o "D • •»- «J

o o • O Q . c lO Q£

o >- < "oJ » c o o a

to "W c «/l - ^ 01 O l 1-

.— C 0) • • - * J

a; •.- •.-4-> <T3 ^ i - ,— O)

O O . ( -

OJ § c «_ t j — t o to «- - D OJ (-

I S I/I o C 4-> o <J 1/1

OJ

"SI 4-) 4-> lO • . -C 4 ^ OJ 4-1

• . - lO

IT> OJ

>

O l to

5g.

i / i 4 J c

l O

I / I c o (J

OJ

I / I • O 4-> OJ c > •.-OJ r -O Q . 1- e OJ o a. u

Ol • o ^ O) — c

11 Ol

4-> I/ I OJ c

• c o

I / I .^J

c — 0> l_

.^^ 4-> IQ 4->

4- ) <0

O "O o — c •.-4.> C 4.1 u OJ u <0 4 J <o

4.) 01 to

f - >— 4 J <0 lO IQ

.— c to O . ( - I / I g OJ — O 4 J -O

u c %- o O Ol

J= OJ OJ 4-> I / I Q. 3 >> > . <o

t— ^ O

> l C • J 4>' OJ 0 )

• .-• .- .^ I/I — c_ C lO lO Ol E Q . - 0 O C Ol (. <« ^ Q . 4->

c

I/I <o 5 OJ — «-

• O Q . " ^

4-> 5 c •»- o o 4-> -r-*J O *-> < 4 J <0

. OJ u <o — Q .

1 . -

1 . -

•^ •o

•o c <o

t o c o •^ 4 - >

3

^ •^ t .

4 . ) 4->

<

1 e 5 u

• <*- t o O C

o t o • » -01 4->

o. u >. <«

4 J 4 J

•^ c

c •.-OJ IT> L. , _ OJ Q .

( - OJ

•>- u a. > OJ (_ (- a,

to O

4 J - D 3 C

< <o

c o to

H c -^ 01

"O • <o 0 0 0) O l

0 0 ^

^

u OJ Q . I / I

OJ

19

> •c C <o

. u o (. a .

oc c u <a < c

•^ &. ^

O l p ^ O l 1 - 4

\n vo

Page 51: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

41

to Ol

C

T3 OJ 3 C

- H •P C 0

u

(N

U

OQ <

•o OJ

19 O l

I/I Ol >

0 0

o

OJ JZ

(-<o OJ to OJ oe.

OJ

^ <o

t o 4.> 1 — Q . IQ 0 » 3 U •o O • ^ <0 >

•'- >» •o •— C 1— t - l O

•^ OJ u > o • f - t o </l to <a OJ t . t o O l <o O l lO O l

c T3 • • -c c w •»"

l o OJ 1— > Q .

Z § (- u OJ I / I OJ t o > (O t -

OJ OJ u ;- (_

. u o • r -

> <o x: £ OJ OJ

^ ; ; = IQ C OJ

•.- o » c •.-

t o - r - O

OJ c <o > 4.< O — : <0 f - Q . > 4.> E C

•— 3 O <0 ( - ^ U ^ Q . - 1 - 4-»

f~ 01 OJ 4-) - o c ( - 4.1 3 OJ Q lO 4 ^ 4 ^

,— +3 o Ol IQ 4.> ^ C IQ OJ It} L. ( _

4.* 01 01 Q 4 J > E

^ C •>-(O • » - 4 J S

* - ' <0 4 J OJ O l —

O •>< OJ X 4.1 l « C OJ

E : • O OJ C ' - v t _ OJ 0 1 J O Q t o

4 J « — E 3

«/l • OJ I / I C I / I > C

• OJ r— O .

§ t/Y

• </)

. => OJ .c 4 . )

I .

o 1 . -

OJ 3

(-4.1

t o

• -OJ I / I

c OJ > OJ (_ OJ

l O C l O (O J C U O f u

• r - *^" ' r ->< 4 J — » X

ii ^^±

4 . )

"O c

o 4-» O 4.>

>» •— t o Ol c

^ IQ

; = ! t o "O «/l OJ t o

^ - <o

OJ t o

c c <o o

• . t o 4.> t - o OJ lO

E 3 4 . ) t o C C • # -O IQ t J . —

. c o

4.1

u 19

I t ­lO

•^ 4-» 19

Q . t o OJ E > O

•>- O 4 J «- O 01 *•> to l / l 4.> t l ( . 1 o

c to

52!

t o

•^ •o OJ

4 . ) lO

• r -

> OJ ^^ "iS

4. ) O c 11 (-<o t o

4. ) c <Q

a. § u

"S o

o >

"S 4.1

•^ u

• ^

^ o t o

c

• o OJ

> • ^

OJ o I . OJ a.

_ <9 3

..> t J IQ

<.-O

C O

4 J 19

4.> C . OJ t o t o C OJ o t _ - 1 -a . 4 . ) OJ U t . l a

<*-OJ to

4 J • » -(Q 4.1 L lO 3 l / l O (O O • • -HJ "O

1

Ol 4.) 4.) C o • • -lO IQ

c .— IQ Q .

x: E o o o >> 4J T3 •.- C > — < Q

<9 C (/I o o to -P-( . 4.) OJ l/>

a . •>-

. l/l c o

•^ 4 J

u <9

4.) c

•^ <o r * . .

a. c E 0) o .^ u le 4.>

<«-O l/l c

to O OJ •— a . 4 J > i o

1— <o

. t o

(_ 0) a. a. o ^ to

t /1

=> • o c TJ

"S

U I I .

» o to "^ c

^ PO U 0 0

•f- O l

"S

o 3

• o o

u OJ Q .

t o

u OJ o . to

o <o E <o o

<o

. u o l _

o.

f OJ c t c_ i n

— ^* •^

«« m 00 O l

I c_ Ol

4 J 4 J O C IQ "T" t- <o <Q I —

J Z Q . U E > i <J 4-> • I - - o I— c • IQ (Q t o C UI c o o o t o • » - ••— C- 4.> 4 . ) OJ l/l o

Q . - ^ IQ

OJ ( . o

4 . ) to

^ . OJ to

o S ( . 4 . *

o o

^ • 4 J O l/l o OJ (-3 a .

o .— < "oJ * C Ol

O <T>

I O C to OJ

••- OJ ^ • D C- «4- "Q

3 O 4J p— 4 J •Q IQ OJ C C C O . O — >,-.->«- - 4 ^ 4 ^

C O " O O " D <Q C • • - C IQ 4.) (Q 4 J

O C .— IQ E ••" IQ I t - Oj IQ

• I - UI •— .— 4 J •>- ^ Q . •— 4J o e C IQ < - O

•—' U I Q . O

to 4 J O 3

•o o • ( . UI a . OJ

o c •»-OJ > > I . OJ OJ l/l to

IQ

5

c < t OJ u UI "o •Q OJ • t- r -

• O !"«. C 0 ^

« « . - 4

^ I Q

•^ U o to

T3 C I Q

O l C •^ c •^ < Q

^ v

a. e o o to

• o (_ I Q

S o 4 ^

l/l OJ

"O 3

4 . )

• • 4.> 4-> «c

O l c o (-

4 - J

UI

01 (. ( Q

to 4 . )

c •^

I Q

• c

•^ I Q

— a. s 5 o o

4 J

10 c o

• p -

0 . 4 . ) B

o u O l c •^ . 1 ^

!S E O l c

•f—

c C-OJ u c o

c OJ

4 . >

c •^ 1 . -

o to

4 - >

c I Q

c •^ e 01

4 > J

OJ tJ "O

OJ • o 3

4 . ) • ^

4 . 1 • * » '

I Q

OJ >

4-->

•^ to O Q .

OJ (.

i I Q

OJ > I Q

. C

4 - J

l/l • ^

c-Ol E 3 UI c o u

. to (-OJ E 3 to C o u

t—

I Q

l -OJ c OJ O l

c " Q

X 4 - J

U I

t o OJ c

• ^

UI 3 ^ to • o (. I Q

X o *->

*J

to

•^ 4->

Q

OJ <.-OJ

T3

I Q

• o I Q

X

4 . 1

to I Q

C_ 4 . )

c o o c

•^ to C-OJ E 3 to c o o ^

I Q

(-OJ

c .$

. OJ •o 3

4 . )

•^ 4 . J

4 . 1

I Q

-C *-i

o ^ o

. - J

TJ OJ

4 . 1

I Q

^— u t .

> 1

— 01 >

•^ 4 - >

• -to O a. UI •^ 4 . )

to o u t . OJ E 3 to C o

t_)

1 •^ t .

4 . 1

! / •

• ^ •

C^

. c o • • 4 . 1

U I Q

<«-to •^ 4»>

I Q

U I

to •^ •o

> i 4 . )

•^ »— I Q

3 o-

T3 C I Q

OJ o •^ t .

OJ O

• ^

I -a. o *-> " a;

4 - 1

• Q

^— V L.

> p *

OJ >

•^ . - l

•#— UI o

o *-> " 1;

4 - 1

• Q

— OJ t -

> l f—

OJ >

•.— 4 . )

< Q

O l OJ

c Q.-C

to

• -X 4 . )

• o I Q

OJ c. ^ c o •.— 4 - >

3 Q . ^

c I Q

C o •^ 4 - >

u I Q

1 . -

to •f—

4 . )

I Q

to to

•r—

T3

. C o •^ 4 - >

l_) I Q

<.-l / l

- p -

4 . )

I Q

to to

• ^

TD

> l 4 . >

•^ — I Q

3

o-

I c o

l ^

•.- o 4-1 to

0 c — O) > 4 J IQ C •

x : •— Ol OJ c

XI T3 •>-C C

0 1 'Q '*— C (Q

• ^ to .— (- OJ a . O T3 E 4.> 3 O • • - 4 J O

UI - o

S IQ

UI ( - 01

• • - o IQ -f-Q . > OJ ( . ( - (U

l/l o

4 J - O 3 C

< : <Q

o *J c- c

IQ •O <— • O) Q . to > E 4->

•— O O OJ U OJ U •'-> «- -O ^ OJ C 3

a . IQ UI

•• O l l/l c OJ — u ^

• C 4J > , IQ O ( - U •— OJ — o o .— O Q . - 0 C Q . C - T IQ IQ

c o •^ *-> IQ t . 4 J

c OJ u c o o

St-

o 4 . )

u OJ

1 . -1 . -« t

c o

• F —

4.1 3

^ •^ 1 .

4 J t o

• r .

•o m

i/i

E •w

>*->*-o

(-Ol

E 3 l / l

c o u

•o c IQ

. X 4. )

•o <Q OJ c_

XI

c o •^ 4. ) U IQ f-t o

• -4-> IQ t o t o

• » • o

C

o 4.* l / l

o u

• > l

*J

•.— .— IQ 3 o-

• o

c IQ

0) o •^ C-Q .

J C 4-> • ^

X

T3 to C 4.) IQ U • 3 l/l O l

1 3 OJ C O > , . — — e — ^ ^ 0 . 4 . 1 IQ 4 J

UI ( . o VC O 3 «—

c o to

oc O l

• a ^ c IQ •

OJ c OJ

"O l~ o • IQ o a£ OJ I . CO

00 O T

o 0 0 O l

IQ > , O IQ O

UI X (-OJ O Q -c — IQ OJ <-> oc ^ <

o

u

'oJ • C PO I- 00 O Ol

CSJ

0 0 O l

Page 52: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

42

tJ Ol 3 C •H

C 0

u

CN

U

OQ <

UI O l

c

u 3 "O o

(J

•.— l/l r— l/l . O OJ 3

r - Q .

OJ O )

> c IQ - 1 -• C ^

I Q

C 4.) O

• - OJ •*-> t o

o o IQ ^

4 . )

OJ 4-> C IQ IQ > JZ

• 1 - 4->

c a . UI

OJ O I T 3 C 3

• I - 4 J . M : •»-IQ 4. )

* J 4 J

UI

c o •^ 4 J

c 01

4>J

c •^ 4 - >

c •^ I Q

p w

01 c OJ

X 4 J

"O c I Q

to

c o •^ 4 J

U Q . IQ

o o

l « -

o o. •^ x: U I

c o •^ 4 J

I Q r —

OJ (Q Of

l/l ( . OJ OJ >

E • -3 4 J U I • ! -C U I

o o U Q .

. c o

4 J

u I Q

^ I Q

(-O " > I Q

. C OJ

X I

— I Q

3 4 J

U I Q

J C 4 J

•^ X

;-o o Q .

4 J

c •*— I Q

— a . s

o u OJ

^* O L

•^ 4 - >

— 3 E

c • r -

OJ O l I Q

O l

c OJ

o 4 . )

> l

u c OJ

•o c OJ

4 . 1

I Q

to • 1 —

• U I

( . o

> I Q

J C

OJ

^

> l

"c o OJ

JZ 4->

l/l

OJ

u c OJ

•^ t . OJ

a . X OJ

O

-5 U I

c_ OJ E 3 to

c o o

c OJ OJ X

* J

OJ

^ OJ

o c OJ

(-OJ

<«-O I < 4 -

c • f —

> l 3

^ to 3 O

> OJ

(-O L

• p .

• o

4 . )

c I Q

u •^ >»-•^ c

c OJ

-5 4 . )

o c

o • o

o • i

OJ to

o JZ

*•>

• o

c I Q

c o •^ 4 J

o I Q

OJ E o U I

OJ O l . ^

•^ to I Q

4 J

l / l

OJ

u c OJ

(. OJ

>*~ •*-•.— T3

u .c

OJ

> I Q

X

.

. o 4 . 1

OJ

» c o

0 . 4 . ) I Q

t . O l

o E OJ

T3

4 J

to

£ •

"S • p .

>•-to

•^ 4 J

I Q

U I

U I

•^ -o

I Q

U 3

"S * OJ

O l IQ

* OJ

c o u c •^ U I I Q

X

u 3 to

. to * J

u OJ

1 4 -

l « -

OJ

4 - ^

o OJ

(-• r—

•o c •^

> 1

c o

1

X OJ

* > l

4 . ) • r -

c OJ

> OJ to

1 — X

o C -

Q .

x: 4 - >

•^ »

"O OJ to I Q

OJ C-

u c

• p .

z 1

o 1

3

<•-o

• o o o

X

•^ ,— OJ . X

p —

t . OJ

o —

• o c I Q

. U I

c o

*r—

* J 3

X •^ C-4 J 4 ^ I Q

f—

I Q

C t . OJ

4 . J

. >»

• o OJ

1 (-I Q

OJ

•o •r—

> o

o.

o 4 J

to to OJ

c O l c •^ ^ ,— •^ s to

— I . OJ

^-• -I Q

4 . 1

OJ

(-

1

e Q U

o 4 . >

>> 4 . > • p -

t f l

c 01

a. o c Q .

C

•^ c o •^ 4 . > I Q

•*— (-I Q

> OJ

.c 4 . 1

<*-o

»« 0 0

^r

OJ • o 3

4 . )

•^ 4 . >

4 . > I Q

TJ C

I Q

• k

OJ O l I Q

> i X

•o OJ c I Q

^ Q . X OJ

OJ X

• o

3 O

u

c ' r -

<9 ^m.

a.

. O l c

• p -

c •^ I Q

r ^

O . E o o U I

•o C -

i Q

3 O

4 . >

1 •r—

to 3

X

OJ X 4 . J

.c *J • r -

J

*-> c OJ

4 ->

c o o l O

>t—

X>

Ol L .

I Q

O

-i to l -OJ E 3 to

c o u

O l

c — OJ ^ TS <Q 3 E 4->

•^ C 4.1 • • - 4-1

I Q

to 4_l OJ

c > IQ 4-> t - IQ

4 J O l l/l Ol OJ c

(- IQ UI l/l Ol OJ >

. — IQ X

OJ

> -o — c OJ IQ

u ( - to

^t •9—

U I IQ OJ — U I C L to g OJ O

c o

. to to OJ

c *^-Ul 3

X

to

•o 1 .

I Q

X o

4 . )

T3 OJ

4 - >

I Q

O l • r— 4 . )

to OJ

> c 1 . ^

0 0

o < _ }

OJ • 4.> t o IQ C > O

• ^ »r~ C- 4 J Q . O

I Q

• o C 4.) IQ C

•^ O IQ • ^ r— — Q .

-c s 3 O a. o

<•-o to O •^ 4->

to •f—

t -OJ

4.) U IQ

(-IQ

^ O

OJ . X IQ

4.)

o -i UI I . OJ E 3 to c o o

•a c IQ

to • C 4.J o o

•w- C 4.>

o o IQ "O

4J O

= -s 10

>— OJ a . l/l O 4J

c to 3 4J

E C O IQ U >—

Q . • c E 4-> O 3 O O

1 o < 4 -

o c 1 o

• o •«-C- 4 . ) O IQ 3 U

^ OJ 111

( Q

C • ' —

to • o

O X OJ to 3

5

• < !g t o

•a • OJ «c

to t /1

to 4.) c

•r— . IQ t o

UI -f-OJ IQ

4-» O l

u c 3 ••-< - ^

4.) IQ

UI OJ o

• p . UI > OJ ( .

I— OJ . X l/l IQ

«- -o a IQ

to

OJ X o t-IQ OJ UI OJ

OC

« 5

c o to

£ TJ c . IQ OC

u c <: OJ

T3 • u ^ IQ 0 0 OJ Ol

0 0 PH

1

o (-C L - O •

c to to IQ OJ 3 O O l/l •<-< - 4 J > t_ o t -IQ 3 01

> "O UI

. 1 - 4

00 O l

O l • <J 3 C O IQ IQ L .

X E Q-C 4.> o f— a£ ( . IQ U

o INI <

•g . 10 l/l

OJ O l o

c c • 1 - IQ J C •>-4 J f— O Q .

•— Q . U IQ

_ l

I Q

. C

U I C -

£ *^'l

to - o

c •f— •

X m U 00

— O l 0 £ 1-1

( - OJ • 1 - o IQ - 1 -

O . > OJ ( -

C <u U I

o 4J T3 3 C

« t IQ

• o C IQ

« t

» o . — CO OJ O l OJ •—1

» — • 4->

C 4.1 OJ OJ

TJ ^ C- U IQ O OJ C-

• o C-

a .

1 0 0 O < |

• "S •r—

< 4 -

•^ o 4-> OJ O Q .

Z to

• 0 0 > , IQ <_> .— .— <_) P — T —

OJ o i - o ^ IQ C t /1 Q . IQ

•5° 5 oioo t_ 1 - o

4-) O UI t /1 •

•o ^ C -o r*. 3 C 0>

_ l IQ 1—1

. u O (-Q .

tr> vo 00

Page 53: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

(B£ARC£N AND TEEL, J^. FEB., 1983, PP. 21-8)

4 3

COGNITIONS AFFECTIVE :JNAriVE

A€GAT!VE / DlSCONFIRh*-! I -HON OF \E/PECrATIONS

DISSATISFAC-•riON P<KCESS

<

lOM'LAINT 3f .-lAv ; Jfi ,

CQC^ITIONS •OT. . ir iON AFFECTlVE/COGNl':aNS >:Ot*AT|,E

(WY, ACR PROCEEDINGS, 198*4, PP. 496-9)

FIGURE 2.1

TWO COMPETING CONCEPTUALIZATIONS OF CONSUMER COMPLAINT BEHAVIOR

Page 54: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

44

CN

CN

U

D

<

z

u X CL b] cn < X u a: D a.

I

cn o a.

a: a u.

u Q a x:

en en u u a cc a.

Page 55: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

45

TO S E E K

R E

D

R

E

S S

T O

C H A N G E

F U T U R E

B E H

A V [

!

D Y

A D I C

T H I R D P

A R T Y

D Y A D I

r. T H I R D

P

A R T

Y

PJBLIC AND

FORfV\L ACTIONS

" V O I C E " COrf>LAINTS

TO RETAILER OR

VVNUFACTURER

LEGAL ACTIONS

INTERVENTION BY

PUBLIC OR COfJSUr AGENCIES

SEEK BETTER SERVICE

FROf R E T A I L E R /

riANUFACTURER

COfPLAIN TO oilBLIC

OR C"i'i<;irER

AGENCIES

^ P [ ' . " T XUD

Vpy^'Ai ACTIONS

"irJOR IXIDE'JTS

IN"A:L.'E T COST

rp ccrr.pT

1

SEE" HELP OF

"^ 'ENDS AND/OR

" E X [ - ' = CHANGE

PAT^~-WGE

"iiORD-OF-MDirTVi"

COfTIUNICATION

FIGURE 2.3

A CLASSIFICATION SCHEMA FOR CONSUMER COMPLAINTS

Page 56: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 3

THE CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF A HOLISTIC MODEL

OF CCB

The development of the holistic model of CCB Is

based an attempt to bring together the various conceptual

streams of CCB into a holistic framework. However, the

development of a general model presumes the existence of

some general laws that do not vanish from product to

product, service to service, or situation to situation.

Empirical support for the existence of such general

relationships is found in a recent study by Richins

(1983), who found that the relationship of problem

severity and retailer responsiveness to complaint actions

was not significantly different across product groups.

Conceptual Foundations of the Model

A holistic framework for the CCB process is

presented in figure 3.1. Consistent with the earlier

discussion on the conceptualization of CS/D and CCB, CCB

is postulated as a process triggered by CS/D. The

intensity or level of consumer dissatisfaction is assumed

to be the underlying motive which determines the amount

of effort expended in the CCB process (Richins 1983; Day

1984). However, since the level of dissatisfaction is

proposed to have no direct impact on the nature and kind

46

Page 57: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

47

of complaint actions chosen by the consumer (Fornell and

Didow 1980), CS/D is not explicitly represented in Figure

3. 1.

Complaining intentions can be conceptualized as the

likelihood that any particular complaint action would be

chosen. Therefore, intentions to engage in specific

complaint actions are proposed to have a one-to-one

correspondence with actual complaint behavior. However,

situational constraints such as cost and frequency of

purchase may result in complaint behaviors that are

inconsistent with intentions (Richins 1982).

It is also proposed that there are two routes that

lead to complaining intentions. One route represents the

psychological perspective that a consumer's attitude

toward the act of complaining is related to his/her

intentions (Richins 1982; Day 1982; Bearden and Crockett

1981). The more positive the attitude, the greater the

likelihood of complaint actions.

The second route represents the economic framework

of Hirschman (1970) based on the concept of expectancy

and value for each alternative course of complaint

action. It is proposed that expectancy-value Judgments

directly affect the consumer's intentions to engage in

corresponding complaint actions. That is, the likelihood

of intentions is greater for an action that has a higher

expectancy-value attached to it. This conceptualization

Page 58: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

48

assumes a multiplicative rule for combining expectancy

and value judgments. Other information processing rules

(lexicographic, etc.) are plausible and should be

investigated in future research.

Generalized affective feelings pertaining to

consumption activities are postulated to be antecedent to

the attitude construct and have no direct affect on the

intentions construct. Westbrook (1980) and Day (1980)

suggest that consumer discontent and alienation from the

market place fit the definition of the generalized

affective feelings construct. Research findings show

that whereas consumer discontent has a positive relation­

ship with attitude towards the act of complaining,

alienation from the market place has a negative relation­

ship (Bearden and Mason 1983; Lundstrom et al. 1979).

While it is hypothesized that consumer discontent is a

concept distinct from alienation and probably inversely

related to it, it is probable that the attitude toward

the act of complaining is related to discontent and

alienation in a nonlinear fashion. That is, attitudes

toward the act of complaining are negative for both, low

levels of discontent and high levels of alienation, with

positive values corresponding to region between these two

extremes. These generalized affective feelings of

discontent and alienation are in turn proposed to be a

function of personality characteristics, demographics,

Page 59: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

49

and the socio-political environment (Jacoby and Jaccard

1981; Lambert 1981).

At least two determinants of expectancy of alterna­

tive complaint actions are proposed: attributions of

dissatisfaction and prior experience in making

complaints. The attributions construct is conceptualized

based on attributional theory of CCB discussed in the

last chapter. That is, external attributions with a high

degree of stability and controllability would result in

higher expectancies of seller responsiveness attached to

these dissatisfactions. Similarly, consumers with

greater prior experience in making complaints would have

higher subjective probabilities that future complaint

actions would be successful. Neither the attributions

nor the the prior experience have a direct affect on

intentions but instead affect intentions indirectly

through their effect on the expectancy-value construct.

It is also proposed that "structural constraints"

(Fornell and Robinson 1983) which reflect the nature of

the industry may affect the expectancies of the various

complaint actions. In the model of figure 3.1 these

structural constraints are contained in the environmental

conditions prevailing for a given purchase episode.

Thus, when consumer dissatisfactions result from products

or services that are produced by an industry that is

known to discourage complaints, the expectancies of

Page 60: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

50

complaint actions would be correspondingly affected.

This link is based on the propositions of Hirschman

(1970).

The value construct attached to each of the alterna­

tive complaint actions is proposed to be a function of

prior knowledge, personality characteristics, and

demographics.

If the expectancy-value Judgments and attitudes are

two routes to the determination of CCB intentions, it is

legitimate to ask: How the expectancy-value Judgment and

attitude constructs interact to determine intentions?

Which route is the dominant mode in a specific situation

of dissatisfaction? Since a general model that incorpo­

rates the psychological and the economic perspectives has

not been proposed before, no conceptual or empirical

answer is suggested by the CCB literature.

However, research on the role of involvement and

prior knowledge in multi-attribute models can provide

some theoretical guidelines to answer this question.

Bagozzi (1983) has proposed a typology that specifies

conditions when expectancy-value Judgments, attitudes, or

both together would be the predominant modes of the

underlying process that result in purchase intentions.

His typology uses the criteria of involvement and prior

knowledge (or learning). The concept of involvement as

used here implies the extent of effort expended in the

Page 61: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

51

particular purchase activity. The corresponding concept

in the CCB process is the level of dissatisfaction since

it too represents the amount of effort that will be

invested in the complaint decision making process

(Richins 1984). The concept of prior knowledge is

similar in both areas, though it is more specific in the

case of CCB process, where it refers only to complaint

actions. Therefore, it can be hypothesized that a

typology similar to that proposed by Bagozzi (1983) may

be suitable for CCB process, when the level of dissatis­

faction is substituted for involvement. This typology is

represented in Figure 3.2. It is reiterated that this

typology is only a hypothesis based on an extension of

general findings in the area of multi-attribute models,

and its applicability to CCB process is open to empirical

investigation.

The interpretation of Figure 3.2 is fairly

straightforward. For instance, when the level of dis­

satisfaction is either medium or high and the consumer

has no previous complaint experience and knowledge (cells

2 and 3), the predominant mode of the CCB process is

hypothesized to be the expectancy-value Judgments.

Similarly, cells 5 and 6 suggest that with substantial

prior knowledge and learning about making complaints, the

tendency to adopt the attitude route for CCB actions is

high for moderate or high levels of dissatisfactions.

Page 62: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

52

For low levels of dissatisfactions, the motivation to

expend effort in the CCB process is missing, and the

predominant mode is either impulse complaint behavior, or

a habitual response depending on the level of prior

knowledge and learning.

One reason for developing a theoretical model of the

CCB process is to facilitate predictions of specific

complaint actions (exit, voice, word-of-mouth, etc. ).

Day (1980) states that such an objective is desirable but

suggests no guidelines for making specific predictions.

Nevertheless, at least some predictions of complaint

actions at a level of specificity greater than general

complaint behavior can be made based on current research

findings. These hypothesized predictions result from

either high or low levels of expectancy value Judgments

and either positive or negative attitudes towards the act

of complaining. This represents a 2 X 2 table of

possible outcomes and is shown in Figure 3.3. The

specific prediction in each of the cells is based on the

work of researchers shown in the respective cells.

Specifically, Figure 3.3 hypothesizes that public actions

of VOICE as well as private actions of WORD-OF-MOUTH

communication would be used under conditions of high

level of expectancy-value Judgments and positive

attitudes toward the act of complaining (cell 1).

Similarly, private actions of WORD-OF-MOUTH communication

Page 63: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

53

and EXIT are hypothesized to be expected when attitudes

are positive but expectancy-value Judgments are low. NO

ACTION is hypothesized to be the most probable response

under conditions of low expectancy-value Judgments

combined with negative attitudes towards the act of

complaining (cell 4). Finally, cell 2 indicates a pheno­

mena that has not been investigated before, that is,

conditions of high expectancy value Judgments but

negative attitudes toward the act of complaining. It is

hypothesized that under the conditions of cell 2,

consumers would either VOICE their complaint (because of

high expectancy-value) or take NO ACTION (because of

negative attitudes). Again, it is important to note that

these are hypothesized predictions which will be tested

in the present research and that previous studies in the

area have not directly examined these relationships

within a single study.

A holistic model for the consumer complaint process

is proposed that brings together the economic perspec­

tive, psychological perspective and the attribution

theory in a well specified framework. However, in order

for this to be useful, this model must be specified in

more detail to enable further theoretical development and

empirical testing. This task is referred to as theory

formalization in the philosophy of science literature and

is addressed in the next section.

Page 64: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

54

Partial Formalization of the Model

Definitions

1. The purchase episode is the consumer's entire

experience in the purchase and consumption of a

particular product or service. Subsequent evalu­

ations of additional experiences with the same

product/service are to be treated as new episodes

(Day, 1980, p. 211).

2. Dissatisfaction is a feeling or emotion triggered by

the consumer's comparison of the rewards and costs

of the purchase in relation to anticipated conse­

quences within a purchase episode (Day, 1984, pp.

496-7).

3. Consumer Complaint Behavior (CCB) is the set of all

non-behavioral and behavioral responses which

involve communicating something negative regarding a

purchase episode including the product/service that

is triggered by dissatisfaction. The complaint

behavior is conceptually distinct from and not

necessarily related to the intensity of dissatisfac­

tion with the purchase episode (Day, 1980, p. 211;

Day, 1984, p. 497; Jacoby and Jaccard, 1981, p. 61;

Fornell and Didow, 1980, p. 319). Further, the

process leading from the dissatisfaction stage to

the complaint response is called the consumer

complaining process.

Page 65: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

55

4. The energizing components or the dynamic aspects of

human personality that activate and sustain a

process and account for its termination are the

motivating forces of the process (McGuire, 1976, p.

302).

5. Attitude is the enduring positive or negative

feeling about some person, object or issue (Petty

and Cacioppo, 1981, pp. 6-7).

6. Factual statements that a person perceives about

other people, objects or issues are termed as

cognitions. Cognitions are action neutral, i. e. ,

they are not charged with attractive and repulsive

characteristics of affect and may be proven to be

true or false in an objective sense. Common examples

of cognitions are beliefs, expectancies and

subjective probabilities (Bagozzi, 1980, pp. 40-2).

7. The subjective probability of an outcome or

expectancy is the cognition a consumer has that a

particular course of action or an event will have

some specific consequences (Landon, 1980, p. 335;

Fornell and Didow, 1980, p. 319).

8. The rational evaluation of the degree of desira­

bility or undesirability of some specific conse­

quence is termed as the value of that consequence

(Landon, 1980, p. 335; Bagozzi, 1980, pp. 41-2, 47).

Page 66: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

56

Axioms

1. The cognitive consistency theories state that there

is a strong tendency for people to maintain

consonance (consistency) among the elements of

cognitive system, i.e., beliefs, subjective

probabilities, expectancies, affect and values.

These theories:

(a) attempt to describe the conditions for

equilibrium and disequilibrium among cognitive

elements,

(b) assert that disequilibrium motivates the person

to restore consistency among the elements, and

(c) describe procedures by which equilibrium might

be accomplished (Petty and Cacioppo, 1981, pp.

126-7).

2. The three major theories of cognitive consistency

are Balance Theory (Heider, 1958), Congruity Theory

(Osgood and Tannenbaum, 1968), and Cognitive

Dissonance Theory (Festinger, 1957).

3. Learning theories attempt to explain the conditions

and the processes by which relatively permanent

changes in the response tendencies including

attitudes, beliefs, evaluations, etc. result from

the effects of prior experiences. Common theories of

learning are Classical Conditioning (Staats and

Staats 1957; 1958), Operant Conditioning (Skinner

Page 67: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

57

1938) and Cognitive Learning (Engel and Blackwell,

1982, pp. 236-63).

4. A consumer's dissatisfaction with a purchase

episode is the net response resulting from the

expectation level, plus or minus a magnitude of

disconfirmation. This disconfirmation of expec­

tations as a phenomena is explained by many theories

such as Adaptation Level Theory which proposes that:

(a) Expectations are individual's perceived

Judgments of how a product/service should per­

form based on a number of factors including

prior experiences, word-of-mouth, manufac­

turer's reputation and advertising, etc.

(b) The perceived performance is the result of

experience with the current purchase episode.

(c) Disconfirmation is the distance or the discre­

pancy between (a) and (b) above. Complaining

behavior only results when (a) is greater than

(b) (Oliver, 1980, pp. 206-9).

Law-Like Statements

1. If a consumer X is dissatisfied with a particular

purchase episode, then, a necessary but not

sufficient condition for engaging in complaint

response exists (Fornell and Didow, 1980, p. 319).

2. Consumer complaint responses result from specific

aspects of experiences with particular products or

Page 68: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

58

services (as opposed to generalized feelings about

the market system) (Day, 1980, p. 211).

3. Consumer complaint response in specific situations

is a function of (a) intentions to engage in

complaining responses in order to obtain desirable

payoffs (Landon, 1977, p. 33), and (b) certain

situational factors such as the item's cost, and

frequency of patronizing (Richins, 1982, p. 503;

Landon, 1977, p. 33).

4. Consumer complaint intentions result from one or

both of the following conditions:

(a) psychological reactions favorable or unfavor­

able to taking actions, i.e., attitudes toward

the act of complaining (Day, 1980, p. 214).

(b) a rational analysis of the benefits and useful­

ness of taking any plausible courses of action

(i.e. expectancy-value) of each of the possible

actions (Day, 1980, p. 214).

5. The expectancy value of each of the plausible

courses of action (in a specific experience) is a

multiplicative function of both:

(a) the value gained from a successful action, and

(b) the probability or expectancy of achieving a

successful action (Hirschman, 1970;Landon,

1980, p. 335; Fornell and Didow, 1980, pp. 318-

19).

Page 69: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

59

6. Previous experience in and knowledge about (a)

seeking redress in similar or other situations, (b)

buying and using the particular product/service and

(c) sellers policies, laws and consumerism are

determinants of a consumer's estimate of the proba­

bility of achieving a successful complaint action

(Day et al., 1981, pp. 95-6). This generalization is

supported by theories of learning (e.g., classical

conditioning and cognitive learning) (Bagozzi, 1982,

p. 572).

7. Affective feelings or attitude toward the act of

complaining will directly affect complaining

intentions. This generalization is due to theories

of motivation, learning and purposeful behavior.

Consumers will be motivated to engage in those

actions that lead to satisfaction of needs and to

avoid those actions which are aversive (Bagozzi,

1982, p. 574).

8. The direct path from expectancy value Judgments to

complaining intentions results from a rational

consumer's choice based on preferences or values of

all alternatives and the availability of those

alternatives (probability of successful actions).

This is the economic theory of consumer choice

applied to post-purchase processes (Fornell and

Didow, 1980, p. 318-9).

Page 70: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

60

9. The relationship between attitudes toward the act of

complaining and expectancy value Judgments of alter­

native complaining actions is characterized by the

following process:

(a) They would tend to remain in balance driven by

cognitive-affective consistency requirements

(Bagozzi, 1982, p. 574).

(b) Affect can be expected to influence expectancy

value processes in contexts in which (i) the

behavior is impulsive or (ii) the affective

reaction originates from a relatively strong

arousing stimulus (Bagozzi, 1982, p. 574).

(c) On the other hand, expectanc/ value Judgments

of complaining actions can be expected to

influence attitudes toward the act of com­

plaining in instances when the purchase episode

evokes beliefs and evaluations of actions/-

consequences that are cognitively arousing. The

mechanism of this influence is provided by

balance theory (Heider 1958) and the drive to

maintain cognitive-affective consistency

(McGuire, 1968).

10. Relatively stable influences pertaining to aspects

of the marketing system and the domain of consump­

tion (e.g., the goods and services offered in the

market place, business practices, attitudes towards

Page 71: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

61

the business, consumerism as well as sentiments of

pervasive consumer discontent), are distinct from

transient affective influences (i.e., temporary

favorable or unfavorable sentiments evoked by

specific purchase episodes). However, the former

have predictive relationship with the latter (Day,

1980, p. 211; Westbrook, 1980, p. 50).

11. The stable affective influences pertaining to

aspects of the marketing system and the domain of

consumption result from or are correlated with:

(a) Demographic characteristics including age,

income, social class, family life cycle, etc.

(Bearden and Mason, 1983, p. 6-7).

(b) Personality variables including assertiveness,

aggressiveness, etc. (Richins, 1983, p. 73-4;

Fornell and Westbrook, 1979, pp. 105-6).

(c) The cultural, economic and political environ­

ment (Day et al., 1981, pp. 99-104).

12. The extent of dissatisfaction with the purchase

episode and the importance of the product/service

are the underlying motivating forces of the consumer

complaining behavior (Day, 1984, p. 497).

Discussion

This chapter first proposed a holistic model of

consumer complaint behavior that would incorporate within

Page 72: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

62

a single model previous empirical findings and the

different conceptual frameworks for the study of CCB.

Second, the proposed model was partially formalized to

enable further theoretical development and empirical

testing. It is not claimed that the proposed holistic

model is "correct"--only empirical testing can Justify

the truth content of a model or theory. However, the

model is (a) consistent with past research, (b) helpful

in summarizing past research, (c) useful in providing

directions for future research, and (d) amenable to

empirical testing. This dissertation proposal presents a

framework for the empirical investigation of a part of

this model. The next chapter discusses the operationali­

zation and development of hypotheses followed by research

methodology to be adopted.

Page 73: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

63

—1/»

a:

D c/i ft: z o o u

n U] at: ID a

o

> < X UI OQ

- ] U ID Q Z a M E Z U M H cn M .J o X

<

a. o u

Page 74: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

64

LEV

EL

OF

D

ISS

AT

ISF

AC

TIO

N

LOW

5

5 X

PRIOR EXPERIENCE OF Q>f>L;\iNiNG

LOW

IffVLSE BEHAVIOR

EXPECTANCY-VALUE

JUDGErtNTS

EXPECTANCY-VALUE

JlJDGE^€NTS

M I ^

HABITS

ATTITUEE -••--=13 THE

ACT OF C O M = ' _ - ; N I N G

ATTITUDE TOWARD THE

ACT OF COMPLAINING

FIGURE 3.2

A TYPOLOGY FOR THE NATURE OF CCB PROCESSES

Page 75: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

65

z

1

.a J

J

E

PO

SIT

IVE

N

EG

ATI

VE

EXPECTANCY-VALUE ,IUDGEA€VTS

HIGH

'VOICE'

WORD-OF-MDUTH C0^nJNICATI0N

corf LA IN TO PUBLIC AGENCIES

7

'VOICE' OR 'EXIT'

LOW

'EXIT '

WORD-OF-MOUTH co^^UJICATION

NO ACTION

FIGURE 3.3

A MODEL FOR PREDICTING SPECIFIC CONSUMER COMPLAINT ACTIONS

Page 76: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 4

OPERATIONALIZATION AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF KEY HYPOTHESES

In chapter 3, a theoretical framework is developed

based on current literature in the CS/D and CCB area.

Then a formalization of this theory is attempted to

rigorously define the relationships among the various

"terms" and "concepts" in the theory. However, theories

that purport to explain and predict phenomena must

satisfy the "empirically testable" criterion (Hunt 1983,

pp. 243-8). That is, the relationships predicted by

theory ought to be empirically observable and either

supported or rejected based on these observations. Since

theories, being generalized conditionals, are not

directly testable, the task undertaken in this chapter

is to derive predictive type statements from the theory

that can then directly confront empirical data. These

predictive type statements derived from the theory are

referred to as Hypotheses. The confrontation of

hypotheses with empirical data would lead to either

rejection or acceptance of these hypotheses, which then

suggests an empirical evidence of lack of support or

corroboration of theoretical relationships.

Hunt (1983) proposes the philosophy of science

perspective on deriving hypotheses:

66

Page 77: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

67

The requirement that research hypotheses be predictive type statements that are amenable to direct confrontation with data implies that all of the descriptive terms in the statements (or theory) must have rules of interpretation containing empirical referants (sometimes referred to as opera­tional definitions, empirical indicators or epistemic correlations).

In other words, first the concepts or constructs in

the theory must be operationalized into empirical

referants or entities and then predictive type statements

called hypotheses can be stated in terms of these

empirical entities. Therefore, hypotheses are statements

of predicted relationships among empirical entities and

only indirectly refer to any concepts or constructs in

the theory.

What are these empirical entities? Bagozzi (1980)

suggests that empirical entities or concepts are observa­

ble concepts that achieve their meaning through

operational definitions that specify procedures for

measuring observations in the world of experience. In

other words, operationalization of theoretical constructs

implies a definition of an empirical concept that has

correspondence with the theoretical construct as well as

the procedures for measuring the empirical concept.

Thus the purpose of the chapter is twofold: First,

to operationalize the theory of consumer complaining

behavior and the constructs therein; and second, to

develop specific hypotheses based on these

operationalizations.

Page 78: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

66

Operationalization of Key Constructs

One desirable criterion for the operationalization

of theoretical constructs is to incorporate corres­

pondence rules for individual concepts that have either

been employed or suggested in the literature. This would

assist in tying current research with previous research

findings and thus be useful in the accumulation of the

body of knowledge in the area. Therefore, the objective

in this section is to suggest operationalizations of the

concepts in the holistic model of consumer complaining

behavior that meet the above criterion and hence provide

a link with previous research.

The Dissatisfying Experience

The theory conceptualization defines the consumer's

experience of dissatisfaction with a purchase episode as

the trigger that activates the process of consumer

complaining. However, consumers experience different

dissatisfactions with different products and services.

Bad appliances can be repaired, whereas "bad" health

services can neither be "repaired" nor "refunded" in the

true sense. Therefore, the operationalization of dis­

satisfying experience ought to involve a sampling across

products or services as well as a sampling across expe­

riences.

Most of the research in this area operationalizes

Page 79: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

69

the dissatisfying experience using recall information for

the "recent dissatisfying experience" (Richins 1983;

Bearden and Mason 1983; Day et al. 1981; Villarreal-

Camacho 1983; Fornell and Westbrook 1979; Andreasen 1977;

Best and Andreasen 1975). It is expected that dissatis­

fying experiences are not often forgotten, irrespective

of action taken. Therefore, recall is used as the opera­

tionalization for the construct of the dissatisfying

purchase episode. The expectancy value Judgments and

intended actions are then measured relative to a similar

but hypothetical incident occurring again in the future.

Four service groups will be investigated in the

present research: health services, auto-repairs, banking

services and grocery retailing. Auto-repairs are reported

to elicit a large number of voiced complaints, whereas

the response to dissatisfaction with grocery retailers is

generally to exit (Best and Andreasen 1975). Banking has

rarely been investigated while health services are sug­

gested to restrict both the voice and the exit options of

dissatisfied consumers (Andreasen 1983). Therefore, this

selection of service groups seems to provide some varia­

tion in the complaint responses and, hopefully, in the

underlying mechanism of the CCB.

Level of Dissatisfaction

The level of dissatisfaction is conceptualized in

the theory as the underlying motivating force in the CCB

Page 80: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

70

process. It is not, however, directly modeled in the

process of complaining.

Westbrook (1980a) investigated the different

operationalizations of the satisfaction/dissatisfaction

construct. Based on this study, a percentage scale is

selected to measure the level of dissatisfaction. Since,

from a theoretical standpoint, initial feelings of

dissatisfaction are distinct from final reaction, the

percentage scale is used to measure the two levels of

dissatisfactions individually. The measurement property

of the percentage scale of dissatisfaction has been

discussed by Westbrook (1980a) and its reliability ranges

from 0.65 to 0.88 depending on the kind of product.

Prior Experience

The operationalization of prior experience as a

consumer and in complaining actions is suggested by Day

(1984). The operational measure is a four item scale

which taps the extent of previous experience respondents

have had in the various complaint actions. Specifically,

the scale attempts to measure the frequency with which

respondents have engaged in VOICEing their complaints

either directly to stores or manufacturers, in taking

FORMAL actions, such as to report to the Better Business

Bureau, in Word-of-Mouth communication to friends and

relatives and in taking LEGAL actions. The higher the

Page 81: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

71

value of the index on this measure, the higher is the

prior experience of engaging in consumer activities and

complaining behavior. The measurement properties of this

scale will be investigated as a part of this research.

Stable Affective Influences

The construct of stable affective influences is

defined as those attitudinal structures that pertain to

the domain of consumption or market-place, are relatively

stable over time, and are not specific to an incident or

a situation. One operationalization of this concept sug­

gested by Westbrook (1980) is the 82-item consumer

discontent scale developed in the marketing literature by

Lundstrom and Lament (1976). Consumer Discontent is

defined to be:

the collection of attitudes held by the consumer toward the product strategies of business, business communications and information, the impersonal nature of business and retail institutions, and the broader socio-economic forces which are linked with the business system (Lundstrom and Lament 1976, p. 374).

Thus the attitude of consumer discontent is fairly

generalized, pertains to the marketing system, and

appears to be generally stable over time (Westbrook

1980).

Another operationalization of the stable affective

influences that has often been used in the CCB literature

is the 35-item consumer alienation from the market-place

scale developed by Allison (1978). The operational

Page 82: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

72

concept, consumer alienation is defined as:

the feelings of separation from the norms and values of the market-place. Such a state includes a lack of acceptance of or identification with market institutions, practices, and outputs as well as feelings of separation from the self when one is involved in the consumption role (Allison 1978, p. 570).

This conceptualization fits well the definition of stable

affective influences pertaining to the domain of

consumption.

These two operationalizations of stable affective

influences, namely discontent and alienation, are

frequently employed in the CCB research and appear to be

useful in explaining the CCB process (Bearden and Mason

1983, Day 1984). Thus both operationalizations will be

used in the present study. The alpha reliability of the

discontent and alienation scales is reported to be 0. 94

(Lundstrom and Lament 1976) and 0.8802 (Allison 1978),

respectively.

A particular operational problem with the use of

consumer discontent and alienation from the market-place

scales in survey research is the long length of the two

instruments. This is especially so for the 85-item

consumer discontent scale. It would be helpful if the

instrument could be reduced to manageable length without

losing much of the correspondence between the theoretical

construct and the empirical measure. This research will

also attempt to do so, as detailed later.

Page 83: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

73

Attitude Toward the Act of Complaining

Richins (1982) has attempted to operationalize the

theoretical concept of psychological costs/benefits and

favorable or unfavorable reactions, that is, attitudes

toward the act of complaining. Richins's (1982) empirical

instrument has 15 items and purports to measure 3 dimen­

sions of the attitude concept. The three dimensions are:

(a) generalized attitudes regarding the trouble involved

in complaining, (b) a personal norm concerning com­

plaining, that is, appropriateness or inappropriateness

of complaining as a normative issue, and, (c) societal

benefits of complaining, that is, feelings regarding the

positive or negative impact on society of registering

complaints.

Richins (1982) has demonstrated external validity of

this operational measure by empirically supporting its

predictive relationship with propensity or intentions to

complain. Other researchers have also investigated the

measurement properties for similar measures of the

attitudes towards the act of complaining scale (Bearden

and Mason 1983; Bearden and Crockett 1981).

Day (1984) has proposed an operational measure of

this construct which shares some items with the Richins

(1982) operationalization. Day (1984), however, shows no

empirical support for the reliability and validity of his

Page 84: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

74

operational measure.

It is proposed to operationalize the attitude toward

the act of complaining construct with an 18 item scale

composed of items from Richins (1982) and Day (1984).

This measure is composed of three facets similar to those

described by Richins (1982). The general facet has 10

items, and the personal norm and societal benefit facets

have four each. The possibility of an underlying general-

factor in these 18 items is unknown and is proposed to be

investigated as a part of this research.

Attributions of Blame

Krishnan and Valle (1979), Foikes (1984) and other

researchers have operationalized the construct of

attributions within the context of consumer complaining

behavior. The theoretical construct of attributions,

which implies imputing causal inferences to felt dis­

satisfactions, is operationalized by Foikes (1984) to

measure its 3 dimensions. These dimensions are: (a)

locus, that is, cause of dissatisfaction located in the

consumer or external to him, (b) stability, that is, the

relatively temporary or fairly permanent nature of the

cause of dissatisfaction, and, (c) controllability, that

is, the extent of volitional or nonvolitional nature of

the cause. Krishnan and Valle (1979), however, operation­

alized only the locus dimension of the attributions and

found its effects on CCB significant. Foikes (1984) in

Page 85: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

75

her study found that the locus and stability dimensions

were most useful in explaining redress/future behavior

types of consumer complaining behavior.

Therefore, for the purpose of this study a five item

scale measuring the locus and stability dimensions of

attributions is defined as the operational referant of

the attributions construct. The scale is based on the two

studies cited above, but is adapted to suit each service

industry investigated.

Expectancy-Value Judgments

The construct of expectancy value Judgments has been

theoretically defined to be a multiplicative function of

value or benefit gained from a course of action and the

subjective probability that such an action would be

successful. Though the importance of this construct in

predicting and explaining consumer complaining behavior

was suggested in 1970 by Hirschman, few empirical

measures exist in the CCB literature. Therefore, it is

necessary that operationlizations of this construct be

developed for the purpose of this study.

Fortunately, guidelines for developing this opera­

tional measure are available from the area of multi-

attribute models in consumer behavior. A large number of

empirical studies have operationalized the expectancy-

value construct for product choices (Bagozzi 1982).

Page 86: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

76

Essentially, the operationalization ought to measure

the subjective probability of success and the importance

for each of the alternative courses of actions available.

A 15 item scale is thus developed for measuring the

expectancy and value Judgments separately. These items

are classified into three sets reflecting Judgments in

case of (a) word-of-mouth communication to friends and

relatives, (b) reporting to the seller or manufacturer,

and (c) complaining to a consumer organization or public

agency. The specific items in each of these sets reflect

the seeking redress and changing future behavior

dimensions of the various actions. However, expectancy-

value (E-V) Judgments for exit or no action are not

measured since they are expected to result from low

values of E-V Judgments for each of these three sets. The

measurement properties of the E-V scale are proposed to

be investigated as a part of this research.

Complaining Actions

The construct of complaining actions is directly

observable and thus requires no correspondence rules for

connecting it with some empirical concept. It is itself,

by definition, an empirical construct. However, as

defined in the classification schemata there are certain

actions such as word-of-mouth communication to friends or

relatives, or a personal decision to exit, that are

difficult, if not impossible, to observe directly. In

Page 87: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

77

such cases, we are constrained to accept self-report data

on certain measures as valid representations of the

construct.

One such measure for complaining actions is proposed

by Day et al. (1981). This nine-item scale attempts to

measure responses to each of the nine possible courses of

complaining actions. Bearden and Teel (1983) operation­

alized the complaining behavior construct based on the

measure of Day et al. (1981) using a five-item scale.

They also investigated the measurement properties of the

scale and demonstrated that it conformed to a Guttman

conceptualization. That is, increasing agreement with the

items reflects an increasing intensity of engaging in

complaining actions. The coefficient of reproducibility

is reported to be 0.98 with an index of scalability of

0.78 (Bearden and Teel 1983).

For the purposes of this research, the operation­

alization of complaining behavior is a twelve-item scale

that is based on the five items of Bearden and Teel

(1983) and nine items of Day et al. (1981). The scale is

conceptualized to be Guttman type, and its measurement

properties are proposed to be investigated as a part of

this research.

Summary

The operationalizations of the various constructs in

Page 88: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

78

the proposed holistic model of CCB are summarized in

Table 4.1. Other constructs in the theory but not

investigated in the present research are not discussed

here. Some operationalizations for these constructs are

available in the literature, such as for situational

variables (Richins 1982). In general, the research in

this area is still in its infancy and a focused study of

the key constructs in the theory would contribute to the

development of the body of knowledge.

Based on the above operationalizations of the

salient constructs, the next section states and discusses

the key hypotheses to be investigated in the present

research.

Key Hypotheses

The first part of the study investigates the

relationship among consumer discontent, alienation from

the market-place and attitude toward the act of

complaining. Lundstrom and Lament (1976) as well as

Lundstrom et al. (1979) show that consumer discontent is

conceptually distinct from alienation and inversely

related to it. In other words, consumers with high levels

of discontent are not necessarily alienated from the

market-place (Lundstrom et al. 1979, p. 154).

Yet Allison (1978) and Lambert (1980) among other

researchers suggest that consumer discontent and

alienation are similar concepts and are positively

Page 89: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

79

related. An empirical study that incorporates both these

measures and directly addresses the question of the

relationship between discontent and alienation has not

yet been attempted.

Therefore, it is hypothesized that:

HI: The constructs of consumer discontent and alienation

from the market place are distinct concepts, possess

divergent validity, discriminant validity, and are

inversely related.

Research by Bearden and Mason (1983) and others

shows that alienated consumers express feelings of

helplessness, powerlessness, meaninglessness,

normlessness and cultural estrangement in their interac­

tion with the market-place. Thus, these alienated

consumers feel that any actions of complaining against

businesses would be unrewarding and fruitless.

Other empirical findings, for instance by Lambert

(1980), suggest that alienated consumers have higher

feelings of dissatisfaction with the market-place and may

complain vociferously to acquaintances, friends,

relatives, consumer organizations, and public agencies.

They may even take legal action.

Thus it is hypothesized that:

H2: The greater the perceived alienation from the market

Page 90: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

80

place, the less positive the attitudes towards the

act of complaining.

Many empirical studies have investigated the rela­

tionship between consumer discontent and the complaining

behavior. Westbrook (1980) found that consumers with

lower feelings of discontent had higher levels of satis­

faction with automobile products. Lundstrom and Lament

(1976) reported that a highly consumerist group with

higher propensity of engaging in complaining behavior

also had higher levels of discontent with the market­

place.

However, theory suggests that stable affective

feelings such as consumer discontent influence com­

plaining behavior through their effect on attitudes

toward the act of complaining. Little empirical evidence

is available regarding the relationship between the

affective feelings of consumer discontent and the

attitude concepts.

Therefore, it is hypothesized that:

H3: As the discontent with the businesses increases, the

attitudes toward the act of complaining tend to be

more positive.

Hirschman (1970) suggests that the value gained from

a successful complaint times the probability of that

successful outcome, that is, E-V Judgments, determine the

Page 91: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

81

probability of voicing complaints. From a theoretical

standpoint, Fornell and Didow (1980) and Landon (1980)

support Hirschman's prediction. Day (1980) has also

suggested that consumer complaining behavior may be

partly predicted by a rational analysis of the benefits

and usefulness of each of the alternative courses of

action. Yet, no empirical investigation of this predic­

tion is reported in the literature.

Thus it is proposed to empirically examine:

H4: The higher the expectancy-value Judgments of each

of the various courses of complaining actions, the

stronger the intentions to engage in complaining

behavior.

Richins (1982) empirically investigated the rela­

tionship between attitudes toward the act of complaining

and the actual complaining behavior. She found that atti­

tudes are significantly related to both the propensity to

complain and self reported complaint behavior. Other

researchers (Bearden and Mason 1983; Bearden and Crockett

1981) have also found empirical support for this rela­

tionship.

From a theoretical standpoint. Day (1980) and

Richins (1979) suggest that attitudes toward the act of

complaining will directly affect complaining intentions

due to theories of motivation, learning, and purposeful

Page 92: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

82

behavior. Thus for instance, if consumers have positive

feelings towards complaining, they will be motivated to

engage in such behavior.

Therefore, it is hypothesized that:

H5: The more positive the attitude towards the act of

complaining, the greater the tendency to engage in

complaining actions when faced with a dissatisfying

experience.

Day (1981, 1980) has theoretically explored the

issue of how the expectancy-value Judgments and attitudes

interact to determine intentions to complain. He posits,

"then it might be feasible to combine the two indexes

(the E-V Judgments and the attitudes) to predict whether

or not action would be taken" (Day 1980, p. 215). Yet he

provides no practical guidelines to combine these two

concepts. Other researchers have usually ignored the

issue.

However, this problem of combining E-V Judgments and

attitudes is addressed in the area of consumer behavior

in the case of product or brand choice. Bagozzi (1983)

has attempted to resolve the issue using the concepts of

involvement with the product or service and prior know­

ledge or learning. His conceptualization has been

modified to be applicable to CCB using the concepts of

level of dissatisfaction and prior experience of com-

Page 93: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

83

plaining. This modified conceptualization is discussed

in chapter 3 as a part of the development of the holistic

model of CCB.

Based on Figure 3.2, the hypothesized typology of

the predominant mode of CCB process, the following

hypotheses can be developed.

H6: For moderate to high levels of dissatisfaction, the

greater the prior knowledge and experience in

complaining, the stronger the relationship between

attitudes and intentions, and the weaker the

relationship between expectancy-value

Judgments and intentions.

H7: Similarly, for moderate or high levels of dissatis­

faction, the lower the prior experience and knowle­

dge in making complaints, the weaker the relation

between attitudes and intentions, and the stronger

the relationship between expectancy value Judgments

and intentions to complain.

H8: For lower levels of dissatisfaction, the intentions

to engage in complaining behavior are dependent

either only on prior experiences (i.e., habits) or

on impulse reactions.

The effect of attributions of product failure on

consumer complaining actions was empirically investigated

by Foikes (1983). She concluded that predictive ability

Page 94: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

84

of attributional approach is limited, though attributions

of product failure may seem to be more useful as deter­

minants of expectancies of various outcomes. Richins

(1979) also theoretically posed that expectancy-value

Judgments may result from such attributions. No empirical

investigation of this theoretical link has yet been

reported.

Thus it is hypothesized that:

H9: The greater the dissatisfaction is attributed to

the members of the distribution channel, rather than

to the consumer (external attributions), the higher

the expectancy-value Judgments perceived by

consumers. These attributions have no direct affect

on the intentions to complain but only affect

intentions indirectly through expectancy value

Judgments.

It is also hypothesized that feelings of consumer

discontent and alienation from market-place directly

affect attitudes but have no direct affect on intentions

to complain. Accordingly, Bearden and Mason (1983) found

alienation a poor predictor of consumer complaining

actions.

Therefore, it is hypothesized that:

H10: The feelings of alienation from the market-place

Page 95: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

85

and consumer discontent have only indirect effects

on intentions to engage in complaining behavior.

This indirect effect is through attitudes towards

the act of complaining.

Much of the earlier research in CCB is often

criticized to be descriptive in nature (Robinson 1979).

Most of it described the demographic correlates, such as

age, sex, income, social class, etc., of complainers and

non-complainers. Thus it became a well-established

finding that complainers are usually younger, highly

educated, and belong to higher social classes.

In an insightful article, Gronhaug and Zaltman

(1981) suggested that the demographic correlates of

complainers, so often found in the CCB literature, may be

mere artifacts of market-place participation. That is,

consumers who are more involved in the market-place would

expect to have higher probabilities of being exposed to

buying problems. In the CCB model discussed in the

present research, the differences in these participation

levels are directly modeled by measuring expectancy-value

Judgments and attitudes. Therefore, it can be

hypothesized that:

Hll: The demographic variables of age, sex, income and

social class have no direct effects on intentions to

engage in complaining behavior but have indirect

Page 96: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

86

effect through the constructs of expectancy-value

Judgments and the attitudes towards that act of

complaining.

Richins (1983) empirically investigated the rela­

tionship between expectancy-value Judgments of retailer

responsiveness and complaining actions across two product

groups. She found the relationship between the two

concepts to be similar across the two product groups.

This finding supports the possibility of generalized

structural relationships that are not dependent on the

particular product or service. An empirical investigation

of the structural relationships across services is not

evident in the literature. Therefore, it can be hypothe­

sized that:

H12: The strength and direction of structural relation­

ships among expectancy-value Judgments, attitudes,

and intentions is similar across the four services

investigated in the present research.

Day (1984) among other researchers has suggested

that one key underlying rationale for proposing a process

conceptualization of CCB is that the level of dissatis­

faction alone is found to have rather limited ability in

predicting CCB. For Instance, Bearden and Teel (1983)

could explain only 15% of the variation in CCB using the

level of dissatisfaction. Thus the process model of CCB

Page 97: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

87

ought to perform better than the naive model of Bearden

and Teel (1983), An empirical investigation of the

explanatory power of the two models in a single study has

not been found in the literature. It is thus hypothe­

sized that:

H13: The variance explained in the intentions to engage

in complaining behavior by the process model of

expectancy-value Judgments and attitudes is higher

than that explained by the degree of dissatisfaction

alone.

One of the key interests in developing a model of

CCB process is to be able to predict not only consumer

complaining behavior in general, but also specific

complaint actions, such as exit, word-of-mouth, voice,

etc. A framework for predicting specific complaint

actions is presented in chapter 3 as a part of the

holistic model of CCB (see Figure 3.3). Based on this

framework, the following hypotheses can be generated.

H14: For high levels of expectancy value Judgments of

seller responsiveness together with positive

attitudes towards complaining, the intentions to

take public actions would be high. The most pre­

ferred action would be voice to seller and W-O-M to

friends and relatives (based on Hirschman 1970; Day

Page 98: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

88

1980, pp. 214-5).

H15: Low levels of expectancy value Judgments of seller

responsiveness and positive attitudes towards

complaining would in general result in higher pro­

pensity for W-O-M communication to friends and

relatives and/or exit actions (based on Richins

1983, pp. 74-6).

H16: In the case of low levels of expectancy value

Judgments of seller responsiveness and negative

attitudes towards complaining, the most preferred

complaint behavior would be to take no action (based

on Day 1981, pp. 93-6).

The nature of intentions to act in the case of high

levels of expectancy value Judgments of seller respon­

siveness together with negative attitudes towards com­

plaining are not previously recorded in the literature.

Since expectancy value Judgments are perceptions of

sellers responsiveness to VOICEd complaints combined with

value of outcomes, it may be expected that when E-V

Judgments are high, the propensity to take VOICE actions

would also be high. When high E-V Judgments occur along

with negative attitude toward the act of complaining, the

intentions for VOICE actions may be decreased somewhat,

yet VOICE may remain the most preferred action because of

the high probability of the seller responding positively

to the VOICEd complaints. Thus it is proposed that:

Page 99: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

89

H17: If the level of expectancy value Judgments is high

but attitude toward the act of complaining is

negative, voice may still be the most preferred

action.

It is also desirable, from a managerial and public

policy standpoint to investigate the variation across the

different industries. Hirschman (1970) suggests that in

the most competitive industries, the channels for exit as

well as voice are open, whereas in monopolies, only voice

is the viable alternative. Andreasen (1983) has further

elaborated on "loose monopolies" such as health services,

where opportunities for both voice and exit are generally

blocked. It can, therefore, be hypothesized:

H18: When the level of dissatisfaction is controlled for,

the mean levels of expectancy Judgments are highest

for services that are perceived to be supplied by

most competitive industries, lowest for loose

monopolies, and somewhere in between for monopolies.

Discussion

This chapter has attempted to elaborate the various

hypotheses to be investigated in the present research.

The key objectives in their development are: (a) to

attempt to seek empirical testing of the structural

relationships suggested by the holistic model of CCB,

Page 100: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

90

and (b) to examine those predictions that may have

significant theoretical, managerial and/or public policy

implications.

The next chapter provides the details of the survey

procedures and methods used to collect data for phase I

and phase II of this dissertation research. This is then

followed by data analysis and summary of results.

Page 101: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

TABLE 4.1

OPERATIONALIZATION OF KEY CONSTRUCTS IN THE HOLISTIC MODEL

91

S. No.

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

CONSTRUCT

LEVEL OF DISSATISFACTION

PRIOR EXPERIENCE.

BASIC AFFECTIVE INFLUENCES.

ATTITUDE TOWARD THE ACT OF COMPLAINING.

ATTRIBUTIONS OF -BLAME'.

EXPECTANCY OF COMPLAINT ACTIONS.

VALUE OF COMPLAINT ACTIONS.

INTENTIONS TO COMPLAIN.

OPERATIONAL MEASURE

TWO SINGLE ITEM RATING SCALES.

BASED ON FIVE ITEM CONSUMER'S KNOWLEDGE AND EXPERIENCE SCALE

(a) 82 ITEM CONSUMER DISCONTENT SCALE.

(b) 35 ITEM ALIENATIO^ FROM THE MARKET--PLACE SCALE.

Based on (a) 15 ITEM ATTITUDES

SCALE, and

^b) 10 ir^M ATTiriioE TOWARD 'WE ACr OF COMPLAINING SCALE

PROPOSED FIVE ITEM SCALE.

PROPOSED 15 ITEM SCALE.

PROPOSED 15 ITEM SCALE.

Based on (a) 9 ITEM ACTIONS

SCALE. AND

(b) 5 ITEM COMPLAINT ACTIONS SCALE.

DEVELOPED 8r

WESTBBOOf.dHO JM. pp. 58-'2.

DAY, (19«4) ACR Proc.

LAMONT, ( H ; 6 ) .

ALLISON. (1978).

bOt*1 in 'MO .

RICHINS. i\^^2).

•^Ay. (\ '-: .

KRISHNAN AND vALLE (19791. and FOL<ES ( 19«4».

Bas°'J on BAGOZZI (1982).

•HAGn^ZI ( I98?i.

OA' ET «L.'1981.

ciFARDEN AND '•fC'

PHOPCSTIES

ALPHA=G.65 to 0.88

HOT

REPOR'tO.

ALPHA^O.94

ALPHA»0.88

NOT BEPURfED.

NOT REPORTED,

ro RE INVESTIGATED

ro a INVESTIGATED

'•O BE INVESTIGATED

'GUTTMAN' rrPE.

REPR0.'0.98 scat*.'0.78

Page 102: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 5

SURVEY PROCEDURES AND METHODS

This chapter focuses on the survey process itself--

addressing questions such as how the sample was selected,

how the survey was conducted, and the attempts made to

reduce nonresponse error. These aspects are important

since the quality of data is dependent largely on the

manner in which the data is collected.

The Two Phases of Research

The research was conducted in two phases, hereafter

referred to as phase I and phase II, In phase I investi­

gation is limited to three constructs: discontent, alie­

nation, and attitude towards the act of complaining. The

objective of phase I was twofold: (a) to test the rela­

tionships among these constructs specified by hypotheses

HI, H2 and H3 (see chapter 4), and (b) to purify the

scales used for measuring these constructs so as to

reduce them to a manageable length while minimizing loss

of information. The specific questionnaire used in phase

I of this dissertation research is as per Appendix A.

Phase II was conducted subsequent to the completion

of phase I. The purpose of phase II is to test the

remaining hypotheses, H4 to HIS. The instrument for this

phase contains all constructs operationalized in chapter

92

Page 103: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

93

4 including the shortened versions of the alienation,

discontent and attitudes scales obtained as a result of

phase I analysis. Further, each questionnaire for this

phase measures responses to dissatisfying experiences in

one of the four selected industries. That is, each

respondent would provide complete responses to only one

dissatisfying experience for one randomly assigned indus­

try. The survey methods and procedures adopted for phase

I and II are now discussed in turn.

Phase I

Selection of Census Tracts

A two stage area sampling plan was employed for

phase I. The first stage involved selection of census

tracts with in the city of Lubbock. The second stage

involved a systematic selection of households from the

pre-selected tracts (e.g., every fifth member). The

critical step here is the selection of tracts to ensure a

representative sample. The sampling unit is a household,

and a sample size of 1000 households was selected to

ensure generalizability of findings.

Census tracts were divided into four groups based on

median household income as tabulated by the Census of

Population: (1) up to $10,000, (2) from $10,001 to

$15,000, (3) from $15,001 to $20,000, and (4) greater

than $20,000. Using random number tables, two tracts

Page 104: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

94

were selected from group 1 (12.02 and 13), another two

from group 2 (23 and 3) and one each from group 3 (20)

and group 4 (17.02). Within each of these tracts, the

interviewers were instructed to select every fifth house­

hold along each block in the tract to be a part of the

sample.

Method of Data Collection

Undergraduate business students were recruited to

drop off questionnaires to the selected sample of house­

holds. An effort was made while selecting the students

to match their characteristics with those of the

potential respondents. The selected group of students

consisted of 4 white, 1 black and 1 Hispanic. They were

trained to interact with and request participation from

the potential respondents in an appropriate manner. A

text of the standard introduction to be used by each

student in seeking cooperation of the household is

enclosed in Appendix B. Each student was also instructed

to wear, at all times, a label clearly showing his name

and the sponsoring party (Texas Tech). The dress code

selected was informal since it was believed that a formal

attire may give an impression of a "door-to-door"

salesman.

Two methods were adopted for pick-up of completed

surveys in phase I. Approximately one-half of the total

sample (that is 500) was provided with a stamped return

Page 105: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

95

envelope to mail back completed responses. For the

remaining, the students were instructed to go back after

a week and pick up the completed responses. In the

latter method the students were instructed to inform the

respondent of the time and day of the pick up at the time

they delivered the survey. The two methods were employed

to study the differences in response rates when different

methods for collecting the responses were adopted.

However, this particular study did not constitute a part

of this dissertation research. Further, irrespective of

the method, the students were trained to keep a record of

the addresses of the households who agreed to participate

in the survey. This was done to ensure proper follow up

and callbacks.

The phase I survey was conducted on all days of the

week excluding Sunday (for religious reasons) and Friday

(end of week syndrome). The survey was conducted between

4.30 p.m. and 8 p.m. on all days of the week, and between

9 a.m. and 1 p.m. on Saturday. It was believed that the

probability of contacting the respondents would be high

during these time slots. An effort was made to contact

Not-at-Homes during different days of the week than that

on which the original attempt was made.

Nonresponse and Callbacks

When student pick up was used, up to three callbacks

Page 106: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

96

were made to collect the responses. For the second

method, telephone callbacks (up to 3 times) were used to

contact the respondents to ensure their participation.

Telephone numbers were obtained from the city directory

based on the addresses collected earlier (Lubbock City

1983).

A total of 512 responses were received out of 1000

surveys given out--a 51.2% response rate. However, only

460 responses could be used because of incomplete

responses. An analysis of the 460 completed responses

shows that 85.5% are white, 4.8% are black, 7.7% are

Hispanic and the balance 1.9% are of other ethnic groups.

These percentages compare well with those based on

Lubbock city census data--81% white, 8X Hispanic and 7%

black household population (Census Information 1983).

The median household income of the sample is in the

$20,001 to $30,000 range and the mode is in the $10,001

to $20,000 bracket. This represents fairly well the

city's 1979 median income of $15,735 and a mode in the

$5,000 to $9,999 bracket (Census Information 1983). The

latter figures will correspond better once they are

corrected for the inflation. The sample, it appears, is

generally representative of the parent population and the

non-response has, perhaps, not eroded the quality of the

sample--at least based on demographic characteristics.

Page 107: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

97

Phase II

Selection of Tracts

The census tracts were selected in phase II using a

procedure similar to that in phase I. Income was used to

stratify the census tracts and then a random sample of

tracts was selected from each stratum. Since phase II

required sending out four surveys (corresponding to the

four industries), each with a sample size of 1000, this

procedure was repeated four times. It was desirable not

to send more than one questionnaire to any given house­

hold. Therefore, an effort was made not to select the

same census tract in more than one survey. Appendix C

provides the diagrams of the census tracts selected in

each of the four surveys using the above procedure.

The second stage, which involved selecting the

specific households, was also similar to phase I. A

systematic sample of households was selected from the

already selected census tracts. Using a city directory,

every 10th household from each census tract selected

became part of the sample till the total sample size

desired was obtained (City Directory 1983).

Method of Data Collection

Considering the large sample size for phase II (4000

in all) a mail survey was selected as the appropriate

method for collecting the data. This method was also

Page 108: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

98

better suited to the phase II surveys, since the ques­

tionnaires were more involved and lengthy. Using the

city directory, a packet was mailed to each of the selec­

ted households. The packet contained a covering letter

explaining the purpose and sponsorship of the survey, the

questionnaire, and a pre-paid return envelope. Copies of

the questionnaires and the covering letters for the four

industries are enclosed in Appendix D.

Callbacks

The city directory provides the addresses as well as

the telephone numbers of all households. These telephone

numbers were used to contact the selected households 7-10

days after the mailing of the questionnaire packet. On

contact, a request for their participation was made, in

case they had not done so, and the importance of their

responses was reiterated. This procedure was adopted for

three out of the four industry surveys. For the last

survey, a reminder card was mailed to the sample, 5 days

after the packet of questionnaire was sent out. A dif­

ferent method for reminding the respondent was used for

financial services survey in order to study the effects

of different follow up methods on response rates in mail

surveys. However, this particular study did not consti­

tute a part of the present research. The four surveys

were conducted one after the other over a period of three

months (February-April 1985). Response rates for the

Page 109: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

99

four surveys are shown in Table 5.1.

Nonresponse

Nonresponse is an important concern here since the

response rates in phase II were far below that obtained

in phase I (51.2%). Since low response rates do imply a

source of bias in the obtained data and the corresponding

analysis, the response rates obtained in phase II of this

research do impose a limitation on the validity of the

results. However, the following discussion attempts to

show that, because of the particular nature of this

survey, the severity of the nonresponse bias may be

somewhat mitigated.

Several reasons can be attributed to the lower

response rate--the questionnaire was longer and more

involved. Most important is the nature of the question­

naire. The whole questionnaire revolved around a

particular "recent problem" or "dissatisfaction" that the

respondent had with the given industry. What if the

respondent felt he/she had had no. "recent problem" of any

significance, with grocery shopping for example? It is

suggested that such a situation resulted in nonresponse.

This conclusion is based on two evidences.

When respondents were contacted by phone, two

typical responses were observed. People who felt they

had been satisfied with their grocer, for example, had a

Page 110: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

100

higher probability of having discarded the questionnaire,

and showed no interest in participation. On the other

hand, people who had bad experiences were eager to parti­

cipate and "let some one know" about the problem(s) they

had faced. Motivation to complete and mail the question­

naire was high in the latter situation.

The second evidence comes from the wave analysis of

returned responses. As the responses were received, they

were classified into waves by industry. There was a gap

of at least 1 to 2 days between the two consecutive

waves. The percentage of respondents indicating "no

problem" was calculated for each wave. Later waves had a

much higher percentage of "no problem" than did earlier

waves. For example, in the grocery industry, the "no

problem" respondents were 17% in the first wave, 29% in

the second wave and over 72% in the third wave. A chi-

square test for the difference in cells by chance was

significant at alpha=0.05 level of significance for all

the four surveys. This shows that respondents who had

less interest in the survey (later waves) had a much

higher probability of experiencing "no problem." Table

5.2 gives the chi-square statistic for each of the four

industry surveys.

Further, respondents who had faced a recent problem

or dissatisfaction were analyzed to determine if the

latter (wave) respondents were significantly different

Page 111: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

101

from the earlier (wave) respondents. Specifically, dif­

ferences were examined for several key constructs, such

as attitude toward the act of complaining, VOICE, W-O-M,

and FORMAL intentions as well as expectancy value

judgments. Respondents were divided into three groups

based on waves (the third and fourth wave had to be

collapsed because of small number of responses in the

fourth wave). An analysis of variance was done with the

three groups as the treatment and the individual

constructs as the dependent variable. The null hypothe­

sis of no difference in the construct means among the

three waves was not rejected at 0.05 level of signi­

ficance for each of the four industry data. Infact, the

F value for significant difference in the means was less

the 2.0 for 26 out of 28 construct means investigated.

This implies that respondents who had experienced a

recent problem but had responded in the latter waves

were, perhaps, no different from respondents who had also

faced a recent dissatisfying experience but responded to

the questionnaire early.

Since the objective of this research is to only look

at those people who are dissatisfied, and investigate the

kind of complaint behavior they engage in and why, non-

response from people who do not have a high probability

of recently experiencing a dissatisfaction would,

perhaps, not adversely affect the quality of data.

Page 112: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

102

TABLE 5.1

RESPONSE RATES FOR PHASE II SURVEYS

Industry

Grocery

Auto Repair

Medical Care

Financial Services

#Mailed

1000

1000

1000

1000

#Received

176

155

166

172

Response Rate

17.6 %

15.5 %

16.6 %

17.2 %

Usable Responses

124

116

125

104

Usable Rate

12.4 %

11.6 %

12.5 %

10.4 %

Page 113: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

TABLE 5.2

CHI-SQUARE TEST FOR "NO PROBLEM" RESPONDENTS

1 0 3

INDUSTRY CHI-SQUARE STATISTIC DP P-VALUE

Grocery

Auto Repair

Medical Care

Financial Services

59.5

36.4

7.83

10.65

3

3

3

3

0.0000

0.0000

0.049

0.0049

Page 114: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 6

PHASE I RESULTS

The objective of phase I is to investigate the

nature of the relationship among the three affective

constructs--alienation from market-place, discontent, and

attitude towards the act of complaining. Specifically,

it is proposed to empirically examine hypotheses H1-H3

(Figure 6.1). Hypotheses HI and H2 attempt to examine

the measurement properties of the alienation and discon­

tent constructs. This is of particular interest, since

researchers debate the validity of treating these two

constructs as distinct and different from each other.

For instance, Allison (1978) who developed the scale of

alienation from the market place suggested that alie­

nation and discontent are similar and, perhaps identical

constructs. On the other hand, Lundstrom et al. (1979)

argue, from both a methodological and theoretical stand­

point, that the alienation and discontent are distinct

concepts, and are in fact inversely related to each

other. Since this dissertation proposes to use these two

constructs, phase I attempts to resolve this dilemma

before proceeding to phase II.

Further, empirical examination of hypothesis H3

should also provide insights in to the measurement

104

Page 115: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

105

properties of alienation and discontent scales. H3

examines the relationship of these two constructs with an

external criterion variable--attitude toward the act of

complaining. If these two constructs are truly distinct

and possess discriminant validity, then the relationship

between each of these two scales and attitude toward the

act of complaining should neither be identical nor

similar. The empirical investigation of H3, therefore,

will help clarify the nature of alienation and discontent

scales.

Several methodologies are used to test the hypoth­

eses H1-H3. In particular, factor analysis is used to

examine the measurement properties, and path analysis is

used to investigate the structural relationships in

hypothesis H3. In addition. Item Response Theory (IRT)

is used to analyze the measurement properties of the

alienation and discontent scales. IRT is a measurement

theory which affords stronger conclusions about scales

and what they are measuring than the traditional

classical test theory (CTT) (Hulin, Drasgow and Miller

1983). Further, IRT can be very useful for scale devel­

opment and modification since it explicitly accounts for

the information provided by each item on the scale (Lord

1980). Perhaps, the most attractive consideration for

the adoption of IRT for phase I analysis, is that it

provides measurement and information properties of items

Page 116: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

106

on a scale that is independent of the sample selected,

except for the effect of sampling variations (Lord 1980).

Although marketing has been slow in the adoption of this

powerful technique to address measurement problems, this

technique is widely in use in educational psychology, and

to some extent in psychology.

In the following discussion, hypotheses H1-H3 are

examined with both, the traditional methods of factor

analysis as well as Item Response Theory. In that sense,

the objective also to show the two methods can be used in

a complimentary manner to enrich the understanding of

what is being measured by a pool of items and with what

efficiency.

Measurement Properties Using Traditional Methods

The measurement properties of the three constructs

are first investigated before examining the structural

relationships among them. The alpha reliability of the

18-item attitudes towards the act of complaining

construct is 0.626, the 35-item alienation scale has a

reliability of 0.902 and the 82-item discontent scale has

an alpha of 0.944. This compares well with the

reliability values reported in the literature for these

three constructs. For instance, Allison (1978) reported

an alpha of 0.88 for the alienation scale; Lundstrom and

Lament (1976) found an index of 0.96 for the reliability

Page 117: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

107

of the discontent scale. A proper development of the

attitudes towards the act of complaining construct has

not been undertaken in the marketing literature, and its

measurement properties can not be compared. However, the

scale used in phase I is based on the efforts of various

researchers in the area, specifically Richins (1982).

Items that compose a scale are expected to relate

positively to each other since they are purportedly

tapping the same trait. Items that have a negative (or

zero) correlation with the total score on a given scale

do not meet this criterion and should be deleted to

improve the measurement properties of the scale. Using

this procedure, four items were dropped from the attitude

scale and the revised reliability of the scale improved

to 0. 685.

The next step in the measurement analysis is to

investigate the factor structure for each of the three

constructs. A common factor analysis of the 35-item

alienation scale shows the presence of a single dominant

factor. This is depicted in the scree plot shown in

Figure 6.2. The first factor is associated with an

eigenvalue of 8.23 which represents 65% of the shared

variance. The second eigenvalue in contrast is only

1. 44. This eigenvalue structure is representative of

unidimensional scales (large first eigenvalue and the

remaining values about the same range but small). This

Page 118: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

108

finding supports the unidimensional structure for the

construct of alienation from the market place and is

consistent with the results of Allison (1978) (see

Bearden, Lichtenstein and Teel 1983 for a different

conclusion).

Similarly, the attitudes construct on factor

analysis of the correlation matrix with squared multiple

correlations on the diagonals shows that the first factor

represents 70% of the shared variance (eigenvalue 2.40).

The second eigenvalue in this case is less than half of

the first eigenvalue. This supports a unidimensional

conceptualization of the attitudes towards the act of

complaining scale. Finally, the discontent scale also

indicates a similar conclusion. The scree plot of the

eigenvalues is given in Figure 6.3. The first eigenvalue

is 16.58 representing 45% of the total shared variance.

The next eigenvalue is only 3. 18. The factor analysis,

therefore, suggests that each of the three constructs

have good measurement properties and the items that

compose them tap a distinctive and well defined trait.

Structural Relationships Using Path Analysis

It has been suggested previously that considerable

controversy surrounds the nature and measurement of

discontent and alienation constructs. Hypothesis H1-H3

directly addresses this issue by examining the divergent

Page 119: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

109

validity of these two constructs by examining their

relationship with a third, external variable, attitude

toward the act of complaining. A correlation matrix

using summated indexes for the three scales is given in

Table 6.1.

Discontent and alienation are positively related

(r=0.896, p<0.0001), which suggests that these two

constructs are not distinct concepts and do not possess

discriminant validity. When attenuated for reliability

of the two constructs, the correlation increases to

0.971. Consequently, hypothesis HI is rejected. A

somewhat different conclusion results when the corre­

lations with the attitudes construct are examined.

Alienation from the market place does not have a

significant relationship with attitude towards the act of

complaining (p=0.1186), whereas discontent bears a sig­

nificant and positive effect on the attitudes construct

(p<=0.0001). The unattenuated correlation for the latter

is 0. 22. It is recommended that path analysis be used in

situations where relationships among three or more

constructs are being examined in the presence of corre­

lated constructs (Johnson and Wichern 1982). This is

relevant here since discontent and alienation are corre­

lated while at the same time their effect on attitude

toward the act of complaining is under investigation.

Figure 6.4 gives the path analytic diagram for phase I

Page 120: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

110

data. This figure presents a striking result: the path

coefficient between discontent and attitudes is 0.555

(unattenuated 0.690) and the path coefficient between

alienation and attitudes is -0.426 (unattenuated -0.542).

This implies that discontent has a positive effect on the

attitudes construct, whereas alienation has an inverse

relationship. Therefore, hypothesis H2, which proposed

that alienation is negatively related and hypothesis H3

which indicated that discontent is positively related to

the attitudes construct, are both supported by this data.

And yet, it is also observed that discontent and alie­

nation are positively and significantly correlated so as

to appear to lack discriminant validity.

This finding clearly supports hypotheses H2 and H3,

and casts a doubt on the earlier conclusion about

rejecting HI. In other words, discontent and alienation

are almost perfectly correlated while simultaneously

possessing strikingly different path coefficients with

respect to the construct of attitudes towards the act of

complaining. What then is the real nature of these

constructs and what do they measure? How can these two

opposite pieces of evidence be explained? Item Response

Theory is an appropriate tool for this investigation.

Measurement Properties Using IRT Based Procedures

The assumption of unidimensionality of a given pool

Page 121: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

Ill

of items is a necessary precondition before employing IRT

based procedures. Do the data meet this requirement? It

has been shown earlier that each of the three

constructs--alienation, discontent and attitudes--

evidence a unidimensional eigenvalue structure. As the

concern here is the distinction between alienation and

discontent, it is important to investigate if the items

pooled from the discontent and alienation scales also

show a unidimensional factor structure. To investigate

this, the 35 alienation items and the 82 discontent items

are combined into a single pool of 117 items. This

combined pool is then factor analyzed. The eigenvalue

plot for this correlation matrix shows that the first

eigenvalue is approximately 6 times the second value and

explains about 40% of the shared variance (see Figure

6.5). This is evidence of the unidimensionality of the

trait underlying this pool of 117 items. Thus, IRT

procedures can be safely employed for this data.

A two parameter logistic function was fitted to the

response to each item in the pool using the Legist

program (Wingersky, Barton and Lord 1982). The output

from this procedure includes the approximate maximum

likelihood estimates of "a" and "b" parameters, their

standard errors and the "thetas," that is, estimates of

the level of the trait for each respondent. Based on

these parameters and the IRT procedures it is possible to

Page 122: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

112

calculate the amount of information contained in a group

of items for different levels of the underlying trait,

theta. The theta in the present investigation is the

affective feelings of discontent and alienation. Each

item on the scale provides some information about this

theta, called the item information. Information provided

by an item is not necessarily constant throughout the

range of theta. For instance, if the item is very

"easy," it may provide very little information at higher

levels of the underlying trait. Nevertheless, the same

item may provide a substantial amount of information

around the low levels of theta. Further, the total

information or test information provided by a scale of

items is simply the sum of the informations provided by

individual items in the scale (Lord 1980). The test

information also provides a measure of reliability. When

information is high, uncertainty about the measurement of

the underlying trait is low and therefore reliability is

high. Since the test information function varies for

different levels of theta, the reliability of a scale is

also a variable, possessing different values at different

levels of the underlying trait (Hulin, Drasgow and Miller

1983).

These information curves can provide useful insight

into the measurement properties of a pool of items. In

the present case, two information curves are estimated.

Page 123: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

113

one for the a2-item discontent scale and another for the

35-item alienation scale. Figure 6.6 shows that the

discontent items provide their maximum information

(highest reliability) at approximately -0.7 trait level,

while the alienation items have their maximum information

at approximately -0.55. The trait level is standardized

with mean zero and standard deviation one. The two infor­

mation curves suggest that the given set of items provide

much of their information in the middle range of the

underlying trait and relatively poor information at the

two extremes. The closeness of the two information

curves also explains the high correlation observed

between discontent and alienation constructs.

Some key implications of the above analysis can now

be stated. The pool of items purportedly measuring the

discontent affective feelings and the other pool suppose­

dly tapping the feelings of alienation from the market

place are in fact measuring the same underlying trait--

with one difference. The alienation items provide the

bulk of their information at a trait level slightly

higher than that for the discontent items. Since,

alienation and discontent are somewhat different

(evidenced by the support of hypotheses H2 and H3), it is

possible that higher levels of discontent lead to

alienation. In other words, as people become more and

more discontent they also tend to become alienated.

Page 124: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

114

Therefore, an appropriate measurement of discontent can

be achieved if its information curve peaks at lower

levels (less than the mean) of the underlying trait.

Similarly, the alienation construct can be better tapped

if its information curve peaks at higher levels (greater

than the mean) of the common underlying trait. An exami­

nation of Figure 6.6 shows that current scales for the

above constructs do not meet this criterion. How, then,

should these scales be modified to provide a better

measurement of the alienation and discontent constructs?

The item difficulty parameter, "b, " obtained from

IRT procedures provides necessary guidance in the

selection of items for the modified discontent and

alienation scales that meet the above condition. For any

given item, the "b" parameter represents the value of the

trait at which the given item would have a peak for its

information curve. Since the total or the test informa­

tion for a complete scale is simply the sum of the

information from each of its items, it would be desirable

that all discontent (alienation) items have their "b"

parameter less (greater) than zero. Thus, the following

criteria for the selection of items are proposed:

Assume: The underlying trait being measured--the

generalized affective feelings of discontent

and alienation--is standardized with mean 0 and

standard deviation 1.

Page 125: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

115

Criterion #1: The "b" parameter values for all discon­

tent items should be negative (less than

mean).

Criterion #2: The "b" parameter values for all aliena­

tion items should be positive (greater

than mean).

Criterion #3: No two items should have the same "b"

parameter value. In fact, the items

selected should have "b" values that span

the entire range from •»-3 to -3.

Criterion #4: The standard errors of the "b" parameters

should be small compared to the parameter

value.

Criterion #5: Each selected item must have a strong "a"

parameter value (for instance > 0.4) with

small standard error compared to the

parameter value.

Criterion #6: The item should satisfy face validity

requirements for inclusion as a discontent

or an alienation item.

Criterion #7: The number of items selected in each of

the two scales should be between 8-15

items.

This procedure will yield shortened versions of the

discontent and alienation scales that (a) tap a substan­

tial range of the underlying affective trait, (b) provide

Page 126: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

116

adequate measurement (information), and (c) can differen­

tiate better between the feelings of alienation and

discontent. It may be noted that no external criterion

variable is being used in the selection of items for the

shortened scales (Nunnally 1978).

Using this procedure, thirteen items were selected

out of 35 original items to compose the alienation scale

and 12 items were selected out of 82 items for the

discontent scale (Appendix E). The "a" and "b" parame­

ters for the selected items are tabulated in Table 6.2.

Figure 6.7 presents the revised information curves for

the shortened scales.

A comparison of Figure 6.7 with figure 6.6 shows

some striking differences attributable to the above

procedure. The information curves are now distinguish­

able, the peak of the discontent items occurring around

-1.25 trait value and that of the alienation scale at

around 0.75. Thus the revised scales do seem to measure

distinctively the two dimensions of the underlying trait.

The discontent dimension is measured with greater relia­

bility than the alienation dimension because of its

higher information peak. In addition, the alienation

scale does not provide much information about the more

alienated persons (trait value>2.0), though there is a

definite improvement over the original scale. It is

suggested that newer items may have to be constructed to

Page 127: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

117

measure effectively the higher levels of the alienation

dimension.

As discussed earlier, IRT procedures provide

estimates that are in general invariant across different

samples. Therefore, the "b" parameters and consequently

the information curves should correlate highly across two

independent studies. To investigate this, IRT parameters

of the shortened scales were calculated based on the data

collected in phase II for the grocery and automotive

repair industries. The data consisted of 240 completed

responses. Some of the items had to be dropped since

their standard errors could not be estimated accurately.

The reduced scale consisted of 12 discontent items and 9

alienation items.

Figure 6.8 is the plot of "b" parameter values

calculated independently from the two different samples.

The slope of the line (correlation) is 0.864 (p<=0.0000)

indicating close correspondence. Similarly, the infor­

mation curve derived from phase II data ought to be

similar to Figure 6.7 if IRT procedures are valid.

Figure 6.9 shows the information curves for the dis­

content and alienation dimension for the grocery and

automotive repair data. Once again, a close correspon­

dence is evidenced between the information curves derived

from the two different studies, suggestive of the

stability and validity of the measurement procedure. In

Page 128: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

118

addition, if the measurement of a construct is thus

improved, then its relationship with other constructs

should also be better defined. The next section investi­

gates the structural relationship of the reduced

discontent and alienation scales with the external

construct of attitude toward the act of complaining.

Structural Relationships With Reduced Scales

Figure 6.10 shows the path analytical diagram

(similar to Figure 6.4) using summated indexes from the

shortened versions of the discontent and alienation

scales but based on phase I data. The correlation

between discontent and alienation is now 0.592, substan­

tially lower than the earlier value of 0.93, indicating

the existence of two different concepts: discontent and

alienation. This reduced correlation also helps in part

to stabilize the two path coefficients and reduce their

standard errors. The path coefficients support the

earlier conclusion, that is, discontent has a positive

effect, while alienation has a negative effect on the

attitudes construct. In other words, the revised scales

provide partial support for hypothesis HI, in that

alienation and discontent are distinct facets, though of

a common underlying trait. Support for H2 and H3 is

provided as well. This is an improvement in the understa­

nding of these constructs, since the significant differe-

Page 129: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

119

nces in the two path coefficients can be attributed to

the two dimensions of the underlying generalized

affective feelings.

A contrary argument that can be made is that since

the items selection was based on the analysis of phase I

data, and the structural relationships were tested using

the same data, it is plausible that the conclusion is

merely an artifact. To examine this argument, a path

analysis is undertaken using the automotive repair data

in phase II (Figure 6.11). The magnitude and direction

of path coefficients is very similar to that observed for

phase I data (Figure 6.10). Discontent and alienation

are positively correlated but the correlation is relati­

vely smaller than 0.93 (Figure 6.4). This suggests that

for the automotive repair data, discontent and alienation

also appear to be distinct dimensions. Discontent has a

positive path while alienation a negative path leading to

the attitudes construct, implying support for hypothesis

H2 and H3. A comparison of the path coefficients in

Figure 6.10 and 6.11 shows that they are of comparable

magnitude. The automotive repair data, therefore,

provides an independent support of the conclusions

obtained for phase I data. Thus the procedures adopted

for the phase I of this dissertation appear to be valid

and reliable.

The question remains as to whether alienation and

Page 130: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

120

discontent are positively or negatively related, or

positively related in one range and negatively in the

remaining range? To explore these questions, the data

obtained from the grocery and automotive repair surveys

(240 responses in all) were divided into two groups,

based upon thetas (value of the underlying trait) being

either below or above the mean. Below zero (average)

theta represent predominantly discontent and theta above

zero (average) denote mainly alienated respondents. As

shown in Figures 6.12 and 6.13 the correlation between

discontent and alienation drops from 0. 51 in the

discontent sample to 0.45 in the alienated sample. This

suggests that the distinction between these two concepts

improves at higher levels of the underlying trait. In

other words, it is hypothesized that alienation may not

be identifiable at lower levels of the underlying trait.

Figures 6.12 and 6.13 show that while the

discontent >attitudes path remains of almost the same

magnitude across the two groups, the alienation >atti-

tudes path relatively changes substantially from 0.027

(no relationship) to -0.10 (negative relationship). This

provides an additional evidence in support of the sug­

gested conceptualization of these two constructs.

Summary

Based on the data obtained in phase I, the analysis

suggests that: (1) discontent and alienation are two

Page 131: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

121

dimensions or facets of an underlying generalized

affective feelings concept; (2) discontent and alienation

are directly and positively related; (3) alienation

becomes better defined and distinct at higher levels of

the underlying trait; (4) alienation is negatively

related to the attitudes construct; and (5) discontent

has a positive relationship with the attitude towards the

act of complaining. This implies that hypothesis HI is

supported in part while hypotheses H2 and H3 are both

supported by the data.

Further, shortened versions- of the three scales were

also constructed using IRT based procedures. The reduced

scales now consist of 13 items in the alienation scale,

12 items in the attitude scale and 12 items on the

discontent scale. These shortened versions are used in

phase II to test the remaining hypotheses.

Page 132: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

122

TABLE 6.1

CORRELATION MATRIX FOR DISCONTENT, ALIENATION AND ATTITUDES

Discontent

Alienation

Attitudes

Discontent

1.00

0.896* (0.0000)

0.176* (0.0001)

Alienation

1.00

0.073 (0.1186)

Attitudes

1.00

* Significant at 0.01 level.

Page 133: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

123

TABLE 6.2

AND "B" PARAMETERS FOR THE DISCONTENT AND ALIENATION SCALES

ITEM "B" PARAMETER "A" PARAMETER

Discontent # 1 # 2 # 3 # 4 # 5

#e # 7 # 8 # 9 # 1 0 #11 #12

Alienation # 1 # 2 # 3 # 4 # 5 # 6 # 7 # 8 # 9 # 1 0

-1.3127 -1.2553 -1.7583 -1.3234 -1.6211 -2.1529 -0.6401 -1.4695 -1.0319 -2.4676 -1.1569 -0.8371

0.4715 2.6433 0.4912 0.2843 1.4143 0.8604 2.0943 1.7187 0.1667 2.0456

0.7940 0.7146 0.9035 0.9144 0.7227 0.4386 1.0967 0.8111 0.8634 0.4027 0.8776 0.7275

1.6309 0.2075 0.5709 0.8750 0.1667 0.6817 0.4204 0.4091 0.8956 0.4591

Page 134: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

124

FIGURE 6. 1

THE EMPIRICAL MODEL TO BE TESTED IN PHASE I

Page 135: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

125

E I G E N V A L U E S

10 ^

a ••

6

- ?

45 ^7<^'70l2

U 567. '^901? ^45671-? O l ? U S o

0

N UM R F 9

IC 20 n 4 0

FIGURE 6 . 2

EIGENVALUE STRUCTURE FOR THE 35 ITEM ALIENATION SCALE

Page 136: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

c I G r

V

L U c

126

20

15

10

0 f

- 5 *•

4

?0 4 0 ^0

P j U M i l P R

FIGURE 6.3

EIGENVALUE STRUCTURE FOR THE 82 ITEM DISCONTENT SCALE

Page 137: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

127

0.176

-. 0.073 ,

FIGURE 6.4

PATH ANALYTICAL DIAGRAM FOR COMPLETE SCALES

Page 138: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

128

£ I G c N V A L U

5

45 •!

2 0 f

15 >

\0

^ k k k i

0 ' A^^fr*^*******^******?

50 TOO 153

f J \jr n ' R

FIGURE 6.5

EIGENVALUE STRUCTURE FOR THE COMBINED POOL OF DISCONTENT AND ALIENATION ITEMS

Page 139: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

^0-

18.

i29

/6- J

i^.

Z; / ^

^

= ^

I, : \

LCGr^Q. scAu-

3 ^ ^iSccur^'jr

FIGURE 6 . 6

Page 140: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

i3Q

i.£:GCNO " CQCC

•"> Cf^r-g.jrr^r

'"IGU/JE 6. 7

" " '"'^^So^-« - . . . . ,

Page 141: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

3 . 0 *

?. . J

131

. 0 •

I . > ^

\ .') •

0 ,'> f

J(l I I

0 . 0 *• I I I

- J . ' j •

I I I

- 1 . 0 ••

- I . > •

- ^ . J • I I I —

2 . 'S •

- 3 . U *

- 4 2>

FIGURE 6 . 8

PLOT OF "B" PARAMETERS FROM TWO STUDIES

Page 142: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

132

7 . 0 -

'yry^ » ^l^'ON'f^r

^^GUHE 6 . 9

SCALES ^ ° « PW- SE I I

Page 143: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

133

0.36

0. 12

FIGURE 6.10

PATH ANALYTICAL DIAGRAM USING SHORTENED SCALES

Page 144: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

134

0. 39

0.26

FIGURE 6. 11

PATH ANALYTICAL DIAGRAM BASED ON AUTOMOTIVE REPAIR DATA

Page 145: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

135

0.24

0. 14

FIGURE 6. 12

PATH ANALYTICAL DIAGRAM FOR THE DISCONTENT GROUP

Page 146: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

136

0.20

0.009,

FIGURE 6.13

PATH ANALYTICAL DIAGRAM FOR THE ALIENATED GROUP

Page 147: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 7

PHASE II RESULTS

The objective of phase II involves examining the

remaining hypotheses H4-H18 (see chapter 4) using the

data collected across the four industries of study:

grocery retailing, automotive repair, medical care, and

financial services.

Measurement Properties

The testing of the various hypotheses in phase II

involves over twelve different constructs. Each

construct is purportedly measuring different and distinct

phenomena. Measurement of the various constructs in the

hypothesized model is investigated first. Relationships

among constructs are then examined for support of

hypotheses H4-H18.

A construct cannot be valid unless it is also

reliable (Zeller and Carmines 1980). Therefore, reliable

measurement is a precondition to substantive interpre­

tation of construct relationships. Table 7.1 provides

the alpha reliabilities of all 12 constructs to be used

in the data analysis. Prior experience (3 items) is a

behavioral construct, unlike the attitudinal and

intentions constructs. Conceptually, prior experience

conforms to a "formative" structure (Fornell and

137

Page 148: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

138

Booksteln 1982) where prior experience of complaining is

composed of three dimensions: how often one has (1)

VOICEd one's complaints, (2) undertaken Word-of-Mouth (W-

0-M) communication to friends and relatives about the bad

experience, and (3) taken FORMAL action, such as legal,

complain to Better Business Bureau. Therefore, prior

experience is operationalized as the sum of its three

Indicators; measurement error in the classical sense of

alpha reliability is not applicable in this case (Fornell

1985).

All other constructs (excluding prior experience)

are conceptually "reflexive, " in that it is hypothesized

that there is an underlying latent variable, the

construct, which causes the responses on specific items.

Two of the constructs, internal and external attributions

of blame, are measured by only two items each. There is

a particular problem when dealing with such constructs,

specifically in latent variable structural equations

(LVSE) modeling. The measurement model for a two item

construtrt may not be identified, in that only three

pieces of information (two variances and one covariance)

are available for estimating a minimum of four

parameters. Even in cases where the parameters of a two

item construct are estimable, such estimates may be

particularly susceptible to interpretational confounding

(Burt 1976). To circumvent this potential problem, it

Page 149: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

139

may be necessary to determine arbitrarily the reliability

of the two item constructs of internal and external

attributions of blame.

Data collected in phase II of the study show the

constructs of internal and external attributions of blame

to be weakly correlated, with correlations ranging from

-0.06 to 0.17; an exception is medical care with a corre­

lation of -0.60. Previous research reports higher corre­

lations, generally greater than 0.5, among the various

dimensions of the attributions of blame construct (Foikes

1984). While it is probable that internal and external

attributions of blame are distinct and discriminable

constructs, a different reason for the poor correlation

in the present data is suggested. The two items used to

measure the external attribution of blame reflected two

possible causes of the problem--store policies and

personnel. External attribution could arise from a host

of other reasons, e.g., quality of materials stocked,

long lines for check out. In other words, the items used

to tap ttie external attribution construct provided, post-

facto, poor measurement. The results regarding attri­

butions of blame based on these items would indeed be

biased. While arbitrarily setting the reliability of

these two constructs, therefore, it was desirable not to

have either too high a relaibility index because of some

evidence of poor measurement, or too low an index since

Page 150: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

140

that would artificially inflate the path coefficients.

Thus the reliability of the two item constructs, internal

and external attributions of blame, is arbitrarily set at

0.64.

Alpha reliabilities of the remaining constructs

range between 0.64 and 0.94 with the exception of the

four item expectancy-value (W-O-M) scale, for which the

reliability value is between 0.49 and 0.64. This is a

relatively weak measurement and reflects the need to

improve the structure and perhaps the nature of the items

that compose this scale. The reliability of expectancy-

value (W-O-M), however, could not be improved with the

present set of items.

The alpha reliabilities in Table 7.1 show a high

degree of consistency across the four industries. This

indicates that the items provide reasonable measurement

of the underlying phenomena across different samples and

different questionnaire contexts. This is particularly

interesting, since many of the items had to be adapted to

be suitable for the financial services and medical care

survey. This also resulted in unequal number of items

for three of the constructs--alienation, discontent and

attitudes towards the act of complaining. For instance,

discontent was measured by 10 items for the grocery,

automotive repair and medical care surveys but by 8 items

in the financial survey. Two of the discontent items

Page 151: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

141

were dropped since they did not make sense in the context

of financial services. Yet the reliability of discontent

is comparable for the four cases--ranging between 0.79

and 0.85. This is true, in general, for the other

constructs, also. Thus, the constructs appear to measure

the underlying phenomenon in a consistent and distinct

manner.

Empirical Investigation of the Typology for the Predominant Predictor of CCB Intentions

(Hypotheses H6-H8)

Hypotheses H6-H8 involve an empirical investigation

of the hypothesized typology of the predominant mode of

CCB responses (see figure 3.2, chapter 3). The suggested

typology is based on two constructs: high/low prior expe­

rience and low/medium/high dissatisfaction. To investi­

gate this typology, prior experience is dichotomized at

its median into "high" experience and "low" experience

categories. For dissatisfaction, since the proposed

hypotheses are identical for medium and high level of

dissatisfaction, this variable is dichotomized at the

one-third percentile. Since, from the standpoint of

theory, no definitive predictor can be identified under

the condition of low dissatisfaction, the following

analysis examines only the cells corresponding to low-

/high prior experience and medium or high dissatisfaction

(2X1 vector).

In order to examine empirically the proposed

Page 152: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

142

hypotheses H6, H7 and H8, it is required to determine the

predominant determinant of the CCB intentions under

different conditions of prior experience (low or high).

Since the expectancy-value judgments and the attitude

towards the act of complaining are two competing predic­

tors of CCB intentions, a partial correlation framework

is used to test these hypotheses. Partial correlation is

calculated between a predictor of CCB intentions and CCB

intentions holding the other predictor constant, for each

level of prior experience. For instance, for the case of

low prior experience two partial correlations are

calculated: (1) correlation between attitude towards the

act of complaining and CCB intentions, holding expectancy

value judgment constant, and (2) correlation between

expectancy-value judgments and CCB intentions, holding

attitude towards the act of complaining constant. A

higher partial correlation would thus indicate a rela­

tively greater effect on the CCB intentions. The results

are shown in tables 7.2-7.5. Further, the sensitivity of

the results is also investigated for alternative methods

of categorizing the two constructs (e.g., the mode or

mean for prior experience). While the cell partial

correlations do change as alternative methods are

employed, the change is marginal and does not affect the

direction or the relative magnitude of the values

significantly.

Page 153: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

143

For the case of (a) high prior experience and (b)

medium/high dissatisfaction, attitude towards the act of

complaining appears to be the dominant predictor of

complaint intentions in grocery and automotive repair

industries. These results are in contrast to the

findings for similar conditions in the medical care and

financial services data. In the case of medical care,

attitudes and expectancy value judgments appear to be

equally important, while for the financial services,

expectancy-value judgments are the predominant mode of

CCB intentions. Hypothesis H6 proposed that, for high

level of previous experience in complaining combined with

medium/high level of dissatisfaction, the affective

factor or attitude toward the act of complaining would be

the key predictor of CCB intentions, while a weak rela­

tionship would exist between the cognitive level or

expectancy value judgments and complaint intentions.

Thus hypothesis H6 is supported for the grocery and

automotive repair data but is not supported for the

financial services and medical care data.

Under the condition of (a) low prior experience, and

(b) medium/high dissatisfaction, the findings indicate

that expectancy value judgments appear to have a dominant

influence on intentions in three out of the four

industries investigated: grocery, automotive repair and

financial services. In the medical care industry.

Page 154: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

144

expectancy value judgments and attitudes have about equal

influence on intentions to complain. Hypothesis H7

proposed that, for low level of previous experience in

complaining combined with medium/high level of dissatis­

faction, the cognitive level or expectancy value

judgments would be the key predictor of CCB intentions

while a weak relationship would exist between the

affective factor or attitude toward the act of complai­

ning and complaint intentions. Therefore, hypothesis H7

is not supported for medical care but is supported for

the remaining three industries. .

These findings suggest that automotive repair and

grocery industry data conform to the hypothesized typolo­

gy of the predominant mode of CCB responses. A qualifi­

cation to this statement is in order in that, for grocery

data, the effect of attitude on intentions appears to be

unaffected by the extent of prior experience (r=0.504 and

0.441). This is in contrast to automotive repair case,

where the effect of attitudes is significant only when

prior experience is high. The implication of this

finding is that, in the case of grocery shopping, where

the consumer has a frequent contact with the suppliers,

attitude or affective feelings play an important role in

the type of complaint actions taken. On the other hand,

the effect of expectancy value or cognitive judgments

depends on the extent of previous experience in handling

Page 155: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

145

complaints.

For the case of financial services. Tables 7.2-7.5

provide an additional insight into the comparative effect

of affective (i.e., attitudes) and cognitive (i.e.,

expectancy value) factors on the CCB intentions. Speci­

fically, results suggest that expectancy value judgments

are, in general, the key determinant of the intentions

irrespective of the level of prior experience (r=0.682

and 0.4341), though the effect tends to diminish somewhat

as the extent of experience increases. This implies that

where dissatisfaction regarding bank accounts or money is

concerned, people engage in considerable cognitive

activity to decide which complaint action they might

undertake. The effect of attitude on intentions is

significant and tends to increase as prior experience

increases, but is smaller relative to the effect of

expectancy value judgments.

The medical care industry lies somewhere along the

continuum from grocery to financial services, ranging

from the predominant effect of affective factors to that

of cognitive factors. The expectancy value judgments

(cognitive factors) and the affective factors (attitude

towards the act of complaining) are equally important for

determining the CCB intentions; and both tend to decrease

with increasing prior experience. This decrease in the

effect of affective factor on intentions as prior

Page 156: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

146

experience increases from low to high, can not be

explained by any a priori theory.

Summary

Based on the above findings and discussion, it can

be summarized that:

1. When people have low previous experience in

complaining but their dissatisfaction with a

particular problem is medium or high, their specific

complaint intentions can be mainly predicted by

their cognitive level or expectancy value judgments.

However, a weak relationship between the affective

factor or attitude toward the act of complaining is

evidenced only in automotive repair context. In the

case of grocery, medical care and financial

services, the affective factor is an equally or less

(though significant) important predictor of CCB

intentions. Therefore, hypothesis H7 is partially

supported by data.

2. Wh^n people have high previous experience of

complaining and their dissatisfaction with a parti­

cular episode is moderate or high, the affective

factor is a strong predictor of specific complaint

intentions in two of the four industries investi­

gated: grocery and automotive repair. In the

remaining two industries (financial and medical

Page 157: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

147

care), the affective factor is a significant

predictor but its effect is equal to or less than

the effect of the competing predictor--expectancy

value judgments. Thus, for more complex and

involving problems, specifically concerning medical

care and finances, it appears that people engage in

considerable cognitive activity prior to deciding

what complaint actions they intend to undertake.

Hypothesis H6 is supported in automotive repair and

grocery data only.

3. As people gain more experience in complaining about

their dissatisfactions, the cognitive factors become

less important while affective factors tend to

become more important in determining what specific

complaint behaviors people intend to carry out.

This finding is evident in all industries except in

the case of medical care where it is only partly

true--both factors tend to decrease with increasing

prior experience.

Empirical Investigation of the Framework for Predicting Specific CCB Responses

(Hypotheses H14-H17)

The model proposed in the dissertation for predic­

ting the specific CCB's is based on the value of two key

constructs: (a) seller responsiveness, that is, expec­

tancy value judgments regarding VOICEing complaints to

seller, and (b) attitudes towards the act of complaining

Page 158: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

148

(see figure 3.3, page 55, chapter 5). The specific

hypotheses in each of the four cells are H14-H17. In

order to empirically examine these hypotheses, the two

constructs of expectancy value (VOICE) and attitudes

towards the act of complaining need to be dichotomized.

Both constructs are summated indexes of their respective

items and show a distribution which approximates a normal

distribution (for instance, p=0.056 for H0=normal based

on the Shapiro-Wilk statistic for the test of normality

in the case expectancy-value (VOICE) scale). The median

is used to dichotomize the two independent constructs

into high/low expectancy value of seller's responsiveness

and positive/negative attitudes towards the act of com­

plaining. A sensitivity analysis is also undertaken by

using the mean and mode to dichotomize the constructs.

The sensitivity analysis showed that while cell means did

change somewhat, the results regarding pairwise compari­

sons and significant differences between cells remained

unchanged.

Three dependent variables are used in the analysis:

(a) intentions to engage in VOICE actions, (b) intentions

to engage in Word-of-Mouth (W-O-M) communication, and (c)

intentions to engage in FORMAL actions to third parties.

ANOVA is used as the statistical technique to examine

these hypotheses. Interaction effects between the two

independent variables are first investigated for each of

Page 159: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

149

the dependent variables individually. The interaction

effects are not found to be significant at the 0. 05

level and, therefore, only the main effects are retained

in the model. Least square means are then calculated for

each cell and Bonferroni T test is carried out to find if

there are significant differences between cell means

after controlling for Type I experimentwise error rate.

This method of comparing multiple means is somewhat

conservative as compared to other alternative methods,

such as the Fisher's LSD which control for the error on

per comparison basis (Ott 1977).. The results are now

examined for each dependent variable.

Dependent Variable: Intentions to VOICE

Table 7.6 shows the Bonferroni T tests and cell

means for each of the four industry data using the above

dependent variable. An examination of this table shows

that the results tend to vary considerably depending on

the industry. For the grocery industry, VOICE intentions

are high whenever attitudes towards complaining are

positive and are significantly lower when the attitudes

are negative, irrespective of the expectancy value of

seller's responsiveness. In contrast, for financial

services, the VOICE intentions are dependent on the level

of bank's responsiveness. When this expectancy value is

high, intentions are also high, but as these cognitions

Page 160: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

150

about banker's responsiveness fall, the desire to VOICE

also drops. Attitudes, however, do seem to have an

effect but only under the condition of low expectancy

value, when negative attitudes drive the cell mean to a

low level of 3.76, making it significantly different from

the remaining three cells.

The results are mixed for the medical care industry.

Attitudes as well as expectancy value judgments have an

impact on the intentions to VOICE. The mean for the

intentions variable is highest when expectancy value is

high and attitudes are positive. The mean for VOICE

intentions, on the other hand, is lower and about the

same for two conditions: (1) when attitudes are negative

and expectancy value is high, or (2) when attitudes are

positive while expectancy value is low. The mean in the

above three cells are, however, not significantly dif­

ferent from each other. Cell 4 which corresponds to

negative attitudes and low expectancy value has a mean of

4.21, and is significantly different from mean for cell 1

after controlling for type I experimentwise error rate.

Surprisingly, for the automotive repair data, VOICE

intentions are statistically unaffected by either

attitudes or expectancy value of seller's responsiveness.

This is an unexpected finding, in contrast to results in

other industries. Perhaps, it may be suggested that

other variables, such as prior experience or attributions

Page 161: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

151

of blame might explain the reasons underlying the VOICE

intentions in automotive repair industry.

Thus the conclusion is that, in general, VOICE

intentions are high when attitudes are positive and

expectancy value is also high and, in general, are low

when attitudes are negative and expectancy value is low,

across all four industries with the exception of auto­

motive repair. In the other two cells, results tend to

depend on the nature of industry. The findings suggest

that in "low involvement" industries, such as grocery

retailing, attitude towards the act of complaining is the

predominant predictor of VOICE intentions. In "high

involvement" industries, such as banking, expectancy

value take on a key role in determining VOICE actions.

The results also show that for medical care industry

which seems to lie in between the two extremes, both

independent variables have a near equal effect. The case

of automotive repair is unique and no prediction is

possible with the independent variables selected.

Dependent Variable:

Intentions to W-O-M Communication

Table 7.7 provides the cell means and Bonferroni T

tests for each of the four industries using the dependent

variable as the intentions to engage in W-O-M communica­

tion with friends and relatives. An examination of this

table shows that the results are, in general, consistent

Page 162: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

152

across the four industries. The W-O-M intentions are

enhanced by positive attitudes and low expectancy value

regarding seller's responsiveness. In other words,

people in general seem to have a greater propensity to

engage in W-O-M when they perceive that the provider or

seller is not going to redress their complaints

(expectancy) and/or the problem is not "important" enough

(value), but have a positive attitude towards the act of

complaining.

Further, results in Table 7.7 show that W-O-M

intentions are least under conditions of (a) negative

attitudes, and (b) high expectancy value of seller's

responsiveness. The mean for this ceil is statistically

different from the mean in cell 2 at 0.05 level. This

implies that when people, in general, expect a high

degree of seller responsiveness to their problems, they

may not engage in any W-O-M if they have negative

affective feelings about the act of complaining.

In addition, if the mean value of intentions to

engage in W-O-M in cell 3 is compared to the mean in cell

4, the latter mean is always higher. This implies that,

perhaps, people engage in W-O-M when they have a low

expectancy value of seller's responsiveness, more often

than when they have a high expectancy value. In other

words, when consumers, in general, feel that their

complaints and problems have a low probability of being

Page 163: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

153

satisfactorily addressed by the seller or provider, they

have a higher propensity to spread the "bad experience"

through W-O-M to their friends and relatives. The

intention to communicate the "bad experience" is enhanced

if they also have a positive attitude towards the act of

complaining. In contrast, when people have a high proba­

bility that their complaints would be properly handled,

the propensity to spread the "good word" is not nearly as

high, particularly so if their attitude is negative. The

above results appear to hold irrespective of the nature

of the industry.

Dependent Variable:

Intentions to Take FORMAL Actions

Table 7.8 gives the cell means and Bonferroni T

tests for the four industries using the dependent

variable as the intentions to engage in FORMAL actions to

third parties, such as Better Business Bureau, legal

system, etc. An examination of the table shows a

striking result that is consistent across the three

industries (excluding automotive repair). The results

show that none of the cell means are statistically

different from one another. In other words, the FORMAL

intentions could not be predicted by (a) attitude towards

the act of complaining, or by (b) expectancy value of

seller's responsiveness, or when (c) the two variables

are used jointly. This suggests that, perhaps, decisions

Page 164: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

154

to undertake FORMAL actions may be dependent on other

factors, for instance, the expectancy value of FORMAL

actions. This is proposed to be examined later

(hypothesis H4).

However, in the case of automotive repair, results

indicate that the mean in cell 3 (negative attitudes and

high expectancy value) is significantly lower than the

mean in cell 2 (positive attitudes and low expectancy

value). This might suggest that, in the case of automo­

tive repair, attitude towards complaining may play some

role in predicting FORMAL intentions. Such evidence is

not forthcoming from the analysis of the data pertaining

to the remaining three industries.

Summary

Based on the above findings and discussion, it can

be summarized that:

1. When attitudes are positive and expectancy value is

high, the preferred intentions are found to be (a)

VOTCE to the seller or provider, and (b) W-O-M

communication to friends and relatives. Thus

hypothesis H14 is supported by the data.

2. When attitudes are positive but expectancy value

judgments are low, the preferred intentions are

found to be (a) W-O-M communication to friends and

relatives, and (b) no definite comment can be made

Page 165: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

155

regarding VOICE or EXIT. Thus hypothesis H15 is

partly supported.

3. When attitudes are negative but expectancy value

judgments are high, the preferred intentions are

found to be (a) not to engage in W-O-M communica­

tion, and (b) to VOICE in industries like medical

care and banking, but to take NO ACTION in

industries like grocery retailing. Thus hypothesis

H17 is supported with a qualification.

4. When attitudes are negative and expectancy value

judgments are low, the preferred intentions appear

to be not (a) to VOICE complaints, but (b) to engage

in W-O-M communication. Thus hypothesis H16 is

partly supported.

Process Model Versus Naive Model (Hypothesis H13)

Hypothesis H13 proposes to empirically examine the

two competing conceptualizations of the CCB process (see

figure 2.1, Chapter 2). One conceptualization proposes

that the^extent of dissatisfaction or the severity of the

problem is the sole explanation and predictor of the

specific complaint actions people intend to undertake

(Bearden and Teel 1983). This model, often criticized as

too simplistic, is referred to as the naive model for the

purpose of the following discussion (Day 1984). The

competing model proposes that the level of dissatisfac-

Page 166: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

156

tion merely triggers a process which includes cognitive

and affective evaluations, as antecedents to CCB actions

(Richins 1983, Day 1984). This model is, therefore,

referred to as the process model of CCB in the following

analysis. To examine this hypothesis, the two models

were evaluated, the first corresponding to the naive

model in which dissatisfaction directly affects the CCB

intentions. The second is the process model (see figure

5.2) in which dissatisfaction is necessary but not suffi­

cient for determining CCB responses. The two models used

in the present estimation are shown in figures 7.1 and

7.2. The dependent variables in both models are the

three dimensions of CCB: VOICE intentions, W-O-M

intentions and FORMAL intentions.

Latent Variable Structural Equations Modeling (LVSE)

methodology is used to estimate the naive model and the

process model individually for the four industries.

LISREL software Version VI is used for the analysis of

latent variable structural equations and obtain various

parameter estimates (Joreskog and Sorborm 1981). The use

of LVSE approach to obtain parameter estimates has

several advantages over the more traditional approach

(Bentler 1980). In particular this approach allows for

the simultaneous assessment of (a) the correspondence

rules (measurement model) linking the constructs in the

theory with their observable operationalizations, and (b)

Page 167: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

157

the structural model representing theory-implied linkages

among constructs (Bagozzi 1984). Further, this approach

estimates the relationships among constructs contained

within the theory as opposed to estimating relationship

among observables or linear composities of observables.

As such, measurement error is explicitly taken into

account and the estimated relationships are disattenuated

to the extent that the errors in measurement are random.

Table 7.9-7.12 provide the goodness of fit measures, the

coefficient of determination and the squared multiple

correlations for the dependent variables in the naive and

the process models. An examination of these tables shows

that irrespective of the industry, in comparison to the

naive model, the process model explains more of the

individual variance of the three dependent constructs.

For example, in the case of grocery data, the naive model

explains 14.8% of the variance in VOICE intentions

compared to 42.5% in the process model, and 17.2% for W-

0-M intentions as compared to 42.8% in the process model.

This conclusion is found irrespective of the dependent

variable considered or the nature of the industry.

In addition, the coefficient of determination for

all three dependent constructs of CCB intentions can be

compared for the process and the naive models. These

coefficients of determination are also shown in tables

7.9-7.12. In each of the four industries, the coeffi-

Page 168: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

158

cient of determination is substantially higher for the

process model. This suggests that, taken together, more

of the variance in the CCB intentions is explained in the

process model.

Further, the ratio of the chi-square value to the

degrees of freedom can also be compared between the two

model conceptualizations. A lower ratio would appear to

suggest a better fit to the data. This ratio for the

process model is lower than that for the naive model in

each of the four industry cases (see tables 7.9-7.12).

This suggests that the process model fits the data better

than the naive model. Thus, these consistent findings

support hypothesis H13 and suggest that the process

approach to the explanation and prediction of CCB may be

preferred to the naive approach of using dissatisfaction

alone.

Summary

Based on the above discussion and analysis, it can

be summarized that:

1. The extent of dissatisfaction or the severity of the

problem does influence the complaint actions people

intend to carry out.

2. However, the severity of the problem by itself does

not appear to be either a good explanation or a

prediction of CCB responses.

3. The competing model of CCB, the process model, which

Page 169: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

159

includes the cognitive as well as affective antece­

dents, appears to both (a) be better representation

of empirical observations, and (b) explain a higher

proportion of the variance in the complaint

intentions.

4. Hypothesis H13 is supported by the data across all

the four industries. Severity of the problem is,

therefore, necessary but not sufficient to explain

the variety of complaint responses. Instead, the

severity of the problem triggers a process, which

includes both affective and cognitive dimensions,

and results in intentions to engage in specific CCB

actions (see also Richins 1983).

Empirical Investigation of the Process Model (Hypotheses H4, H5, and H9-H12)

The basic process model of CCB for which parameters

are required to be estimated is depicted in figure 7.2.

The figure does not show the measurement part of the

model to maintain clarity. However, the measurement

model is based on the measurement properties set out in

Table 7.1. LVSE methodology was used to estimate the

various parameters using the LISREL software.

The focus of interest in the present dissertation is

the structural parameters linking attitude towards the

act of complaining to complaint actions (VOICE, W-O-M and

FORMAL) and those linking expectancy value judgments and

Page 170: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

160

complaint actions (hypotheses H4 and H5). In particular,

it is interesting to see how these parameters differ

across the four industry data (Hypothesis H12). It is

also the purpose of this dissertation to examine the

structural paths among prior experience (Hll), genera­

lized affective feelings (H10), internal and external

attributions of blame (H9) and other constructs hypoth­

esized in the proposed model (see figure 7.2). Figures

7.3-7.6 provide the estimated structural parameters in

each of the four industries. In order to maintain

clarity, only those parameters are shown that are signif­

icant (parameter>two times standard error). It must be

noted, however, that the standard error used to determine

"significant" parameters is actually the asymptotic

standard error available from the maximum likelihood

solution of the LISREL model. Table 7.13 depicts a

comparison of all estimated parameters across the four

industries. The "goodness of fit" properties for estima­

tion of the overall model are also included in Table

7. 13.

Goodness of Fit Measures

An examination of table 7.13 shows that in each of

the four models, the goodness of fit index is greater

than 0.6 and the root mean square error is around 0.1.

The chi-square test for the equality of the varcovariance

Page 171: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

161

matrix is, however, significant. Recent research has

shown that the chi-square test for assessing the goodness

of fit may be misleading for several reasons, among them

that (a) its sensitivity to sample size, and (b) its

"power" to reject the null hypotheses (of equal var­

covariance matrix) when it is false, is "unknown"

(Fornell and Larcker 1981). Researchers have suggested

other criterion to determine how "good" the data fits the

hypothesized model. Specifically, two procedures are

proposed: (1) examine the coefficient of determination

for the structural equations, and (2) set up nested

models to determine if a more restrictive model would fit

the data equally well (Bagozzi 1980). In the first

procedure, the "goodness" of the model should be ref­

lected in the coefficient of determination for the struc­

tural equations. One would typically expect the coeffi­

cient to be greater than 0.5. In the second procedure, a

nested model (A) is considered which is a special case of

a less restrictive model (B). Then the null hypothesis

of S= —Is tested versus the alternative hypothesis S =

The appropriate statistic is the change in the chi-square

value evaluated for the change in the degrees of freedom.

If model A fits the data equally well, it suggests that

the hypothesized model B is not a "good" fit to the data.

This method reduces the seriousness of Type II error with

the chi-square test (Fornell and Larcker 1981).

Page 172: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

162

Table 7.13 shows that the coefficient of determina­

tion for the structural parameters is greater than 0.5 in

each of the four cases. In fact, with the exception of

automotive repair data, the coefficient of determination

exceeds 0.65, indicating a "good" fit. Further, when a

more restrictive nested model is used, the chi-square

value is significant in each of the four cases indicating

that the more restrictive model is a "poor" fit to the

data than the hypothesized model. Thus the hypothesized

model appears to meet the criteria of reasonable fit to

the data in phase II.

Attitude Towards the Act of Complaining

Some interesting conclusions can be drawn from the

parameter estimates shown in figures 7.3-7.6 and Table

7.13. The direct effect of attitudes on VOICE intentions

is found to be significant only in the case of grocery

data and financial services data. In the case of medical

care, the indirect effects of attitudes are significant,

while ttiey are not important at all in effecting VOICE

intentions for automotive repair model. Further, the

relative effect of attitude as compared to expectancy

value judgments is high only in the grocery industry and

is consistently lower for the remaining three industries.

In other words, as products and services become more

complex and involved, the relative effect of attitudes on

Page 173: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

163

VOICE intentions declines.

Further, findings show that attitudes toward the act

of complaining have a direct as well as an indirect

effect on FORMAL intentions in three of the four indus­

tries (excluding grocery). However, the relative effect

of attitude construct is substantially less when compared

to the effect of expectancy value judgments. One expla­

nation as to why, for grocery data, attitudes do not seem

to have any direct or indirect effect on FORMAL inten­

tions could be that options such as legal actions are

rarely considered concerning grocery shopping. More

often, the data shows, VOICE is the preferred action. If

such an explanation is valid, then the data suggests that

attitudes towards the act of complaining do effect the

propensity of FORMAL intentions across the industries

investigated. However, as stated earlier this effect is

substantially lower relative to the effect of expectancy

value judgments.

Finally, attitudes are found to have a direct effect

on W-0-t1—intentions for the automotive repair and

financial services data. Such a finding is not evident

for the case of medical care, perhaps, because people are

more careful in talking to their friends and relatives

about their medical problems. This could also explain

the strong expectancy value effect on W-O-M intentions

observed in the medical care industry. The case of

Page 174: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

164

grocery retailing is indeed surprising. One would expect

that attitudes would play a role in consumer's propensity

to engage in W-O-M actions, yet the data do not support

this conclusion. However, in each of the four industries

investigated, the expectancy value judgments dominate in

their influence on W-O-M intentions, relative to

attitudes. This implies that attitude towards the act of

complaining is not a relatively strong predictor of W-O-M

intentions even in automotive repair and financial

services industry.

Thus hypothesis H5 is supported partly and with

several qualifications. That is, attitudes do have a

positive effect on intentions in general; however, the

effect is relatively dominant only in the grocery

industry for VOICE intentions.

Expectancy Value Judgments

An investigation of the effects of expectancy value

judgments on CCB intentions show a high degree of consis­

tency across the four industries. Expectancy value

judgments regarding FORMAL actions have a direct and

significant effect on FORMAL intentions, irrespective of

the industry. Similarly, W-O-M intentions are predicted

directly and significantly by expectancy value judgments

regarding W-O-M actions, once again irrespective of the

nature of the industry. For the case of VOICE

intentions, with the exception of the grocery industry

Page 175: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

165

(where attitudes have the direct and dominant effect) m

each of the remaining three industries, expectancy value

judgments regarding VOICE actions have a direct,

significant and dominant effect. This implies that, for

the most part, people's intentions to engage in CCB

actions of VOICE, W-O-M, or FORMAL, or any combination

thereof, are based on some cognitive activity concerning

the probability of the outcome and its value to the

consumer. This cognitive activity is, indeed, combined

with the affective feelings toward the act of complaining

in determining specific CCB intentions. Thus hypothesis

H4 is clearly supported by data across the four

industries.

Another interesting finding is reflected in the

negative and direct path between expectancy value judg­

ments of VOICE actions and W-O-M intentions. This path

is significant only for the case of automotive repair and

medical care industry. This implies that as the expec­

tancy value of VOICEd actions increases, it inhibits

W-Q-M communication. This in turn supports an earlier

conclusion, in that people engage in negative W-O-M

communication more often than they do in positive W-O-M

communication.

Page 176: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

166

Affective Feelings of Discontent and Alj^ana-Unn: Generalized Affect

Results indicate that the construct of generalized

affect (discontent and alienation) has a positive and

direct effect on the attitude toward the act of

complaining, across the four industries investigated.

This relationship tends to weaken somewhat in the medical

care and financial services data. Further, generalized

affect does not have a significant direct effect on

either VOICE or FORMAL intentions--its effect is only

through the attitudes construct. However, in the case of

W-O-M intentions, generalized affective feelings have a

direct and positive effect, except in the medical care

data, where all the effect is indirect. This suggests

that, for the most part, affective feelings of discontent

and alienation impact on the person's intentions to

engage in W-O-M communication directly. In other words,

as people become more discontent, they tend to have a

higher propensity to engage in W-O-M communication.

Perhaps^'^his is so because of the personal and social

nature of this (W-O-M) type of communication. Thus

hypothesis H10 is supported for VOICE and FORMAL

intentions but is not supported in the case of W-O-M

intentions.

Page 177: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

167

Internal and External Attributions of Blame

The constructs of internal and external attributions

of blame present mixed findings across the four

industries. Results show that, irrespective of the

industry, external attributions are negatively correlated

with internal attributions of blame (correlation not

significant for financial services only). This direction

of correlation is consistent with attribution literature

which suggests that higher external attributions imply

lower internal attributions.

External attributions of blame have a positive and

direct effect on expectancy value of W-O-M actions across

all four industries. The effect on internal attributions

is mixed. In grocery data, internal attributions have a

negative effect on the expectancy value of W-O-M actions.

While in medical care data, internal attributions are

found to have a positive effect and no effect is

evidenced in the remaining two industries. In addition,

internal attributions of blame have a significant

negrative path to expectancy value of VOICE and FORMAL

actions in financial services data only. No other paths

to expectancy value judgments are significant. Most of

the above parameters are in the expected direction; that

is, external attributions have a positive effect and

internal attributions have a negative effect on expec­

tancy value judgments. Yet the confidence in these

Page 178: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

168

results is adversely effected by the lack of interpre-

table consistency across the four industries. This

deficiency can be explained, in part, by the poor

measurement of these two constructs as discussed earlier.

A multi-item measure of internal and external attri­

butions that measures a greater range and depth of the

construct and meets reliability requirements would

address the measurement problem.

Table 7.13 also shows that external attribution of

blame has a direct effect on FORMAL intentions (automo­

tive repair data) and on VOICE intentions (medical care

and automotive repair data). This suggests that attri­

butions of blame may have a potential of direct effects

on intentions as well as indirect effects through expec­

tancy value judgments. Thus hypothesis H9 is partially

supported, with some qualification, by the data.

Prior Experience of Complaining

Previous research shows that demographic variables

(such as age, income, etc.) are correlated with the prior

experience of complaining; therefore, only the construct

of prior experience is used in the present analysis.

Table 7.13 depicts interesting and consistent findings

concerning prior experience of complaining and its

effects on the endogenous constructs in the model. Prior

experience has a direct and positive effect on expectancy

Page 179: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

169

value of W-O-M actions across all the four industries

investigated. In addition it does not have any direct

influence on W-O-M intentions. In other words, irrespec­

tive of the industry, all the effect of prior experience

on W-O-M intentions is indirect, and either through the

expectancy value judgments or the attitudes toward the

act of complaining.

On the other hand, prior experience has a direct and

positive effect on VOICE and FORMAL intentions in three

of the four industries (excluding financial services).

In financial services, the effect of prior experience on

VOICE and FORMAL intentions is indirect and through the

construct attitudes. However, while the direct paths

from prior experience to CCB intentions are significant,

they are relatively smaller than the effect of expectancy

value judgments. Thus hypothesis Hll is supported for W-

0-M intentions but not supported for VOICE or FORMAL

intentions.

Compari^en Across Industries

From the above analysis, it can be concluded that,

in general, the nature of the model is similar across the

four industries. However, the structural parameters,

implying the direction and strength of relationships are

widely different. In fact, it appears that grocery shop­

ping is most different from the other three industries.

Page 180: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

170

A multi-group analysis for comparing the equality of

structural parameters could not be run due to large

memory and CPU time requirements.

Summary

Based on the above discussion and findings, it can

be summarized that:

1. VOICE intentions are effected positively and

directly by three constructs: (a) cognitive factors,

specifically expectancy value judgment of VOICE

actions, (b) affective factors or attitude toward

the act of complaining, and (c) prior experience.

However, their relative effect tends to vary across

industries. The affective level has the major

impact in grocery shopping context, whereas

cognitive factors are the dominant influence in the

remaining three industries. The prior experience of

complaining tends to have a mediocre effect on VOICE

intentions. Thus people tend to VOICE their

complaints, primarily based on their perceptions of

seller's responsiveness and the value of the

outcome, except for the case of grocery shopping

where the feelings about the "goodness" or "badness"

of the act of complaining is the important

determinant.

2. W-O-M intentions are effected positively and

directly by three predictors: (a) cognitive factors.

Page 181: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

171

specifically, expectancy value judgments about the

VOICE and W-O-M actions, (b) generalized affect,

that is, feelings of discontent and alienation, and

(c) affective evaluation or attitude toward the act

of complaining. Unlike the case of VOICE

intentions, the relative effect of these predictors

is largely consistent across the four industries.

The expectancy value judgments of W-O-M actions is

by far the strongest predictor of W-O-M intentions.

In other words, people talk to their friends and

relatives about their dissatisfactions based on the

usefulness of such a communication and their percep­

tion about the expected response from their friends

and relatives. However, these intentions are

inhibited if there is a high expectancy value of

seller's responsiveness (only in automotive repair

and medical care). The other two predictors, gener­

alized affect and affective evaluation, both tend to

have a positive, direct, and relatively comparable

effect on the W-O-M intentions; except in the case

of medical care, where none of the two direct

effects are significant.

3. The third component of CCB intentions, FORMAL inten­

tions, is found to have up to four antecedents: (a)

expectancy value of FORMAL actions, (b) expectancy

value of W-O-M actions, (c) prior experience, and

Page 182: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

172

(d) affective evaluation or attitudes toward the act

of complaining. Of these predictors, the expectancy

value of FORMAL actions is, perhaps, the consis­

tently dominant predictor of FORMAL intentions.

However, the remaining three predictors have a posi­

tive and comparable influence on this dependent

variable. Thus, it appears that intentions to take

one's complaints to third parties, such as the

Better Business Bureau, are based on complex and

varied factors, including some cognitive assessment,

affective feelings, prior experience in dealing with

complaints, etc.

4. The data seems to suggest that the effects of attri­

butions of blame are not well defined, and perhaps,

these constructs do not have any direct effect on

CCB intentions. Their effect, it appears, is

indirect through the expectancy value judgments.

However, much confidence can not be attributed to

this result since the measurement of these two

constructs was found to be deficient.

Empirical Investigation of Expectancy Value Judgments in the Four Industries

(Hypothesis H18)

Table 7.14 provides the mean expectancy value

judgments for VOICE, W-O-M and FORMAL actions after the

extent of dissatisfaction is partialled out. In order to

Page 183: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

173

compare the mean expectancy values across the four

industries, a Bonferroni T test is conducted with the

level of significance as 0.05. The results for the

expectancy value (E-V) of VOICE actions shows that the

level of mean E-V is lowest for the medical care industry

and is significantly different from the other three mean

values. The remaining three E-Vs for VOICE actions are

not significantly different from each other. This

implies that people in general, perceive that it is

relatively unlikely that their problems and complaints

will be satisfactorily resolved by physicians/hospitals.

Similarly, the mean expectancy values for W-O-M

communication is found to be significantly higher in

automotive repair and medical care industry relative to

grocery shopping and financial services. Since previous

analysis shows that people in general tend to actively

engage in negative W-O-M rather than positive W-O-M, this

finding indicates that consumers of health care and auto­

motive repair, who are faced with low expectancy value of

VOICEd "Complaints to providers of service, tend to "even"

out by engaging in negative W-O-M communication. In

other words, the results seem to suggest that consumers

compensate for their frustration over redress for their

problems by talking to their friends and relatives about

their "bad" experiences.

Finally, regarding expectancy value of FORMAL

Page 184: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

174

actions, the findings show that the mean expectancy

values for seeking redress from third parties is lower m

medical care and financial service industries relative to

the grocery and automotive repair industries. This

implies that while in the case of grocery shopping and

automotive repair, consumers perceive that third parties,

such as the Better Business Bureau, have a higher chance

of "solving" their problems than in the case for medical

care or financial services.

In conclusion then, results seem to show that,

specifically for the case of medical care, the proba­

bility that VOICEd complaints would be satisfactorily

resolved is generally lower for both t.ne providers of the

service (physicians/hospitals) as well as the third par­

ties who could intervene. Thus the only course open to a

dissatisfied health care consumer appears to be W-O-M

communication to friends and relatives. These conditions

correspond well with the notion of loose monopolies, for

which medical care is an often cited example (Andreasen

1983). The present research is one of the few empirical

evidence for the existence of "loose monopolies"

conditions in medical care.

Summary

Based on the above discussion and analysis, it can

be summarized that:

1. Consumers' perceptions of seller's responsiveness

Page 185: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

175

and value of the outcome tends to vary markedly

across the four industries.

2. Expectancy value of VOICEd actions is low only in

the medical care industry. This implies that people

perceive either (a) poor response from physicians-

/hospitals to their complaints, or (b) a low value

of the desired outcome.

3. Expectancy value of W-O-M actions is high in automo­

tive repair and medical care industry only.

Combined with the earlier evidence that people

engage in negative W-O-M communication more fre­

quently, this implies that people tend to transmit

their bad experiences more often concerning automo­

tive care and/or medical care problems rather than

their grocery or financial dissatisfactions.

4. Expectancy value of FORMAL actions is found to be

low in financial and medical care industries as

compared to in grocery shopping. This implies that

people perceive a higher responsiveness of third

parties to their grocery and automotive repair

problems than to their medical care and/or financial

service dissatisfactions.

Page 186: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

176

TABLE 7.1

ALPHA RELIABILITIES FOR ALL CONSTRUCTS

Constnict

1. Alienation

2. Discontent

3. Attitudes

4. EV-VOICE

5. E V - W - O - M

6. EV-FORMAL

7. VOICE actions

8. W-O-M actions

9. FORMAL actions

10. Prior Exper­ience

Grocery

0.73

0.79

0.80

0.81

0.53

0.75

0.65

0.68

0.81

1.0

11. Internal Attributions of Blame 0.8

Auto Repair

0.74

0.82

0.73

0.88

0.49

0.72

0.69

0.80

0.80

1.0

0.8

Medical Care

0.86

0.84

0.81

0.94

0.59

0.64

0.80

0.80

0.85

l.O

0.8

FiDanciaL

0.84

0.86

0.77

0.94

0.64

0.79

0.80

0.61

0.85

1.0

0.8

12. Elxtemal Attributions of Blame 0.8 0.8 0.8 0.8

Page 187: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

177

TABLE 7. 2

PARTIAL CORRELATION TABLE FOR GROCERY DATA

EXPE

RIEN

CE

cr o

£.

h

5 ^^

f tDiLf i OR HI^^ DISSATISFACTION

PARTIAL CORRELATION

DF EXPECTANCY VALUE

To INTENTIONS.AniTUDEs

0.78

f ARTiAL CORRELATION

OF ExPECTAfjCY VALUE

To INTENTIONS.AniTUDEs

0.17

PARTIAL CORRELATION

OF ATTITUDES TO

INTENTIONS.EXPECTANCY

VALUE JuDGE €NTs

0.50

PARTIAL ^JPRELATION

O AniTUDES ro

INTENT 1 or. ".EXPECTANCY

VALUE JuDGer-tNTs

0.44

Page 188: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

178

TABLE 7.3

PARTIAL CORRELATION TABLE FOR AUTOMOTIVE REPAIR DATA

EX

PE

RIE

NC

E

oc o

£

Low

5 ^^

ftDiuH OR HIo^ DISSATISFACTION

PARTIAL CORRELATION

OF ExPECTA JCY VALUE

TO INTENTIONS.AniTUDEs

0.41

PARTIAL CORRELATION

OF EXPECTANCY VALUE

TO iNTENTFONS.ArTITUCES

0.08

PARTIAL CORRELATION

C^ ATTITUDES TO

INTENTIONS.EXPECTANCY

VALUE JuDCErtNTs

-0.03

PARTIAL 0^RPF:J^TION

OF ATTITUDES TT

INTENTIONS.ExPECTANCY

V A U E JUDGEACfn-S

0.25

Page 189: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

179

TABLE 7.4

PARTIAL CORRELATION TABLE MEDICAL CARE DATA

FOR

EX

PE

RIE

NC

E

cr o

Si

Low

5

III

ffeDiLw OR Hi(jH DISSATISFACTION

PARTIAL CORREUVTION

OF ExPECTAfJCY VALUE

TO INTENTIONS.ATTITICES

0.26

•^ARTIAL CORRELATION

OF Ex prTANCY VALUE

To INTENTtDNS.AninxKS

0.16

PARTIAL CORRELATION

OF ATTITUDES TO

INTENTIONS.EXPECTANCY

VALUE incEAtNTs

0.26

°ARTIAL CORRELATION

*> ArTiruDES T-

1 NTT ffri Of JS . LYPEC TANCY

VALUE JLDGEACNTS

0.15

Page 190: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

180

TABLE 7.5

PARTIAL CORRELATION TABLE FOR FINANCIAL DATA

EX

PE

RIE

NC

E

oc o

&

Low

s

f t D I l M OR H l W DiSSATlSFAaiON

PARTIAL CORRELATION

OF EXPECTANCY VALUE

To INTENTIONS.ATTITUDES

0.68

°ARTIAL CORRELATION

OF EXPECTANCY VALUE

To INTEMTIONS.ATTITLCES

0.43

PARTIAL CORRELATION

OF ATTITUDES ^O

INTENTIOTJS . EXPECTANCY

VALUE JuDGE^ENTs

0.14

PARTIAL V^RFLATION

f> AniT'jDEs TO

INTENTI^^.^ EXPECTANCY

VALUE JUDGEACNTS

0.18

Page 191: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

181

TABLE 7.6

CELL MEANS FOR "VOICE" INTENTIONS

GROCERY

INDUSTRY

AoTo REPAIR

INDUSTRY

•toiCAL CARF

INDIJSTRY

SERVICES

INDUSTRY

' n _ "VJIS fed £J-I\T\ "A:":.

"" LU 1

5,55

5,58

5,3

5,61

r^LL 2

5.24

5.26

4.73

4.82

CELL 3

4.01

5.37

4.77

5.62

' " ' L _ ^

4,36

5,13

4,21

3,75

."R|<

a.

k -

[ - . ''.r-'fENTS

: 'r^

CELL 1

'Su. 5

Li>

C£a2

CELL a

Page 192: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

182

TABLE 7.7

CELL MEANS FOR "W-O-M" INTENTIONS

GROCERY

INDUSTRY

INDUSTRY

' t o r o L CARE

INDUSTRY

FirWJCIAL SERVICES

INDUSTRY

""ELL 'iMi: ^OR E < " i n " ; • = ; ,

CELL 4

3.14

4.36

4.35

3.13

CELL 2

3.63

4.88

4,84

3,83

CELJ. 1

2.84

4,0

3.90

3.24

C E - 3

2.52

3,69

3,46

2.75

tf

t-

*—

: . ' -A^ " A - R I X

E V iHXE'ENTS

• t 1 " •

CELL J

^f.ii 5

Low

r £ a 2

CELL 4

Page 193: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

183

TABLE 7.8

CELL MEANS FOR "FORMAL" INTENTIONS

GROCERY

INDUSTRY

AtfTO REPAIR

INDUSTRY

ffeoicAL CARE

INDUSTRY

FINANCIAL SERVICES

IriDUSTRV

''.^'^ "EANS ^CP E / - A ^ '•'^r^lx

r^LL 1

2.07

2.91

2.^5

2.15

CEU 2

1.82

3.07

2.72

1.89

CELL 4

1.61

2.40

2.33

1.88

f '^ 3

1.64

2.03

2.28

1.89

T.'-AF "/.r^ix

1 ' '

L

t

0 .

• •

.•

E-V 'ijDc-e tsTs

• ' ; v

CELLI

' F ( L ^

LD-*

C£ix2

f£UL^

Page 194: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

184

TABLE 7.9

A COMPARISON OF NAIVE AND PROCESS MODELS FOR GROCERY DATA

CHARACTERISTIC

1. Chi-square value

2. Degrees of Freedom

3. Ratio (1/2)

4. R—Square VOICE intentions W—O—M intentions FORMAL intentions

5. Coefikient of Determination

NAIVE MODEL

73.97

39

1.90

0.192 0.001 0.008

0.208

PROCESS MODEL

1887.97

1193

1.58

0.456 0.560 0.419

0.674

Page 195: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

185

TABLE 7.10

A COMPARISON OF NAIVE AND PROCESS MODELS FOR AUTOMOTIVE REPAIR DATA

CHARACTERISTIC

1. Chi—square value

2. Degrees of Freedom

3. Ratio (1/2)

4. R—Square VOICE intentions W—0—M intentions FORMAL intentions

5. Coefikient of Determination

NAIVE MODEL

66.24

39

1.70

0.187 0.144 0.041

0.266

PROCESS MODEL

1879.32

1193

1.58

0.161 0.379 0.301

0.509

Page 196: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

186

TABLE 7.11

A COMPARISON OF NAIVE AND PROCESS MODELS FOR MEDICAL CARE DATA

CHARACTERISTIC

1. Chi-square value

2. Degrees of Freedom

3. Ratio (1/2)

4. R—Square VOICE intentions W—0—M intentions FORMAL intentions

5. Coefficient of Determination

NAIVE MODEL

71.42

39

1.83

0.274 0.215 0.061

0.363

PROCESS MODEL

1667.34

1092

1.53

0.247 0.681 0.260

0.646

Page 197: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

187

TABLE 7.12

A COMPARISON OF NAIVE AND PROCESS MODELS FOR FINANCIAL DATA

CHARACTERISTIC

1. Chi-square value

2. Degrees of Freedom

3. Ratio (1/2)

4. R—Square VOICE intentions W—0—M intentions FORMAL intentions

5. Coefficient of Determination

NAIVE MODEL

69.0

39

1.77

0.148 0.172 0.036

0.287

PROCESS MODEL

1939.68

1193

1.62

0.553 0.819 0.447

0.784

Page 198: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

188

TABLE 7.13

ESTIMATED PARAMETERS FOR THE PROCESS MODEL MAXIMUM LIKELIHOOD STRUCTURAL

PARAMETERS^

Parameter

beta54 beta64 beta74 beta51 beta61 beta62 beta72 beta73

gamma61 gamma52 gamma72 gamma41 gamma42 gammal2 gamma22 gamma32 gammalS ganuna23 gammaSS gammal4 gamma24 gamma34 gamma54 gammai&4 gamma74

phil2

chi—square

degrees of freedom

goodness of fit index

root mean square error

R-square eta5 eta6 eta7

Grocery

0.37(.12) 0.04(.12)

-0.09(.14) 0.12(.08)

-0.00(.12) 1.03(.31) 0.71(.31) 0.83(.32)

0.23(.13) 0.08(.04) 0.28(.07) 0.49(.16) 0.12(.06)

-0.03(.06) 0.11 (.05)

-0.01 (.03) -0.03(.07) -0.09(.06) -0.01(.04) -0.23(.08)

0.05(.06) -0.04(.06) -0.03(.05)

0.07(.09) -O.Ol(.lO)

0.16(.06)

1887.97

1193

0.650

0.099

0.456 0.560 0.419

Auto

0.02(.18) 0.21(.18) 0.64(.23) 0.24(.17)

-0.33(.10) 0.58(.21) 0.22(.21) 0.75(.42)

0.18(.12) 0.24(.08) 0.14(.08) 0.33(.ll) 0.07(.05)

-0.08(.07) 0.12(.05)

-0.03(.03) 0.13(.09)

-0.02(.05) 0.02(.03)

-0.07(.08) 0.08(.06)

-0.00(.03) 0.09(.09)

-0.01 (.08) 0.21(.10)

0.13(.06)

1879.32

1193

0.637

0.106

0.161 0.379 0.301

Medkai

0.24(.18) 0.01(.12) 0.25(.17) 0.33(09) -0.10(.06) 0.86(.21) 0.32(.19) 1.21(.52)

0.15(.ll) 0.20(.08) 0.09(.07) 0.29(.13) 0.11(.05)

-0.08(.06) 0.10(.03)

-0.03(.05) 0.04(.14)

-0.26(.12) 0.02(.05)

-0.21(.14) 0.47(.14) 0.01 (.05) 0.18(.09) 0.01(.08)

-0.01 (.11)

0.18(.06)

1667.34

1092

0.671

0.114

0.247 0.681 0.260

Financial

0.42(18) 0.22(.l2) 0.23(.16) 0.60(.09) 0.06(.06) 0.55(.12) 0.17(.10) 1.04(.25)

0.22(.09) 0.06(.07) 0.07(.07) 0.20(.09) 0.13(.06)

-0.00(.09) 0.21 (.08)

-0.04(.05) -0 .37( . l l )

0.08(.10) -0.13(.07) -0 .17( . l l ) -0.15(17) -0.04(.06) -0.05(.09)

0.2O(.O7) 0.02(.09)

0.18(.07)

1939.68

1193

0.601

0.115

0.553 0.819 0.447

* Standard error in parenthesis

Page 199: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

TABLE 7.14

ESTIMATE PARAMETERS FOR THE PROCESS MODEL STANDARDIZED MEASUREMENT

PARAMETERS

Parameter Grocery Auto Medical Financial

1 8 9

KSTs lamdall lamda21 lamdaSl lamda41 lamda51 lamda61 lamda71 Iamda81 lamda91 lamdalO,! lamdall,! Iamdal2,l lamdal3,l lamdal4,l lamdal5,l lamdal6,l lamdal7,l lamdal8,l

lamdal9,2

lamda20,3 lamda21.3

ETA'S lamdal 1 lamda21 lamda31

lamda4,2 lamda5,2 lamda6,2 lamda7,2

lamda8,3 lamda9,3 lamdal0,3

lamdal 1,4 lamdal 2,4 lamdal3,4 lamdal 4,4 lamdal 5,4 lamdal6,4 lamdal 7,4 lamdal8,4 lamdal9,4 lamda20,4

0.36 0.60 0.39 0.61 0.39 0.79 0.49 0.65 0.45 0.47 0.59 0.59 0.43 0.45 0.69 0.47 0.34 0.34

1.00

0.80 0.80

0.61 0.88 0.85

0.43 0.50 0.61 0.46

0.36 0.90 0.90

0.60 0.47 0.53 0.36 0.68 0.69 0.36 0.63 0.64 0.43

0.57 0.62 0.48 0.46 0.44 0.75 0.61 0.64 0.57 0.41 0.67 0.50 0.38 0.46 0.66 0.61 0.49 0.65

1.00

0.80 0.80

0.72 0.99 0.80

0.42 0.38 0.95 0.09

0.25 0.94 0.87

0.48 0.53 0.48 0.36 0.31 0.67 0.37 0.53 0.60 0.37

0.47 0.76 0.65 0.58 0.35 0.54 0.49 0.48 0.56 0.57 0.73 0.60 0.63 0.54 0.76 0.74 0.67

1.00

0.80 0.80

0.85 0.98 0.90

0.57 0.79 0.36 0.26

0.48 0.87 0.70

0.49 0.39 0.56 0.54 0.56 0.55 0.57 0.77 0.63

0.63 0.67 0.61 0.35 0.89 0.62 0.68 0.52 0.69 0.47 0.67 0.63 0.82 0.42 0.50 0.45 0.38 0.41

1.00

0.80 0.80

0.90 0.92 0.90

0.79 0.39 0.88 0.33

0.48 0.96 0.87

0.52 0.34 0.57 0.35 0.39 0.80 0.39 0.50 0.45 0.60

Page 200: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

190

TABLE 7.15

A COMPARISON OF EXPECTANCY VALUE JUDGEMENTS ACROSS THE FOUR INDUSTRIES

TYPE OF EV INDUSTRY

GROCERY AUTO FINANCIAL MEDICAL

VOICE E-V 77.73 74.41 78.04 60.40 ********************************* A.^^A.A.A.

GROCERY FINANCIAL AUTO MEDICAL

W-O-M E-V 46.98 47.09 61.80 61.35 *******************

GROCERY AUTO MEDICAL FINANCLVL

FORMAL E-V 44.14 37.04 33.03 31.07 *********************************

Page 201: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

191

FIGURE 7.1

A NAIVE MODEL OF CCB INTENTIONS

Page 202: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

192

X 3

cn z o M H Z U H Z

CQ U U

u. o J U] a o i : [Si C/l

u o a: a.

Page 203: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

193

u X D G

Z

o M Z U]

Z

a] < U H U <

Q Lu a >

a: - ] u Ld U Q O a a:

u o <

M

0 1 C<]

111 I

Page 204: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

194

^ •

c LI] Q: D CD M U.

z o M H Z Ul < E- H Z < M Q

CQ X U M U <

0. U. Ul O flC

J Ul u > Q M O H E O

c Q O Ul H H D < < S M X H O cn u. Ul

Ul X t-

Page 205: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

195

in •

c Ul X D G M

u.

cn z o M H z Ul f-z < ^ t-

< CQ Q U U Ul

X u. < o u -] -J Ul < Q U O M z: Q

U] Q E Ul H a: < o E U. M H cn Ul

Ul I H

Page 206: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

196

UD •

[^

Ul X D G M

u.

cn z G M f-z Ul b-z M

< CQ H U < U Q

U, -J a < M

-J u Ul z Q < G Z E ^

U. Q Ul X H O < u. E M H cn Ul

Ul X H

Page 207: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

CHAPTER 8

SUMMARY, IMPLICATIONS AND LIMITATIONS

The purpose of this final chapter is to examine the

implications and contributions, as well as the

limitations, of the present research from the standpoint

of three interested parties--academicians, managers, and

public policy officials. Further, this chapter will

attempt to explore the potential impact of this

dissertation research on other areas within marketing and

related disciplines. Finally, directions for future

research are suggested. The chapter begins with an

overview of the whole dissertation and a brief summary of

the results.

An Overview of the Dissertation

This dissertation reviewed the literature for the

post-purchase phenomena in consumer behavior, that is,

behaviors, cognitions and attitudes that result from or

occur after the consumer makes a purchase. The review

indicated that the area can be divided in to two distinct

streams of research: (a) consumer satisfaction and dis­

satisfaction (CS/D) area, and (b) consumer complaint

behavior (CCB) area. Further, the literature review

suggested that, while the area of consumer satisfaction

and dissatisfaction (CS/D) has benefited from the con-

197

Page 208: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

198

solidatlon of several proposed theoretical frameworks and

considerable empirical activity, the area of consumer

complaint behavior (CCB) appears to give an impression of

a relatively fragmented structure of research (Foikes

1984). In fact, researchers debate the empirical

validity of a CCB process. On one hand, several re­

searchers contend that the extent of dissatisfaction by

itself determines the CCB actions taken and no other

process variables are involved. This is referred to as

the "naive model" in the present research (Bearden and

Teel 1983). In contrast, several other researchers

support the notion that dissatisfaction is a necessary,

but not a sufficient condition in predicting or

explaining CCB (Day 1984). The latter contend that the

sufficient conditions involve a process incorporating

many different perspectives. This is referred to as the

"process model" in the present research. Several

different frameworks to understand and explain this

process have been proposed, such as the frameworks of

attribuTlon of blame (Foikes 1984), expectancy value

judgments (Hirschman 1970), and the attitude toward the

act of complaining (Richins 1982). However, much of the

empirical work in the CCB area has not attempted to

investigate or build upon these conceptual frameworks

(Richins 1979). Thus, frequent calls have been made to

develop and empirically investigate a theoretical model

Page 209: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

199

of CCB which builds upon and consolidates the current

literature base in the area (Day 1984).

The Proposed Model of CCB

A holistic model of consumer complaint behavior has

been proposed that attempts to explain and predict the

process consumers presumably undergo following perceived

dissatisfaction with a purchase. The model is holistic

in the sense that it incorporates four different concep­

tual frameworks, each representing a major stream of

thought in the CCB literature. The included frameworks

are: (a) phenomenological model (Landon 1977), (b) attri­

bution theory model (Valle and Waliendorf 1977; Foikes

1984), (c) economic model (Hirschman 1970; Andreasen

1983), and (d) psychological model (Richins 1979; Day

1984). The linkages between the constructs representing

the various models are developed from a theoretical

standpoint, then the proposed holistic model of CCB is

partially formalized in order to set down clearly its

assumptions, axioms, and law-like statements (Hunt 1983).

It is hoped that this formalization may help future

researchers to criticize and build upon this theoretical

framework.

A part of the proposed holistic model of CCB is then

empirically investigated to determine how well the model

represents the "real world." In order to provide a

richer understanding of the boundaries of the proposed

Page 210: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

200

model, the model was tested in four different industries.

The industries chosen for study included grocery shop­

ping, automotive repair, medical care, and financial

services. This selection covered a wide range in the

tangibility of the product/service provided and the

nature and extent of buyer-seller interaction. A higher

proportion of services were selected from yet another

standpoint, that of concerns regarding the growing dis­

satisfaction with service industries in the US (Day and

Bodur 1977). The effect of this growing dissatisfaction

on consumer welfare is an important concern to many

public policy officials.

Sample Selection

A two-stage area sampling was the basic sampling

methodology employed. Census tracts were randomly

selected and, within each selected tract, households were

systematically selected. An initial study (phase I) was

conducted to empirically investigate some of the

constructs, ascertain their measurement properties and

develop shortened scales to be used in the final survey

(phase II). A sample size of 1000 households was

selected for phase I. The phase II survey consisted of

four independent samples, one each for the four

industries investigated. An effort was made to ensure

that the same household was not selected for more than

one sample. A sample size of 1000 households was

Page 211: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

201

selected for each of the four studies in phase II.

Survey Methodology

A mail survey methodology was employed for phase II

research, while a personal drop off method was employed

for the much smaller phase I sample. Callbacks were made

after one week of mailing or dropping off the question­

naire. Two methods of callbacks were used, by telephone

and by a reminder post card. The response rates differed

substantially between phase I and II studies. Overall

51% response rate was obtained for phase I compared to

15-17% in phase II. Though nonresponse bias is evident

in phase II data, it is suggested that the severity of

the problem may be somewhat mitigated because only those

respondents who had recently experienced a dissatisfac­

tion were eligible to complete the survey. Since, there

is no way to a priori identify and sample dissatisfied

households, the "effective" sample size may actually be

reduced.

Survey Instrument

The constructs to be investigated were operation­

alized into multi-item scales. Some of the constructs,

such as alienation and discontent, were operationalized

in a manner identical to previous research efforts

(Lundstrom and Lament 1976; Allison 1978). Others, such

as expectancy value Judgments, were adapted from similar

Page 212: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

202

operationalizations in the consumer behavior literature

(Bagozzi 1982; Richins 1982). The measurement properties

of many constructs, therefore, were either reported or

could be inferred from published literature.

The survey instrument was then constructed using

these operationalized scales. However, several other

criteria were also used in developing the survey instru­

ment. Chiefly, an effort was made to ensure clarity,

readability, and continuity to maintain respondent

interest and motivation. It was also considered desir­

able to position all behavioral questions, for instance,

prior experience, before the attitudinal or expectancy

value questions to reduce bias in reported behaviors

(Labaw 1980). The instrument was then pretested before

a final version was developed.

Research Methodology

Several different methodologies were employed to

empirically investigate the various hypotheses. A

particii±ar methodology was selected if it appeared to

afford an appropriate way to test the particular

hypothesis. For instance, the holistic model was tested

using the Latent Variable Structural Equations (LVSE)

modeling since it afforded the estimation of both the

measurement and the structural parameters within a single

methodology (Joreskog and Sorborm 1981). Similarly, Item

Page 213: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

203

Response Theory was used to study the information charac­

teristics and measurement properties of certain scales

(Hulin, Drasgow and Miller 1983). Thus the various

methods used for data analysis included Analysis of

Variance, partial correlations and the Bonferroni test

for multiple means.

Results

The results suggested that, of the two competing

conceptualizations of CCB, the process model of CCB

appeared to be more representative of the data. The

present research, therefore, favors the argument that

dissatisfaction is a necessary but not a sufficient

condition for explaining or predicting CCB actions (Day

1984).

The data analyzed also provides evidence of a three

dimensional structure of CCB actions. These dimensions

represent: (a) VOICE actions, i.e., complaining directly

to the seller or provider of the product/service, (b)

W-O-M communication, implying either positive or negative

communication to friends and relatives about the dissat­

isfying experience, and (c) FORMAL actions, i.e., actions

involving third parties such as the Better Business

Bureau. This suggests that consumers distinguish between

FORMAL and INFORMAL actions as well as between actions

involving dyadic and third parties. The third dichotomy

proposed for the CCB classification (Figure 2.4, chapter

Page 214: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

204

2), that of the purpose of complaining as being either to

seek redress or change future behavior, does not appear

to be supported by data. Next, some key findings are

summarized (also see Table 8.1).

VOICE Intentions

The results afforded many insights into the process

that results in the tripartite CCB responses. VOICE

intentions, it appears, are effected by both the

affective and the cognitive evaluations. The impact of

affective factors, however, tends to decrease, while the

effect of cognitive evaluations tends to increase as the

product or service becomes more complex or involving,

such as financial services. The data also indicated that

as the consumers gain experience in complaining, their

propensity for future VOICE actions tends to increase.

In addition to this direct effect, prior experience also

plays a moderating role on the process leading to VOICE

intentions. When prior experience is low, consumers tend

to favdT^a cognitive process, while, when the experience

is high, the effect of affective factors tends to

increase.

Word-Of-Mouth Communication

The second dimension of CCB intentions, W-O-M

communication, appears to be based on somewhat more

complex processes than the VOICE intentions. The

Page 215: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

205

processes involve the additive and relatively comparable

effects of three different predictors: (a) a cognitive

evaluation of the expected response of friends and

relatives to W-O-M, (b) an affective evaluation toward

the act of complaining, and (c) generalized affect

concerning overall feelings of alienation and discontent.

The effect of the cognitive evaluation, however, appears

to be slightly dominant across the four industries inves­

tigated. This process is made still more complex by the

evidence of a negative effect of expectancy value of

VOICE actions on W-O-M intentions in medical care and

automotive repair data. In other words, the greater the

seller responsiveness to consumer complaints, the more it

inhibits the desire to engage in W-O-M intentions for

these two industries.

FORMAL Actions

Finally, the intentions to engage in FORMAL actions

that involve third parties is also found to stem from a

relatlveriy complex process that includes the additive and

positive effects of several predictors: (a) expectancy

value of FORMAL actions, (b) expectancy value of W-O-M

actions, (c) prior experience, and (d) affective feelings

toward the act of complaining. Of these predictors, the

cognitive factor regarding FORMAL actions (expectancy

value Judgments) is, perhaps, the most crucial deter-

Page 216: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

206

minant of the intentions to engage in FORMAL actions.

Indeed, these FORMAL intentions tend to increase as

attitudes become more positive, and/or prior experience

becomes high, and/or consumers perceive high expectancy

value from talking to other uninvolved parties, such as

friends and relatives.

Alienation and Discontent

The results suggest that alienation and discontent

are distinct concepts but yet two dimensions of an under­

lying global construct of generalized affect toward the

market place. Alienations seems to occur at the higher

end of this underlying trait, while discontent occurs at

the lower end. Though these two dimensions are

positively correlated, two conclusions were drawn: (a)

the correlation index between the two dimensions tends to

decrease from the lower end to the higher end of the

underlying construct, and (b) these two dimensions

correlate inversely with an external variable, the

attitude toward the act of complaining.

Further, shortened versions of the alienation and

discontent scales were developed and then employed in the

phase II of the research. Using the scales it was found

that while generalized affective feelings have a positive

and direct effect on W-O-M intentions, much of its effect

on CCB intentions is indirect and through the construct

of attitude toward the act of complaining.

Page 217: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

207

of attitude toward the act of complaining.

Attributions of Blame

The attributions of blame investigated in the

present research were found to effect CCB intentions

indirectly through their effect on the construct of

expectancy value Judgments. However, some direct effects

were also observed on VOICE and W-O-M intentions. The

results also suggested that the observed effects

involving attributions of blame lacked interpretational

consistency across the four industries investigated.

This deficiency was proposed to be the result of poor

measurement of the two constructs, particularly the

external attributions of blame. The results involving

attribution of blame must therefore be interpreted with

caution.

Managerial Implications

This research adds to the list of prescriptive

directives for a practicing manager that would assist in

building loyalty through customer satisfaction (Richins

1983; Biehal 1983). Many of these prescriptions

(normative statements) are not based on positive

statements that are empirically testable. While such

prescriptions may seem to work well in some situations,

normative statements that stem from some positive model

possess greater validity and reliability across varying

situations (Hunt 1983). Additionally, a positive model

Page 218: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

208

provides managers with insights into the complex

processes that underlie seemingly innocuous actions such

as to VOICE complaints and to engage in W-O-M communica­

tion with friends and relatives. The present research is

based on a positive model and provides several prescrip­

tive insights for the practicing manager in at least four

industries, those of grocery retailing, automotive

repair, medical care and financial services.

One major insight provided by this research is in

its direct comparison of cognitive and affective factors

as predictors of VOICE intentions. Up until now,

research had indicated that each of the factors was

individually important, implying managers may need to

change both the expectancy value Judgments and attitudes

in order to encourage customers to VOICE their

complaints, thus providing firms an opportunity to

redress genuine dissatisfactions (Richins 1982; Fornell

and Didow 1980). The findings here suggest that this may

not be entirely true. For low involvement products and

servicesT such as grocery shopping, results indicate that

attitudinal evaluations are the key to VOICE options,

whereas for high involvement products and services, such

as medical care and financial services, the cognitive

evaluations of seller's responsiveness are the main

predictor of VOICE options. A practical implication of

this finding is that managers in high involvement Indus-

Page 219: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

209

tries can effectively use communication channels to

modulate consumers' perceptions of seller's responsive­

ness to VOICEd complaints. Specifically, since expec­

tancy value Judgments are based on cognitive evaluations,

rational appeals, for instance, provide managers with an

effective vehicle to change, modify, or reinforce these

cognitive evaluations. Managers in low involvement

industries would, on the other hand, find much less

latitude since previous research suggests that affective

evaluations are more resistant to firm-initiated messages

and, thus, difficult to change or modify (Engel and

Blackwell 1982). Fortunately, many of the low involve­

ment industries are characterized by frequent buyer-

seller interactions, such as grocery snopping. These

multiple interactions can provide effective opportunities

to the manager to nurture positive affective feelings

among its customers, through personnel training and store

policies. The positive feelings could then be the basis

for building loyalty by encouraging consumers to bring

their chtssatisfactions back to the store rather than

choose other avenues (e.g., EXIT). Low involvement

industries thus provide managers with a greater challenge

in their efforts to persuade their target markets to

VOICE their complaints.

Results regarding W-O-M communication afford yet

another implication for managers. It appears that people

Page 220: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

210

tend to engage in negative W-O-M more frequently than m

positive W-O-M, at least in automotive repair and medical

care contexts. In addition, in almost all industries,

W-O-M seems to be directly effected by feelings of

discontent and alienation. This implies that consumers

who are more discontent and alienated do more W-O-M

communication to their friends and relatives. Together,

these results suggest that discontented consumers, who

feel that sellers may not respond to their concerns and

problems, may carry the "bad experience" to their friends

and relatives. Consumers who find sellers responsive to

their VOICEd complaints, in contrast, may exert rela­

tively less effort in talking about their "good

experience." Since W-O-M has generally been found to be

persuasive (Richins 1983; Engel and Blackwell 1982),

these results indicate that this powerful tool may remain

less amenable to strategic control of the manager. In

fact, practitioners may well find their influence limited

to containing the negative effects of W-O-M. Yet,

practit±oners may find this limited control productive

in stemming the erosion of their customer base, specifi­

cally in automotive repair and medical care industries,

where negative W-O-M is linked to poor responsiveness of

sellers to customers' dissatisfactions.

At a more macro level, industries may find it

fruitful to consider consumer relations as a serious

Page 221: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

211

strategic option--an option that would allow industries

to open communication channels to consumers, reduce the

level of discontent and alienation from the market place,

and thus curtail negative W-O-M. Such a strategic option

may afford managers in the long run to build consumers'

confidence in the industry's genuine concern for customer

welfare, and thus provide the foundation for a stable

consumer loyalty.

While sellers would like to be perceived by their

target consumers as being responsive to their needs and

problems, more often than not seller's perceptions of

their own responsiveness is widely divergent from their

customers perceptions of seller responsiveness. If

consumers could suggest what sellers ought to do in

response to particular customer dissatisfactions and

problems, it would provide managers with insights into

and guidelines for improving the effectiveness of their

complaint handling mechanisms. To this end, respondents

were asked two open ended questions: "what do you think

(sellers^ ought to do to solve the type of problem you

had?" and "what do you think (sellers) ought to do to

improve their service to the consumers?" Some typical

responses for each of the four phase II surveys are

quoted verbatim in Table 8. 3.

Some of the suggestions appear to be similar across

the four industries, such as sincere, honest and helpful

Page 222: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

212

BtB±f (financial services), cashiers (grocery retailing),

automotive repair men (automotive repair) and physicians

(medical care). This aspect of consumer responsiveness

or orientation is also one of the more frequently

mentioned responses. In other words, consumers value and

appreciate sellers that "hear" consumer complaints and

respond with sincerity and fairness.

Several other suggestions are more specific to the

industry investigated. In grocery retailing, consumers

are concerned about outdated stock (e.g., dairy

products), cleanliness in the store and carelessness on

the part of grocery sackers. These concerns are exempli­

fied by the case of a housewife who ran into a dead

rodent while trying to locate a wash room in a grocery

supermarket (a typical grocery store in Lubbock would

have wash rooms at the back, near the stocking area).

Cleanliness appears to be an important concern since the

perceptions of a poorly maintained store are extended to

the produce and the food it carries.

Poor quality work and overcharging appear to be the

most frequent allegations against the automotive repair

industry. Many consumers felt that automotive repairmen

must stick to their estimates and not go over until such

overruns are approved. A small segment of the res­

pondents, mostly women, felt a sense of helplessness in

dealing with automotive repair industry.

Page 223: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

213

Perhaps the strongest criticism in medical care

appeared to be the perceived out-of-proportion costs

which were attributed to "greedy" physicians and

hospitals. Another concern verbalized was that

physicians ought to spend more time with the patient

explaining and talking about the patient's problem.

There was also a strong desire to engage in negative W-0-

M communication by dissatisfied consumers because "there

is not much that can be done" since "most patients are

too frightened or feel poorly to take up for their

rights."

Financial service providers were criticized by

consumers for their working hours. Many felt that

consumer needs would be better served if the banks would

be open for some time on the week-end (e.g., Saturday) or

after 5 p.m. Consumers also felt that upon closing their

accounts at a particular bank, no one would bother to

find out "why I moved my account." This suggested to

the consumer a total lack of seller responsiveness.

Ottrer specific responses are listed in Table 8. 3.

An examination of this table combined with the under­

standing of the CCB process may provide managers with a

measure of their current effectiveness in dealing with

consumer dissatisfactions as well as normative guidelines

for upgrading the target consumers' perceptions of their

responsiveness.

Page 224: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

214

Further, this research also provides the manager

with a process model of CCB that can assist in the

understanding of the mechanism that consumers typically

go through once they are faced with a dissatisfying

product or service. Managers could examine this process

model for their target market and compare it with the

models tested here. This comparison could afford

practitioners with insights into the specific situations

they face, and might provide directions for effective

strategies for building long term consumer loyalties.

Public Policy Implications

The above findings also afford several suggestions

to public policy officials engaged in ensuring, through

regulatory control or otherwise, consumer welfare and

fairness in buyer-seller interactions. In particular,

this study suggests that consumers' perceptions of

sellers' responsiveness to their VOICEd complaints vary

across the four industries investigated. In financial

services and grocery retailing, the level of sellers'

responsiveness is high, and in automotive repair this

level is in the middle range; but in the medical care

industry the consumers' perceptions of physicians/

hospitals' responsiveness to their dissatisfactions

plunges to a low level. In other words, consumers of

medical care are relatively discouraged in VOICEing their

Page 225: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

215

would not result in any fruitful response. When dissat­

isfied consumers do not choose the VOICE option in an

industry, their choices are limited to EXIT in so far as

buyer-seller interactions are concerned. A direct

implication of this is that since consumers' complaints

and problems are not verbalized, the providers of medical

care, for instance, hear less about negative consumer

experiences and, consequently, become even more

insensitive to consumer needs. The end result is that

overall consumer welfare in that industry suffers

(Andreasen 1983). If, in addition, the EXIT option is

blocked as is found to exist in the medical care

industry, the dissatisfied consumer is helplessly caught

between finding VOICE actions fruitless and yet not able

to EXIT from the seller. This condition is character­

istic of "loose monopolies" (Hirschman 1970).

Industries, such as medical care, which indicate signals

of loose monopolies require careful monitoring by public

policy officials in order to provide channels for

upgrading consumer welfare.

Results also provide public policy officials with

consumers' evaluations of the usefulness of approaching

formal agencies, such as the Better Business Bureau, to

intervene and help solve their problems with sellers.

Such uninvolved third parties have always been assumed to

provide a desirable nonregulatory control on sellers and

Page 226: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

216

ensure customer sovereignty in the market place.

Findings obtained here afford several observations on the

role of such formal agencies. The consumers' perceptions

of expectancy value or responsiveness of these agencies

is relatively low (lower than perceptions of seller res­

ponsiveness), particularly in medical care and financial

services. In other words, consumers are less sure that

such formal agencies can assist them in obtaining redress

in general, and particularly for financial services and

medical care related problems. This implies that public

policy officials may not be able to rely completely on

Better Business Bureaus and industry associations to

provide dissatisfied consumers with an easy access for

arbitration. The problem appears to be severe in medical

care, where VOICE actions are inhibited, the expectancy

value of FORMAL means to solve dissatisfactions is

particularly low, and EXIT may be blocked (Andreasen

1985). Such conditions demand the serious attention of

public policy officials.

Theoretical Implications

Several implications can be suggested for theoreti­

cal work in the CCB area. In particular, these results

imply that the explanation and prediction of the varied

consumer complaint actions might involve multiple antece­

dents, each representing a different stream of thought.

Page 227: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

217

Thus far researchers have concentrated their efforts in

exploring how individual antecedents affect the CCB

actions (Richins 1982; Foikes 1984; Fornell and Robinson

1983). The task of combining different streams, each

attempting to predict the same dependent variable, has

been largely ignored (see for an exception. Day 1984).

The empirical evidence presented here suggests that such

a task (of combining different streams) can provide addi­

tional insights into the process underlying the CCB

responses and is worthy of serious investigation.

The dissertation also attempted a first step towards

a comprehensive framework by proposing a holistic model

of CCB. Linkages among antecedents and predictors are

first developed from a theoretical standpoint and then

partially formalized to explicitly state its assumptions,

axioms, and law-like statements. It is hoped that this

partial formalization would assist future researchers to

criticize and further develop the holistic model. This

preliminary step and the empirical support of certain

proposed hypotheses can provide the building block for an

integrated and well grounded framework for explaining and

predicting CCB actions.

Further, the proposed holistic model of post-

purchase processes draws several parallels with the

holistic model of pre-purchase processes (Bagozzi 1982).

Both models suggest two major routes towards the

Page 228: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

218

dependent intentions, the cognitive (expectancy value)

route and the affective (attitude) route. Both models

also incorporate the effect of prior experiences or past

behaviors on future intentions and behaviors (Nord and

Peter 1980). The model for post-purchase processes

contains, in addition, other explanatory variables, such

as the attribution of blame and generalized affect.

Nevertheless, from a theoretical standpoint the

similarity between the model for post-purchase and pre-

purchase processes is very appealing. Since the holistic

model for post-purchase processes is also generally

supported by data, this similarity backed by empirical

confidence provides impetus to the theoretical thinking

and search for general frameworks for explaining

different dimensions of consumer behavior.

Limitations

This research has several limitations which must be

considered in evaluating the above results and their

implications. One key limitation is with the population

specification of this research. All households within

the city of Lubbock were considered as elements of the

population for sampling purposes. To this extent the

generalizability of the results to other populations may

be limited.

As discussed earlier, potential for nonresponse bias

exists in phase II data. The specific impact of

Page 229: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

219

nonresponse is difficult to estimate since the population

of Interest is actually a subset of the population speci­

fication for this dissertation, that of consumers who

have had a recent dissatisfying experience, which by

itself can neither be well defined nor sampled from. The

bias, therefore, would be less severe than other studies

with similar response rates.

This research does not provide a complete test of

the proposed holistic model of CCB. In particular, the

dependent variable of investigation was CCB intentions

and not CCB actions. This may impose limitations on the

validity of the present findings where the interest is

specifically CCB actions.

The data are based on recall of a dissatisfying

experience that the respondent remembers most clearly.

Limits and inaccuracies in the recall process would

affect the quality of data and, thus, impose limitations

on the validity of findings.

These limitations are common to many individual

research efforts and can be overcome only by a stream of

research on the same topic, testing the same model under

varying conditions.

Page 230: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

220

TABLE 8.1

A SUMMARY OF THE VARIOUS HYPOTHESES TESTED IN PHASE I

Proposed Hypothesis Finding

Hi: Alienation and Discontent Partially possess discriminant validity. Supported.

H2: Alienation and Attitudes Supported, are inversely related.

H3: Discontent and Attitudes Supported, are positively related.

Page 231: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

221

TABLE 8. 2

A SUMMARY OF THE VARIOUS HYPOTHESES TESTED IN PHASE II

H4:

H5:

Proposed Hypotheses

Expectancy-Value and CCB Intentions are positively related for: VOICE Intentions W-O-M Intentions FORMAL Intentions

Attitudes and CCB Intentions are positively related for VOICE Intentions W-O-M Intentions FORMAL Intentions

Grocery

+ -1-

1

+ — —

Auto

+ + +

+ +

Medkai

+ + +

+ —

+

Financial

+ -1-

+

+ + +

H6: Under high Prior Experience and moderate/high dissatis­faction. Attitudes are the dominant predktor. + + - -

H7: Under low Prior Experience and moderate/high dissatis­faction, Ebcpectancy Value are the dominant predictor. -I- + — +

H9: Attribution of Blame has a positive indirect effect on CCB intentions. -|- - - +

HlO: Generalized Affect has a positive but indirect effect on CCB intentions for VOICE Intentions + + + + W-O-M Intentions - - + -FORMAL Intentions + + + +

Page 232: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

TABLE 8.2 (Continued)

222

ProfMMd Hypoth«ea Grocery Auto Medkai Financial

Hll: Prior Experience has a positive indirect effect on CCB intentions for: VOICE Intentions. W-O-M Intentions. FORMAL Intentions.

-•-

Hi2: Structural Relationships are similar.

H13: Process Model explains more than the Naive model

HI4: Under positive Attitudes and high E-V, the desirable alternatives are: VOICE W-O-M

+

-I-

-I-

Hi5: Under positive Attitudes and low E^V, the desirable alternatives are: W-O-M + + +

H16: Under negative Attitudes and low E—V, the desirable alternatives are: NO ACTION

Hi7: Under negative Attitudes and high E—V, the desirable alternatives are: VOICE FORMAL

+ +

HIS: Expectancy Value is low when dissatisfaction is controlled for VOICE W-O-M FORMAL +

*tlie symbols are: "+"='supported; "-"asQot supported.

Page 233: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

223

TABLE 8.3

TYPICAL VERBATIM RESPONSES

Industry Typical Responses

1. Grocery "have more trained and efficient cashiers"

"out of date dairy products and packaged meats"

"check for outdated stock more often"

"see that sale items are put into the computer"

"give their sackers lessons on how to fill the grocery sacks"

"include proper cleaning of store as a regular part of an employees duties"

"have adequate supply or not advertise item at bargain prices"

"take bad products off the shelves"

"make sure all their checkers know the specials and have the right price"

"better employee training programs"

2. Auto Repair "salesmen needs to be more honest-when a person asks for steel radials they give you polyester radials for the cost of steel radials"

"the repair part cost $0.35. . they want $300 labor fee"

"stop overcharging and be honest in repairing"

"try to be more conscientious instead of "half fixing" something and hoping the customer would be too tired or busy to bring it back"

Page 234: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

224

TABLE 8.3 (Continued)

Industry Typical Responses

"better train their workmen"

"be fair to women"

"close shop 1"

"the problem was fraud-what could be done?"

"not to be afraid of talking to a dissatisfied customer face the problem"

"get approval before doing any additional work"

"(try) to please them (customers) as they will return if satisfied, otherwise one customer can really hurt business"

3. Medical "recognize that my time is as important as theirs"

"have greater "sincere" concern for the patients problems"

"I was robbed of both my teeth and money"

"reduce cost"

"there is not much that can be done with a greedy person .... except to try to inform others about the bad experiences"

"I think it is real simple--they are just too greedy"

"they need to answer to someone .. most patients are too frightened or feel too poorly to take up for their rights"

"stop trying to fight alligators .. and figure ways to clean the swamp"

Page 235: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

225

TABLE 8.3 (Continued)

Industry Typical Responses

"run the AMA out of the country"

"take time--maybe talk a little more (other than medical talk!)"

"they need to be more caring"

4. Financial "if they would show the slightest bit of concern, I would feel better, but most could care less"

"train employees to give courteous responsible service"

"full banking service on at least one week night on Saturday morning"

"upon closing my account after 30 years plus, no concern or question as to why I moved my account"

"set fire to their computers"

"I know banks do make errors but it sure helps to have .. people who are sincere in wanting to help you. That makes all the difference"

Page 236: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

REFERENCES

Allison, Neil K. (1978), A Psychometric Development of a Test for Consumer Alienation from the Marketplace, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 15, Nov., pp. 565-75.

Anderson, Ralph E. (1973), Consumer Satisfaction: The Effects of Disconfirmed Expectancy on Perceived Product Importance, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 10, Feb. , pp. 38-44.

Andreasen, Alan R. (1977), A Taxonomy of Consumer Satisfaction/-Dissatisfaction Measures, Journal of Consumer Affairs. Vol. 11, No. 2, pp. 11-24.

(1983), Consumer Satisfaction in Loose Monopolies: The Case of Medical Care, Journal of Marketing and Public Policy. Vol. 2, pp. 122-35.

Andrich, David (1978), A Rating Formulation for Ordered Response Categories, Psychometrica. Vol. 43, No. 4, pp. 561-73.

Ash and Quelch (1980), Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior: A Comprehensive Study of Rentals, Public Transportation and Utilities, Refining Concepts and Measures of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Hunt and Day Eds., Indiana Univ.

Bagozzi, Richard P. (1980), Causal Modelling in Marketing. John Wiley and Sons, N. Y.

(1982), A Field Investigation of Causal Relations Among Cognitions, Affect, Intentions and Behavior, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 29, Nov., pp. 562-84.

(1983), A Holistic Methodology for Modelling Consumer Response to Innovation, Operations Research. Vol. 31, No. 1, pp. 128-76.

(1984), A Prospectus for Theory Construction in Marketing, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 48, No. 1, pp. 11-29.

226

Page 237: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

227

(1984), A Prospectus for Theory Construction in Marketing, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 48, No. 1, pp. 11-29.

Barnes, James G. and Kelloway, Karen R. (1980), Consumerists: Complaining Behavior and Attitudes Toward Social and Consu-mer Issues, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings. Jerry Olson Ed., Vol. 7, pp. 329-34.

Bateson, John E. G. (1979), Why We Need Service Marketing, Conceptual and Theoretical Developments in Marketing. Ferrell, Brown and Lamb Eds., AMA.

Bearden, William O. and Crockett, Melissa (1981), Self Monitoring, Norms and Attitudes as Influences on Consumer Complaining, Journal of Business Research. Vol. 9, pp. 255-66.

Bearden, W. 0., Lichtenstein, Donald R. and Teel, Jesse E. (1983), Reassesment of the Dimensionality, Internal Consistency and Validity of the Consumer Alienation Scale, AMA Educators Conference Proceedings, pp. 35-40.

Bearden, W. 0., and Mason, J. B. (1983), Empirical Evidence on Marketplace Alienation, Journal of MacroMarketing. Fall, pp. 6-20.

, and (1984), An Investigation of Influences on Consumer Complaint Reports, ACR Proceedings. Kinnear Ed.

Bearden, W. 0. and Teel, Jesse E. (1983), Selected Determinants of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaint Reports, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 20, Feb. , pp. 21-8.

Bearden, W. O., Teel, J. E. and Crockett, M. (1980), A Path Model of Consumer Complaint Behavior, Marketing in the 80's, 1980 AMA Educators Conference Proceedings. Bagozzi et al. Eds.

Bernhardt, Kenneth L. , Robinson, Larry M. and Semans, Debra (1983), Consumer Complaint Handling: Self Regulation Vs. Government Regulation, AMA Educators Conference Proceedings, pp. 369-73.

Berry, Leonard L. (1980), Service Marketing is Different, Business Week. May-June, pp. 24-29.

Page 238: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

228

Best, Arthur and Andreasen, Alan R. (1977), Consumer Responses to Unsatisfactory Purchases: A Survey of Perceiving Defects, Voicing Complaints and Obtaining Redress, Law and Society Review. Vol. 11, Spring, pp. 701-42.

Biehal, Gabriel J. (1983), Consumer Prior Experiences and Perceptions in Auto Repair Choice, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 47, Summer, pp. 82-91.

Blalock, H. M. (1968), The Measurement Problem, in H. M. Blalock and A. Blalock (Eds.,) Methodology in Social Research, pp. 5-27, McGraw Hill, N. Y.

Brinbaum. A (1968), Test Scores, Sufficient Statistics, and the Information Structures of Tests, in F. M. Lord and M. R. Novick, Statistical Theories of Mental Test Scores. Reading, Addison-Wesley.

Census Information (1983), 1980 Census Information for City and County, Planning Department. City of Lubbock. Aug., 1983.

City Directory (1985), Lubbock City Directory. R. L. Polk and Company, Chicago, 111.

Clabaugh, Jr., Maurice G., Mason, J. B. and Bearden, W. 0. (1979), Consumer Alienation and Causal Attribution as Moderators of Consumer Satisfaction-/Dissatisfaction and Compalint Behavior, New Dimensions of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Behavior, Day and Hunt Eds., Vol. 3, Indiana University, pp. 2-9.

Cliff, Norman (1979), Test Theory Without True Scores?, Psychometrica. Vol. 44, No. 4, pp. 373-93.

Cudek, R., McCormick, D. J., and Cliff, N. (1979), Monte Carlo Evaluation of Implied Orders as a Basis of Tailored Testing, Applied Psychological Measurement. Vol. 3, pp. 65-74.

Day, Ralph L. (1980), Research Perspectives on Consumer Complaining Behavior, Theoretical Developments in Marketing, AMA Educators Conference Proceedings. Lamb and Dunne Eds., pp. 211-215.

(1984), Modelling Choices Among Alternate Responses to Dissatisfaction, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings, Vol. 11, Kinnear Ed., pp. 496-9.

Page 239: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

229

Day, Ralph L., and Ash, Stephen B. (1979), Consumer Responses to Dissatisfaction with Durable Products, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings. Vol. 6, Ed. Wilkie, pp. 438-44.

Day, R. L., and Bodur, M. (1977), A Comprehensive Study of Satisfaction with Consumer Services, Consumer Satisfaction. Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Day Ed., pp. 64-70.

Day, R. L. , Grabricke, K., Schaetzle, T. , and Staubach, F. (1981), The Hidden Agenda of Consumer Complaining, Journal of Retailing. Vol. 57, Fall, No. 3, pp. 86-106.

Day, R. L. and Landon, Laird E., Jr. (1977), Towards a Theory of Consumer Complaining Behavior, Consumer and Industrial Buying Behavior. Arch G. Woodside, Jagdish N. Sheth, and Peter D. Bennett Eds., North-Holland, NY, pp. 425-437.

Drasgow, F. and Parsons, C. K. (1983), Application of Uni-dimensional Item Response Theory Models to Multidimensional Data, Applied Psychological Measurement. Vol. 7, No. 2, Spring, pp. 189-99.

Engel and Blackwell (1982), Consumer Behavior. 4th edition, Dryden Press.

Engel, J. F., Koilat, D. T. and Blackwell, R. D. (1968), Consumer Behavior. 2nd Ed., Prentice Hall.

Enis, Ben M. and Peering, Kenneth (1981), Services Marketing: Different Products, Similar Strategies, Marketing of Services, Donnelly and George Eds., AMA

Festinger, L. (1957), A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford University Press, Stanford.

Fishbein, Martin and Ajzen, Icek (1975), Belief Attitude Intentions and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Addison Wesley.

Foikes, Valerie S. (1984), Consumer Reactions to Product Failure: An Attributional Approach, Journal of Consumer Research. Vol. 10, March, pp. 398-409.

Fornell, Claes and Didow, Nicholas M. (1980), Economic Constraints on Consumer Complaining Behavior, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings, Vol. 7, Olson Ed., pp. 318-23.

Page 240: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

230

Fornell, Claes and Larcker, D. V. (1981), Evaluating Structural Equations Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol.18, Feb., 1981, pp. 39-50.

Fornell, C. and Robinson, W. (1983), Industrial Organiza­tion and Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatisfaction, Journal of Consumer Research. March, Vol. 9, pp. 403-12.

Fornell, C. and Westbrook, Robert A. (1979), An Exploratory Study of Assertiveness, Aggressiveness and Consumer Complaining Behavior, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings. Vol. 6, Wilkie Ed., pp. 105-110.

Gilly, Mary C. and Gelb, Betsy (1982), Post-Purchase Consumer Process and the Complaining Consumer, Journal of Consumer Research. Vol. 9, Dec., pp. 323-8.

Gronhaug, K. and Zaltman, G. (1981), Complainers and Noncomplainers Revisited: Another Look at the Data, ACR Proceedings. Vol. 8, Monroe Ed.

Guttman, L. (1950), Chapters 2, 3, 6, 8, and 9, in S. A. Stouffer et al. (Eds.,), Measurement and Prediction. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N. J.

Hambelton, R. K. and Van der Linden, W. J. (1982), Advances in Item Response Theory and Applications: An Introduction, Applied Psychological Measurement. Vol. 6, No. 4, Fall, pp. 373-8.

Helson, H. (1964), Adaptation Level Theory: An Experimental and Systematic Approach to Behavior. Harper and Row, N. Y.

(1959), Adaptation Level Theory, in S. Koch (Ed. ), Psychology: A Study of Science. Vol. 1, McGraw Hill, N. Y.

Hirschman, Albert O. (1970), Responses to Decline in Firms. Organizations and States. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.,

Howard, John A. and Sheth, Jagdish (1969), The Theory of Buyer Behavior. New York, Wiley.

Hulin, Charles, Drasgow, F. and Parsons, C. (1983), Item Response Theory; Applications to Psychological Measurement. Dow-Jones-Irwin.

Page 241: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

231

Hulin, C., Lissak, R. I. and Drasgow, F. (1982), Recovery of Two- and Three-Parameter Logistic Item Characteristic Curves: A Monte Carlo Study, Applied Psychological Measurement. Vol. 6, No. 3, Summer, pp. 249-60.

Hunt, Shelby (1983), Marketing Theory. Irwin Publications.

Johnson, Richard A. and Wichern, Dean W. (1982), Applied Multivariate Statistical Analysis. Prentice Hall, Engelwood Cliffs, N.J.

Jacoby, Jacob and Jaccard, James J. (1981), The Sources, Meaning and Validity of Consumer Complaining Behavior: A Psychological Review, Journal of Retailing. Vol. 57. No. 3, Fall, pp. 4-24.

Joreskog, Karl and Sorborm, Dag (1981), LISREL VI: Analysis of Linear Structural Relationship by Maximum Likelihood and Least Sguares Method. lES, Chicago.

Kangun, Norman, Cox, Keith K., Higginbotham, J. and Burton, J. (1975), Consumerism and Marketing Management, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 39, April, pp. 3-10.

Kendall, C. L. and Russ, F. A. (1975), Warranty and Complaint Policies: An Opportunity for Marketing Management, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 39, April, pp. 36-43.

Krishnan, S and Valle, Valerie A. (1979), Dissatisfaction Attributions and Consumer Complaining Behavior, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings, Wilkie Ed. , Vol. 6, pp. 445-9.

Lambert, Zarrel V. (1980), Consumer Alienation, General Dissatisfaction and Consumerism Issues, Journal of Retailing. Vol. 56, No. 2, Summer, pp. 3-24.

Landon, Jr., E. Laird (1980), The Direction of Consumer Complaint Research, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings, Vol. 7, Olson Ed., pp. 335-8.

(1977), A Model of Consumer Complaint Behavior, Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Ralph Day Ed., pp. 31-5, Indiana University Press.

Page 242: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

232

Langmeyer, Mellnda and Langmeyer, Daniel (1979), A Behavioral Assesment of Complaining and Its Implications for Further Research, New Dimensions of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Day and Hunt Eds., Vol. 3, Indiana Univ., pp. 26-9.

Latour, Stephen and Peat, Nancy (1979), Conceptual and Methodological Issues in Satisfaction Research, ACR Proceedinqs. Vol. 6, Wilkie Ed.

Lawley, D. N. (1944) The Factorial Analysis of Multiple Item Tests, Royal Society of Edinburgh Proceedings. Vol. 62, pp. 74-82.

(1943) On Problems Connected With Item Selection and Test Construction, Royal Society of Edinburgh Proceedings. Vol. 61, pp. 273-87.

Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1950), Chapters 10 and 11 in Measurement and Prediction. S. A. Stouffer et al. Eds., Princeton University Press, N. J.

Linn, R.1., Levine, M, V., Hastings, C. N. and Wardrop, J. L. (1981), Item Bias in a Test of Reading Comprehension, Applied Psychological Measurement. Vol. 5, pp. 159-73.

Lord, Fredrick (1980), Applications of Item Response Theory to Practical Testing Problems. Lawrence Erlbaum, N. J.

(1952), A Theory of Test Scores. Psychometrica Monograph, No. 7.

Lord, F. M. and Novick, M. R. (1968) Statistical Theories of Mental Test Scores. Reading, Mass., Addison-Wesley Publishing Co. **

Lovelock, Christopher (1983), Classifying Services to Gain Strategic Marketing Insights, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 47, No. 3, pp. 9-20.

Lundstrom, William J. and Lament, Lawrence M. (1976), The Development of a Scale to Measure Consumer Discontent, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 13, Nov., pp. 373-81.

Lundstrom, W. J., Gerald Skelly and Donald Sciglimpagila (1979), How Deep are the Roots of Consumer Discontent: A Study of Rural Consumers, New Dimensions of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Day and Hunt Eds., Indiana Univ.

Page 243: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

233

MacDonald, Roderick P. (1982) Linear Versus Linear Models in Item Response Theory, Applied Psychological Measurementr Vol. 6, No. 4, pp. 379-96.

McGuire, W. J, (1976), Some Internal Psychological Factors Influencing Consumer Choice, Journal of Consumer Research. Vol. 2, No. 4, pp. 302-19.

McGuire, W. J, (1968), Personality And Susceptibility To Social Influence in Handbook Of Personality Theory And Research. E.F. Borgatta and W.W. Lambert Eds., Chicago, Rand McNally, pp. 1130-87.

Miniard, Paul W., Obermiller, Carl and Page, Thomas J. Jr., (1983), A Further Assesment of Measurement Influences on the Intention-Behavior Relationship, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 20, May, pp. 206-12.

Nunally, J. C. (1978), Psychometric Theory. McGraw Hill, N. Y.

Oliver, Richard L. (1980), A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents and Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 17, Nov., pp. 460-9.

(1980a), Theoretical Bases of Consumer Satisfaction Research: Review, Critique and Future Directions in Theoretical Developments in Marketing, AMA Educators Conference Proceedings. Lamb and Dunne Eds. , pp. 206-10, AMA, Chicago.

(1977), Effect of Expectation and Disconfirmation on Post-exposure Product Evaluations: An Alternative Interpretation, Journal of Applied Psychology. Vol. 62, Aug., pp. 480-86.

Olshavsky, Richard W. and Miller, John (1972), Consumer Expectations, Product Performance, and Perceived Product Quality, Journal Of Marketing Research. Vol. 9, pp. 19-21.

Olson Jerry C. and Dover, Philip (1976), Effects Of Expectation Creation And Disconfirmation On Belief Elements Of Cognitive Structure, 1975 Annual ACR Conference Proceedings. Beverlee B. Anderson Ed. , pp. 168-175.

Ott, Lyman (1977), An Introduction to Statistical Methods and Data Analysis. Duxbury Press, Mass.

Page 244: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

234

Petty, Richard E and Cacioppo, John T. (1981), Attitudes And Persuation: Classical And Contemporary Approachegr Wm. C. Brown Company Publishers.

Rasch, G. (1960), Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests. Danish Institute for Educational Research, Copenhagen.

Richins, Marsha L. (1979), Consumer Complaining Processes: A Comprehensive Model, New Dimensions of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Day and Hunt Eds., Vol. 3, Indiana Univ., pp. 30-4.

(1980), Consumer Perceptions Of Costs And Benefits Associated With Complaining In Refining Concepts And Measures Of Consumer Satisfaction And Complaining. Hunt and Day Eds., pp. 105-8, Indiana University.

(1982), An Investigation of Consumer's Attitudes Toward Complaining, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings. Vol. 9, Mitchell Ed. pp. 502-6.

t

(1983), An Analysis of Consumer Interaction Styles in the Marketplace, Journal of Consumer Research. Vol. 10, June, pp. 73-82.

Ritchie, J. R., Brent, M. and Claxton, John (1979), In Search of Actions to Reduce Consumer Shopping Problems, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings, Vol. 6, Wilkie Ed., pp. 466-71.

Robinson, Larry M. (1979), Consumer Complaint Behavior: A Review With Implications for Further Research, New Dimensions of Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Behavior. Vol. 3, Day and Hunt Eds., pp. 41-50, Indiana University.

Russo, Edward J. (1979), Consumer Satisfaction-/Dissatisfaction: An Outsiders View, Association of Consumer Research Proceedings. Vol. 6, Wilkie Ed. , pp. 453-55.

Samejima, F. (1977) Weakly Parallel Tests In Latent Trait Theory With Some Criticisms Of Classical Test Theory. Psychometrica, Vol. 42, No. 2, pp. 193-199.

Samejima, F. (1972) A General Model For Free-Response Data. Psychometric Monograph No. 18.

Page 245: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

235

Sherif, M. and Hovland, C. I. (1961), Social Judgement: Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Communication and Attitude Change. Yale University Press, New Haven.

Shostack, G Lynn (1977), Breaking Free from Product Marketing, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 41, April, 73-80.

Swaminathan, H. and Gifford, J. A. (1979), Estimation of Parameters in the Three Parameter Latent-Trait Model. Laboratory of Psychometric and Evaluation Research, Report No. 90, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Mass.

Swan, J. E. (1977), Consumer Satisfaction With a Retail Store Related To The Fulfillment of Expectations on an Initial Shopping Trip, in Consumer Satisfaction Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior Proceedings, Day Ed., Indiana University.

Thibaut, J. W. and Kelly, H. H. (1959), The Social Psychology of Groups. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Thrustone, L. L. (1928), Attitudes Can Be Measured, American Journal of Sociology. Vol. 33, pp. 529-54.

Torgerson, W.S. (1958) Theory And Methods Of Scaling, New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Tucker, L. R. (1946) Maximum Validity Of A Test With Equivalent Items, Psychometrica. Vol. 11, pp. 1-13.

Valle, V. A. and Waliendorf, M. (1977), Consumer's Attributions of the Cause of Their Product Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction, in Consumer Satisfaction. Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior Proceedings, Day Ed. , Indiana Univ.

Villareal-Camacho, Angelina (1983), Consumer Complaining Behavior: A Cross-Cultural Comparison, AMA Educators Proceedings, pp. 68-73, AMA Chicago.

Warland, Rex H., Herrmann, Robert 0. and Willits, Jane (1975), Dissatisfied Consumers: Who Gets Upset and Who Takes Action, Journal of Consumer Affairs. Winter, pp. 198-163.

Weiss, D. J. and Davison, M. L. (1981), Test Theory and Methods, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 32, pp. 629-58.

Page 246: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

236

Westbrook, Robert A. (1980), Intra-Personal Affective Influences on Consumer Satisfaction with Products, Journal of Consumer Research. Vol. 7, June, pp. 49-54.

(1980a), A Rating Scale for Measuring Product/Service Satisfaction, Journal of Marketing. Vol. 44, pp. 68-72.

(1983), Consumer Satisfaction and the Phenomenology of Emotions During Automobile Ownership Experiences, in International Fare in Consumer Satisfaction and Complaining Proceedings. Day and Hunt Eds, Indiana Univ.

Westbrook, Robert A. and Joseph W. Newman (1978), An Analysis of Shopping Dissatisfaction for Major Household Appliances, Journal of Marketing Research. Vol. 15, Aug., pp. 456-66.

Wingersky, M. S., Barton, M. A. and Lord, F. M. (1982), LOGIST User Guide. ETS, Feb., 1982.

Wood, R. L. and Lord, F. M. (1976), A Users Guide to LOGIST, Research Memorandum 76-4. Educational Testing Service. Princeton, N. J.

Woodruff, Robert B., Cadotte, Ernest and Jenkins, Roger (1983), Modelling Consumer Satisfaction Processes Using Experience Based Norms, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 20, Aug., pp. 296-304.

Woodside, A. G. (1972), Positive Disconfirmation of Expectations and Effect of Effort on Evaluation, 80th Annual APA Convention Proceedings. APA, pp. 743-744.

Wright, B. (1980), Foreward in Probabilistic Models for Some Intelligence and Attainment Tests. University of Chicago Press.

Wright, B. and Mead, R. J. (1977), BICAL: Calibrating Items and Scales with the Rasch Model. Research Memorandum No. 23, Univeristy of Chicago press.

Zeller, Richard A. and Carmines, Edward G. (1980), Measurement in The Social Sciences, Cambridge University Press.

Page 247: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

APPENDICES

A. PHASE I QUESTIONNAIRE B. A TEXT OF THE INTRODUCTION USED BY THE INTERVIEWERS C. PHASE II CENSUS TRACTS D. PHASE II QUESTIONNAIRES E. SELECTED DISCONTENT AND ALIENATION ITEMS

237

Page 248: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

238

APPENDIX A: PHASE I QUESTIONNAIRE

Con^umer Sat 's^iCtipn Sijrve/

This questionnai ro is designed to determine consumers' sa t i s fac t ion , bel iefs and at:it j<les " " i ' - : : ; ^jsi '^'ss. - ^ * ) ' adver t is ing , e t c . There are no r ight or wrong answers to the questions f^at *^orow. Howpvar, /^ur :.>^sor4' : 3 ' - - : important. Therefore, please answer aj_l questions as best as you can.

" . t ) o r ' . la' Please express the extent to which you agree or disagree with each of the foPowng stjtemeitj. C--: • best represents your opinion.

The questionnaire is rather long and has six sections. Some of the questions -wy appear sii-'ir ").' jctja'. "^ic question is different and designed to measure a unique aspect of consumer satisfaction.

1.

2.

3.

4,

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

Section I

5trongly Dlsaqroo

Most Companies are responsive to the demands of the consumer I

The business community has been a large influence in raising our country's standard of living i

Business profits are too high

It seems wasteful for so many companies to produce the same basic products.

Styles change so rapidly a person can't afford to keep up

People who sell things over the telephone -ire always trying to gip you

Unethical practices are widespread throughout business

Advertising is a good source of information

Credit manes things too easy to buy •

Stores do not care why people buy their products just as long as they make a profit

Many times I need assistance in a store and I'm just not able to get

Warranties would not be necessary if the manufacturer made the produc" right in the first place

Shopping is usually a pleasant experience.

Salesmen really take an interest in the consumer and make sure he finds what he wants

Products that last a long time are a thing of the past

People are unable to help determine what products will be sold in the store.

Business takes a real interest in the environment and is trying to improve it

Food which is not nutritious Is another example of business trying to make a buck and not caring about the consumer

Advertising and promotional costs unnecessarily raise the price consumer has to pay for a product

People rate other people by the value of their possessions

Business firms usually stand behind their products i guarantees

What a product claims to do and what it actually does are two different things

When a product is advertised as "new" or "improved" it is the same old thing only in a different package

Industry has an obligation to clean up the waste they have been dumping but they aren't doing it

23.

24.

25. Mass production has done away with unique products

2

2

2

2

2

2

7

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

•^

•>

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

•3

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

Page 249: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

239

Section I I

Chain stores ire getting so big that they --oally don't treat the customer personally

,*'0'-

Disac 5 y f OO

2,

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

23,

24.

25.

Permanent price controls are the only way to end inflation

Misrepresentation of product features is just something we have to live with.

The quality of goods has consistently improved over the years

Many times the salesman says one thing to the shopper but he knows it's just the opposite

Harmful characteristics of a product are often kept from the consumer.

Many times it's easier to buy a new product rather than trying to fix the old one

The only person who cares about the consumer Is the consumer himself.

It is embarrassing to bring a purchase back to the store

The actual product I buy is usually the same as advertised

It is hard to make a buying decision because of all the products to choose from

I tend to spend more than I should just to Impress my friends with how much I have

The small businessman has to do what big business says or else!,

Most companies have a complaint department which backs up their products and handles consumer problems

Even with so much advertising it is difficult to know what brand is the best

Business is the one using up our natural resources (oil, gas, trees etc ) but it does nothing to replace what has been taken.

Many companies listen to consumer complaints but they don't do anything about them

A sale is not really a bargain but a way to draw people into the store....

Generaly speaking, products work as good as they look

Products fall apart before they have had much use

It is difficult to identify with business practices today

Products are only as safe as required by goverment standards, but no more.

Stores advertise "special deals" just to get the shopper into the store to buy something else

It is difficult to Identify with current trends 4 fads in fashion.

Companies are helping minorities and under privileged by providing them with jobs

4 q r a a

5 6

5 "

5 6

5 6

Section 111

1. The information on most packages 1$ enough to make a good decision.

2. I often feel guilty for buying so many unnecessary products

3. Most salesmen who call at home try to force the consumer Into buying something

Strongly Disagree

Sfongly Agree

Page 250: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

240

:)is: •o-g ,/

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

23.

24,

25.

All business really wants to do is to maxe the most icrey it can

Most brands are the same -ith just different names and labels

The business community is actively involved in solving social jroolems

Most people know that advertising lies a "little."

A product will usually break down as soon as the warranty is up

Companies encourage the consumer to buy more than he really needs

The goverment should enforce ethical business practices

Business is responsible for unnecessarily depleting our natural resourc»s.

The consumer knows exactly what he is buying with food products

because tne ingredients are on the package

Companies aren't willing to listen or do anything about consumer gripes..

Companies try to influence goverment just to better themselves

Recycling o* products is one way business is cleaning up the environment.

Business does not help local residents because it's not profitable

One must be willing to tolerate poor service from most stores

When the consumer is unsure of how good a product is, he can get the correct information from a salesman

The consumer is usually the least important consideration to most compan'->s.

It is difficult to know what store has the best buy

Salesmen are "pushy" just so they can make a sale

If all advertising stopped, the consumer would be better off

Business's orime objective is to make money rather than satisfy the consumer

Sales clerks in stores just don't care about the consumer anymore.

Most products are safe when they are used right

2

2

?

2

2

2

2

Z

2

2

7

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

Section IV

1. ; often feel •'rustrated when I fall to find what I want in the store.

2. Advertised "specials" aren't usually in the store when the shopper goes there

3. Service departments "pad" the bill by charging for unneeded work,

4. After making a purchase, I often find nyself wondering "why." ...

5. The price I pay Is about the same as the quality I receive

6. Companies try to take a personal interest in each consumer rather than treating him as a number

7. It is hard to understand why some brands are twice as expensive as others.

8. As soon as they make the sale, most businesses forget about the buyer

9. Comiercials make a person unhappy with himself because he can't have everything he sees

Strongly Oisi gree

10. It is not unusual to find out that business has lied to public.

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

Strongly Agree

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

6

Page 251: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

241

11.

12.

13.

14.

L5.

16.

17.

18.

19,

20.

21.

22.

23.

24.

25.

26.

27,

28.

29.

30.

Health and safety wamifigs on packages are not adequate enough to inform the consumer of possible danger ,

Service manuals aren't provided for products because the company wants to make money servicing products as well as selling them...

Buying beyond one's means is justifiable through the use of :redit.

What is seen on the outside of a package is many times not what you get on the inside

There are too many of the same types of products which is a waste of money..

It is often dif f icult to understand the real meaning of most advertisements.

In general, companies are honest in their dealings with the consumer

Prices of products are going up faster than the incomes of ordi nary consumer

Products are designed to wear out long before they should

Advertising tempts people to spend their money foolishly

Most claims of product quality are true

Business profits are high yet they keep on raising their prices.

I am often dissatisfied with a recent purchase

Companies generally offer what the consumer wants

The wide variety of competing products makes intelligent buying decisions more di f f icul t

Business has commercialized many meaningful holidays, such as Christmas.

Advertisements usually present a true picture of the product

The main reason a company does things for the society is to make more sales

I often don't like to return or exchange products I am dissatisfied wi*:-.

An attractive package many times influences a purchase that isn't necessary

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

•>

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

6

0

S

6

6

6

6

6

S

6

Section V

1. Store employees are often quite unpleasant to customers who return unsatisfactory products

2. A large variety of products allow the consumer to choose the one that he really wants

3. Making a complaint about a defective product usually leads to frustration.

4. Self-service stores leave the consumer at the mercy of how the products looks

5. I envy people who take the effort and courage to complain about unsatisfactory products

6. Conpanles "jazz up" a product with no real improvement, just to get a higher price or sell more

7. Complaining to business is usually done by people with l i t t l e else to do.

8. Most of the things I buy are over-priced

9. It bothers me quite as bit i f I don't complain about an unsatisfactory product

Strongly D'sagree

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

Strongly Agree

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

Page 252: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

242

s

10. Prices are reasonable given the h'gh cost of doing business

11. People have a responsibility to tell stores when a product 'hey purchase 1s defective

12. Promotional or "junk" mail is just a waste

13. It sometimes feels good to get ny dissatisfaction and frustration

with the product off my chest by complaining

14. Repairs take too long because the right part is often not in stock

15. People are bound to end up with unsatisfactory products once in a while, so they should not complain

16. Advertising tells the shopper about things he would not ordinarily hear about

17. Complaining isn't much fun. but it's got to be done to keep business from becoming irresponsible ,

18. A warranty or guarantee may be a good one but the service

department is often unable to do the work correctly

19. Making a complaint about a defective product usually takes a lot of time ,

20. Repair work is usually done right the first time ,

21. I often complain when I an dissatisfied with business or products because I feel it Is my duty to do so ,

22. Business takes advantage of poor people or minorities by charging higher than normal prices

23. By making complaints about unsatisfactory products, in the long

run the quality of products will improve ,

24. The stock market is controlled by big financial institutions

25. "iost stores are willing to adjust reasonable complaints ,

26. Consumer activists, like Ralph Nader, do more harm than good to busines

27. I feel a sense of accomplishment when I manage to get a

complaint to a store taken care of

29. It is difficult to identify with business practices today

29. By complaining about defective products. I may prevent other consumers from experiencing the same problem ,

30. Many business say they want their customers satisfied but they ar» not willing to stand behind their word

31. I don't like people who make complaints to stores, because usually their complaints are unreasonable

3

Sectlon VI

IN THE NEXT SECTION WOULD YOU PLEASE GIVE US SOME BACKGROUND INFORMATION?

1. Are you: ^Male ^Female

2. Please check the category that represents your age.

15 to 20 years 36 to 40 years "21 to 25 years 41 to 45 years 26 to 30 years 46 to 50 years ~Ti tn -x^ w«»r« 51 to 55 years

56 to 60 years "61 years or more

_ 26 to 30 years _^31 to 35 years

Page 253: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

243

3. Are you: ^Married ^Divorced Separated Wiiowed Sing--

4. How many persons, including yourself, presently live in your household? ^ _ _ _ _

5. What is your specific occupation? [job title]

6. What is the last year of formal education you completed? (Circle one number)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1 2 3 4 5 6 or (ipre HTgR School Trade Sch./College Graduate Scn'oo'

7. Please check the category that represents your total household Income ( jo in t i f married) "i :^83'

less than $10,000 $50,001 to $60,000 S100.00'. to $:20.000 "$10,001 to $20,000 $60,001 to $70,000 S'.JO.OO'. to SUC.QOO "$20,001 to $30,000 $70,001 to $80,000 $140,001 to $160,000 $30,001 to $40,000 $80,001 to $90,000 $160,001 to $180,000 $40,001 to $50,000 $90,000 to $100,000 $130,001 o^ ^

8. With which ethnic or racia l group do you ident i fy yourself? [Mark one only]

White ^Black ^Hispanic ^Ot"e' ioec i 'y )

THANK YOU FOR YOUR COOPERATION ( I f you desire further information on this survey, please attach your f u l l address)

Page 254: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

244

APPENDIX B: A TEXT OF THE INTRODUCTION USED

BY THE INTERVIEWERS

Good Morning (Evening, Afternoon):

My name is

I am a student at the Texas Tech University and am

helping with a survey being conducted by the marketing

department.

Many people have problems or dissatisfaction with

products or services at department stores, etc. In this

study we are interested in finding out about your

feelings on such problems.

We would like your help in completing the survey

which will take only a short time and which can be

completed at your convenience. Your responses will be

kept confidential and will be used only to help us do our

survey. This is a university survey and does not involve

any businesses. Therefore, no salesman will ever call

you based on the information you provide.

I would like to return in one week and pick up the

survey between 6 p.m. and 8 p.m. If you find that you'll

be out at this time, please leave the survey in the

mailbox.

Thank you for your assistance.

Page 255: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

APPENDIX C: PHASE I I CENSUS TRACTS

245

SOURCE; US. CENSUS - 1980

J C » > t I • 5 ' " ^ t l

Page 256: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

246

\ r..^^v. I > [ ^

SOURCE: US CENSUS - 1960

Page 257: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

247

( V 1<-'*^

1 .566

SOURCE; US. CENSUS - 1960

Page 258: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

248

SO( ICE;U.S CENSUS - 1980

» L C >' • i ' M l L l t

~>7

Page 259: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

2 4 9

APPENDIX D: PHASE I I QUESTIONNAIRES

This qu«»t1onniir» is designed to iJ«ten»in» th« s p e c ' ' c conoUmtj '"lit -ran^jMr?'. ..c"^ IS <-j -i»« t a ' C f SttOQpInq ) t t grjcgry store. There trt no right or xroflg insii«rs. ^'omti'r your : • ' • . : - ! ' :D "':"S i'» •"oar-? 1 e « t re«d the instruction* c j re fu l ly «nd 4ns«*r i l l questions.

Stctton I F i rs t I ' l l 90lnq to Kk you 50»e gefltril qu«$t1ont ibout /our :o<n*ons ' . i i r r - q ous -'s-. 's " •<• I oould Uko you to re»d eKh t t t t ea tn t «t i t wpetrs . rh«n mdlci'.e ".ni! • i t * " : :* / c - i : - - - ^ * - ' by c i rc l ing tha nupbc^ tb«t best d«jcrib«» your reKtIof l to the stitcMent. > « ei'.*'?:'-'»s r »

(6) Stronqly Aqree («) Mree Sowewhit 2; : S K " » (5) Aqreo (3) OiJiqree 5aiie«h*t 1 i ' s i ; - ' ^ '>:'-.^:

Mean 4 .51 2.56

4.55

4.30 2.38

4.17

5.35

2.94 4.67

3.95

3.26 4.12

4.85

1.74 4.28

4 .60

2.85 4.36

3.94

2.76 3 .52

4.59

1.71 4 .36

2.18

2.47

( s td) 1.16) 1.29)

1.32)

1.28) 1.12)

1.30)

0 .80)

1.23) 1.09)

1.33)

1.25) 1.29)

1.13)

1.08) 1.14)

1.13)

1.37) 1.46)

l^j.

1. CoMpU'iing to business <s usually done by oeoo'e vir.r\ l i c e

2. Most coi"oanies c*re nothing at all about the consjner

1 0 . .

3. Industry has an obligation to clean jo tne «aste they ia»* been l-^o they aren't doing it

«. I t bothers me quite a bit if I do not comolain about in ^nsat'S'act :)ry j ' - ja^ct. i

5. The business community has been a Urge '"fluence in r j i smq :ur : ; j r ' t ' y ' ; standard of l iv ing *

6. Chain stores ire getting so big that they don't treat the consjr^er j e ' s o n i H / - - 5

7. People have a resoonstbiHty to te l l stores inen a product t^ey j ' . rcase is defective *

8. Shopping is usually an unpleasant e«perience

9. Stores advertise "special deals" Just to get the shoooer nto t"e store to buy something else

10. I t sometimes 'eels good to get my dissatisfaction and ' rus t ra fon «ith f e product off my chest by complaining

U . People ire unable to detennine «hat products wi l l be sold n tie store.

12. All business real ly xants to do is to maHe the most money i t can

13. People are bound to end up . i t h unsatisfactory products once "i i . h i i e , so they should not complain

14. Misrepresentation of product features is just something «• - ive to I've « > t i . . . i

15. C(W)*ii«s encourage the consumer to buy more than he ' - 1 / needs 6

16. Canplaining isn' t much fun. but i t ' s got to be done to .??o Businesses 'ram becoming irresponsible

17. The small businessman has to do <<hat big business says ;-• sUe! i

13. I t is hard to understand why some brands are twice as e.pensive is K i e r s a

1 . 3 9 ) 19. I often complain when I n dissatisf ied with business or products because : 'ee' i t is my duty to do so

1 . 2 9 ) 20. The consumer is usually the least important consideration to most companies.... 6

1 ' 3 9 ) 21 . As soon as they make a sale, nost businesses forget about the buyer 6

1 . 0 0 ) 22. By making conplilnts about unsatisfactory products, in tne 'ong run -ne

qual i ty of products wi l l improve

1 . 1 2 ) 23. One must b« wi l l ing to tolerate poor servce 'rom most stores 6

1 . 3 6 ) 2A. P r icM of products are going up faster than the incomes of o rd ' i j ry co"s.«ie' . . . S

0 . 6 6 ) 25. Most Stores *re w i l l ing to adjust reasonable complaints *

0 . 8 5 ) 2». C9»p«nies general ' / of 'er what tne consigner wants *

5

5

5

5

5

5

3

5

S

4

a

)

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

4

•?1 S - ) - » s .

Page 260: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

250

Mean ( s t d ) ;•''

• ( 1 - 1 ' ^ ) 27. 3us.ness p r o f t ; are - , , « , e , »ney .eep on r j .s ing -n j . r p r : , , ,

4 . 6 5 ( 1 . 1 1 ) 2 8 . I feel i sense of accompl ->nn,eot * . n ; , , „ , , , to ^et a comp. n-. -o , ;• - . taken care o * . , , , , , ,

i

2 . 1 8 ( 1 . 0 5 ) 2 9 . In general companies are pU.n dishonest 'n tne'r l e j i n s , , ; , . . . c.^s^r.' . •,

'' 30. 4n attract ive package sometimes in'ljences i ourcmse ••^jt isn t - f c e s i ) ' / o 4 . 8 1 ( 0 . 9 1 ) 31. By complaining about defective products. I nay or. ,go: otner - - , ^ » - , ' rv -

eipenencing the same problem ^

2 . 8 1 ( 1 . 1 1 ) 3 2 . I w often dissatisf ied with a recent purchase ^

4 . 3 1 ( 0 . 9 7 ) „ , ^ . ,. JJ. Lomganies jazz up" a product with no real norovement, i.st -^ a»r , - .n.r

price or sell more . . , . . . . ' . . . ' . . . . . . i

•' 34. I don't l ike people who complam to stores, because ..%^i^',y t"*"- c jnp i i ' - t s i re unreasonable ' . . . . . 6

Z . i o ^ i . U J ; 35_ 4 ij^^g variety of o'cducts allows consumers to choose 'ne -"• 'ne/ ' > i ' ' / »*nt 6

4 . l y ( 1 . Z U ; 36. Companies try to influence government just to better tnemse'«»s i

2 . 7 0 ( 0 . 8 5 ) 37. Business firms stand behind tnoir products and guaranties i

Section I I In this section, I vould l ike to ask you about your experience in handling problems md co»»' i ' - t5 wntie shooo"« at a grocery store.

1 . Have you, in tne last six ( 6 ) , months contacted any store/manufKturer regaro'ng j n , snoso''; orob'«n. -,-;•< as overcharge, bad product, refund, replacement, etc.?

Yes 10

I f yes, how often? about I or 2 times

about 3 to 6 times about 7 to 12 t nes

•nee tnan 12 t-mes

3 .12 (1 .40 )

2. How often do you talk to your friends and relatives regarding problems and complai'^ts / C J ni»» . n ' « siooo'-'g for grocery products (c i rc le appropriate number)?

6 5 1 3 ? ; Often le.er

3. -lave you in the last six (6) months contacted a consumer :r OJOI IC igenc/, sucn is t-e 3 e : t i ' 3ui"ess ^ji-eau regarding any complaint?

'es '*Q

I f yes, roughly how many times m the last six months? times aoprox'njte' /

4. -lave /ou ever taken formal action against a store or •na-^-acturer regarding any : ' /o^r :omp'i"ts?

res

I f yes, roughly now many times? 10

t 'mes loprox imate 'y

Section I I I Next, I would l ike for you to think of * recefit prpblea that you reacmber most c'early stth shopping at a grocery store. For exa4>lt . you may have had probl««« with the store i t s e l f , its cNployces or with products/produce purchased at th« store. Keeping in mtnd your particular p r o b l « , please ansaer the following questions.

1 . Please describe br ie f ly your problem or dissatisfaction:

2. NaM and location of tne jrocery store involved:

Page 261: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

231

Meafl ( s t d ) *• " * "•""*' ••* '"»ol»ed, wnat was it: Product (food, o i l . produce, otnen ^ •rand name of tne uea ( i f aodlicao'ei "

t * r m ??^"* ' "^"^^ ' '^* *" " " ""' • ' • " ""•"'ons about the pr,bl«> and f o you 'ee' s respors.b't 'or t L U f l i l i l K* T f? ''••^ • • ' ' ' " ' *•* 'o"<*'"9 «4t€iM«tj and then indicate the extent of /our igre^enc :r j i ; . aqrevent by clrcHno the appropriate nurter. ' ' '

2 . 1 5 ( 0 . 7 8 ) - • . The store policies arc to bl«ie for the above probioi 3 »

Z . J 3 ( 0 . 7 2 ) 5. The itorp personnel are simply careleis ;

1 . 5 8 ( 0 . 8 0 ) 6. The store didn't mean for the problem to xcur, but mistakes haooen 3 :

' . I exptcted too much of the store ;

2 . 0 9 ( 0 . 5 2 ) 8. It Is probably my fault, since I Should be more careful in Shopping : ;

1 . 9 0 ( 0 . 9 1 ) 9. The manufacturer Is responsible for the bad product ( if a product 's '".o'»eoi

10. Overall how dissatisfied were you before you did anything about the prob'jm s^c r. :: oic< 'o t-e -.t--*, etc. ) . Please circle the number that best represents your feeimg.

lOOS 90t 80S 70S 60S SOS 40S 30S 20S lOS tv Completely Oissatisfied lot Ji.sat'sf-ed at «i'

U . Which of the following action (s) did you take after you expenencad tne ioo«e oroe>"' -•:>'•• t"! ' '•» msaer Is ok).

After I experienced the above problem I:

_ ^ ^ _ ^ Forgot about the incident and did nothing Complained to the store on my next trip

Went back or called the store immediately and asked 'nem to take care : ' iy o'p»i«» Decided never to shop again at that store

Told my friends and relatives about my bad experience _^____ Complained to a consumer agency, such as the Better Business 3ur*iu

Took some formal action against the store/manufactjrer Other, please specify

12. ow please tell us how you felt about the whole Incident after you nad taken tne above Kt-on (s). Please circle the number that best represents your feeling.

lOOS 90S SOS 70S 60S SOS 40S 30S 20S lOS OS Completely Sat-sfied Not Satisfied at A''

13. What do you think grocery stores ought to do to solve the type of problem you nad?

Section I Next, imaglna that soMttmc in the future you were shopping at your most frequpntly v't'tP^ grocery store and a* Incidtnt sniilar to the one you Indicated above Kcurtil agam. Hem read each or tli« foiioving statannts aM indicate tht extent to which you thinli they are likely to happen. The categories an:

(6) Very Likely (4) SoMwhat UUely (2) Unlikely (5) Likely (3) SoMwhat Unlikely (1) Viry unlikely

Very Ke'y l.'<eiy .n' «e'y

1. Assume you reported the incident to the store, how likely is it that the store would:

4.16(1.96) / 7 0 / 1 & M * ' 4Pb'09i'* liut ^ nothing 6 S 4 3 2 I < » . / o v i * 0 1 ^ b. take appropriate action to take care of your prob'em (refund, ' t c . ) . . . S S 4 3 2 1 3 . 8 6 ( 1 . 6 4 ) c. solve your problem and give better service to you m the future 6 5 4 3 2 1 _ * _ - ) , *^-% d. be more careful in future and everyone would benefit 6 3 4 3 2 1 3 . 7 J \ 1 . 6 7 )

2. Assi«« that you mentioned the problem to your friends and relatives who shop . at the same store, how likely is it that they would:

A -17/1 A 7 \ a. go on buyin? as usual 6 S 4 3 2 I n.^i^i-.tij b. be more careful when buying froei that grocery store 6 5 4 3 2 1 1 . 8 5 ( 1 . 1 7 ) c. stop buying froa that grocery store altogether 6 $ 1 3 2 1 2 7 0 ( 1 6 0 ) * ' " • ' * '"*' ***'** '""^ orobleei 8 5 4 3 2 1

Page 262: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

252

» 3 4 !

"1

Mean ( s t d )

3. Assui* tnat you '-oort^o tie inco .n t to a consider iqe'c/ s . c is •»« " ' ^ 3 1 6 n 7 S 1 * * " • • • Business Bureau, now ' <e / s s tnit they .OJ :

3 . 1 2 ( 1 7 6 1 *• """ ' ' • ' ' 9 ' ' * /ou j n t i l many o f e ' s nave s m ' a r -jno i " - - . i - : J - > Q / i * t \ b. take no acton '" ^

3 . 2 9 ( 1 . 6 7 ) c. make the store t « e care of yo^r p rob lem! ! ! " ! ! m ] ] j ^ ] [ [ j [ ] ' ' ] j ] . " | . .; 2 . 9 4 ( 1 6 3 V "• ^o ' * * /•""• problem and ensure that tne store '% : i r . ^ / " i n " - - »

^ future 5

how I would l ike to ask you about what you might K t u a l l y do in the case tne above you were shopping at your most frequently visited grxery store.

J . 0 5 ( 1 . 5 3 ) 4. I f a similar problem occured again, how l ike ly is t tnat you .-.. i: '—^ ' 4 . 6 2 ( 1 . 7 5 ) *• 'orget the incident and do nothing .; i • : 2 : -1 n o / 1 7 Q \ ''• ' lefinately complain to the store naniqe' ;n /c^r -»<t " o •; i '• I Z '. J ' U O v I . /y) c. decide not to shop at tnat r^cery store iq j '^ ^ 5 : ; ; 4 . 7 0 ( 1 7 4 ) '^- 9" ' " ' ' ' "• <^*" *''• *tore imeO' i te ly and ask them 'o •Vi*'-i'-' z' 1 an/A •ic\ your problem 5 • . ' • } : : J .oj\L. /O) e. speak to your friends and relatives about your Bad experience 5 5 • ] 2 '. 2 . 2 0 ( 1 3 6 ) ^' ^o"''""^* y>^'' friends and relatives not to snop at tnat stor? i ^ : 3 : ;

" ^ * ' 9- complain to a consumer sgency and ask them to nake tne store ti«e 2 . 1 8 ( 1 . 5 6 ) care of your problem 5 5 4 3 2 . 1 4 7 ^ 0 9 7 1 ''• • ' ' ' ' • * let ter to the local newspaper about your bad ><oerience 5 ' . ' • } ' . .

^ ^ ' i . report to a consumer agency so tnat they cm .ar- ^r^.r cons j ^ o ' s . . . . 6 5 : 3 : 1 1 . 9 6 ( 1 . 4 8 ) J- take some formal action against the store/manufacturer s 5 1 3 2 1

1 . 5 0 ( 1 . 0 3 ) As the last part of this section, I m going to ask you to think about how I k e ' / is t tnat you . . . ' j - n e «ny action (s) I f you were pretty sure about the response you »rt going to get. For example, •'tn, consjue's .c j :d complain only I f they were confident that someone would take care of tneir concer". whi'e tnere are many otners who would coaplain regardless of the response they get. how read each of the foi'owing statements and then 'no*. cate the extent to which you tre l ike ly to take that action.

Jmry

- <e / 5. How likely is it that you would report the incident to the store, if you

were pretty sure that the store would: 2 .26 (1 .50 ) c ^/.(o Jfl\ ' • *Po' ' '9 ' 'e but do nothing 6 J .J'^K'J ' to) (,_ tj|,g j j^g gf ygy^ problem to /our satisfaction 6 5 . 5 6 ( 0 . 7 6 ) c. solve your problem and give you better service in the 'u t j re 6 c 'iTfn Q1N d. be more careful in future and everyone would benefit 6

6. How l ike ly is i t that you would mention the incident to your friends ind T n n / i £.n\ re lat ives i f you were pretty sure that they would: i . y\j( i . oU j 4 . 8 7 ( 1 . 0 9 ) *• 9° ' " ^"y'"9 ** usual 5 - ' - , \ , ' c.e.\ *• ' * ' ' '°' '* ^^ref'i\ when shopping from that grocery st: '? 5 3 . 3 6 ( 1 . 6 6 ) c. stop buying from that grocery store altogether 6 4 U \ ( \ 3 9 1 ''• *^*'' ^"^ solve your problem 6

5 5 5 0

4 4 4 4

3 3 3 3

2 2 2 2

>'.'J - 1 « * ' /

I I 1 I

5 0

5 5

4 4 4 4

3 3 3 3

2 5

) 2

1 1 \ 1

2 .27 (1 .63 )

7. How l ike ly is i t that you would report the incident to i rrnsjner agency, such as the Better Business Bureau, i f you were pretty sure •.•'at tney would:

a. not believe you unti l many others have simi l i r j j n p ' i m t s 6 1 . 7 0 ( 1 . 1 7 ) b. take no action 6 (. an (\ \ \ \ c. make the store take care of your problem 6 ' • . 7 / V . l . i i ^ d. solve your problem and ensure tnat the store is c irefu' in tne

5 . 2 7 ( 0 . 9 3 ) future 5

5 5 0

4 4 4

3 3 3

2 2 2

1 1 ;

Section V In this last section, I would l ike to ask you a few bKkground questions:

1 . At which grocery store do you buy most of your groceries (cheek one)?

F j r r ' s

^ _ _ united Alber tsons

3ive n Gam Ot-er, o'ease n»Be

2. How many times do you v is i t this store in a (1) month?

3. How long have you been shopping at this g rxery store?

t imes

( i n - i ; - t n s )

Page 263: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

4. At wnat other store (s) do you snop for g rxery products (more tnjn ;re s o«i ' Furr's

res Oon't f'-: No

253

j n i t e d AlOertSOnS

Save n 3 l ' n Otn»r. p l e i i e n » »

5. How frequently do you shop at these other stores eacn month? Furr's

United Albertsons

Save 'n j l ' n other, o'ease - «••!

S. Oo any of tht grocery stores you v is i t have computer 'automat'ici'ly 'eads ir-n .-^:<- ,•

"es to

I f yes, which ones?

7. Oo you think computer (automatically 'eads price) check-outs in grocery stor.s nis -ide ."^o: - j 'a:.;'''

3. Oo you think the computer check-out procedure on the whole, is more accurate f i n t ' l o t c n a : ; - » : • - : . method?

More accurate -ess icr . r i - . ho about the yft

9. All things considered, how do you feel about the use of computer cneck-outs m grocery stores?

10. How often do you buy grocery products each month (other than for f i i u i n items)?

times approximately

u.

12.

13.

14.

Are you: Male

Female

Please check the category that represents

15 to 20 years 21 to 25 years

26 to 30 years

Are you: Single

What is your occupation (Job t i t l e ) :

your age:

31 to 35 /ears 36 •• •) years

to 45 years

Married Divorced

16 to 50 years 51 to 55 /ears

56 to 60 /ears Over W yrs

Separated w1dowed

15. What is the last year of formal education that you completed? (check one)

High school or less Trade school College

16. With which ethnic or racial group do you identify yourself? White

Black Hispanic

Other, please specify

Graduate scnool

17. Please check tht category that represents your total household income ( jo in t if marr*ed) m 1984?

lest than {10,000 SIO.OOO to SZO.OOO

"~ $20,001 to J30.000

S30,001 to t50,000 S50.001 to t70,000

~ $70,001 to $90,000

$90,001 to $110,300 Over $110,001

Thank you fpr your cooperation

Page 264: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

254

Cowscer Survey

This questionnaire Is designed to determine tne specif': tomeli'nts tn«t toi^j^rs'. \jcn is -ou n««e to-'*-- -g auto-reoair. There tre no r'gnt or wrong answers, •'owever your pe'sona' o o " :ns t't m o o r ' . f . ' t m --i: the instructions carefully and answer all questions.

Section I first I'm going to ask you some general questions about your opinions regarding Ous"esses •• •"• ."'tei ^'ites. I would like you to read each statement as it appears, ''hen indicate tne eitent ;' OJ-- i-jr.-«•"•. ;- :isiqr.«aent by elrelint the nueper that best describes your reaction to the statement, 'ne tite^o'es i'?

(6) Strongly Agree (5) Agree

' * ) Agree Somewnat (3) Disagree Somewnat

Mean (std) 4.54(1.34)

2.68(1.31)

4.32(1.12) 4.49(1.18)

2.26(1.07)

4.63(1.30) 5.46(0.80)

3.28(1.44) 4.88(1.00)

3.96(1.44)

3.16(1.26)

4.41(1.17) 5.07(1.02)

1.72(1.12)

4.33(1.19)

4.58(1.25)

2.74(1.42)

3.69(1.54)

3.74(1.44)

2.87(1.33)

3.64(1.35)

4.47(1.09)

2.08(1.40) 4.35(1.41)

2.27(0.79)

•2' O'Sl^r^.

ill :isi;-'«

1. Comolaining to business is usually done o/ people with iitt'e e'se to do 5

2. Most companies care nothing at all about tne consumer 5

3. Industry is not cleaning up the waste tney nave oeen dumping 5

4. It bothers me quite a bit if I do not comoliin about an jnsatisfactor/ orodjct. 5

5. The business community has oeen a large influence in raising our country s standard of living i

6. Cham stores are getting so oig that they don't treat tne consjmer oe'so"!''/.. 5

7. Peoole »ive a responsibility to tell stores ineri a product tney ourcnase is defective '

8. Shopping is usually an unpleasant experience 6

6 9. Stores advertize "special deals" just to get the shopper into the store to

buy something else

10. It sometimes feels good to get my dissatisfaction and frustration «itn tne product off my chest Oy comolaining

11. Consumers ire unable to determine what products will Se sold m tne stores 6

12. All business really wants to do is to make the most money it :in 5

13. People ire bound to end up with unsatisfactory products once n a while. so tney should not complain *

6

17. The small Businessman has to do what big Business says :<• else! 6

18. It is hard to understand why some Brands ire twice as expenswe as otners 6

19. I often complain when I'm dissatisfied with Business or products Because t 'eel it is my duty to do SO

20. '*ie consider is usually the least important consioerition to most tomom'es.

21. As soon as they maxe a sale, most businesses forget about tne ouye'

22. By making complaints about unsatisfactory products, m tie long run tne quality of products will improve ... 6

23. .One must be willing to tolerate poor service from most stores 6

24. Prices of products tr* going up faster than the incomes of oramary consuners.. 6

25. Most stores tre willing to adjust reasonable complaints 6

14. Misrepresentation of product features is Just something <e -i/e to I've wuh... S 5

15. Companies encourage the consumer to Buy more than ne/s-^ '?il!/ needs 6 5

16. Complaining isn't much 'un, out t's got to se done to <?eo Businesses '^om Becoming irresponsible 5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

5

• : - • ) ' /

no'fe

Page 265: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

255

Mean (std) 2.66(0.92)^ -

'26. -ompan'es jeneriM/ o"»r «nit the consjuer .^-^s i 3 . 8 3 ( 1 . 4 0 ) 2 7 . Business profits »re nigh yet tne/ (eep on -usi-g -ne- jr-.s i 4.52(1.03),. , , ,

28. I reel a sense of accomplishment when [ manage to get i cono i nt -o t r o " taken care of ' ^

2.28(1.1I)„ ,

29. m general companies tre plain dishonest in tneir oeilmgs .ith tne :o^s--»' .. 4

. 5 / ( 1 . 0 9 ) 3 0 . An attractive package sometimes influences a purchase tnat M n ' oer.ssi-/ 5 4.82(0.97),, .

31. By complaining about defective prpducts. I may or.yoot o'ler --"s -«-s -r-n experiencing the same problem 5

2.82(1.18),, , 3Z. I am often dissatisfied «itn a recent purchase S

. £ . J \ , l . ] m } j 2 . Companies "jajz up" a product with no real mprovement. jjst -o ge' i -jr.r price or sell more i

t* .OJ(. 1 . U y ) 34. I don't like peoole who complain to stores. Because jsually tne <• como'i'its ire unreasonaole 6

2.25(0.96) 35. A large variety of products a i : : «s consumers to choose tne one tne/ ' e i '/

'-±1

want.

4 . 4 0 ( 1 . 2 1 ) 3 6 . Companies try to influence government just to Better themsel/es 5 5 4

2 . 8 4 ( 0 . 9 3 ) ^ ' ' *"*'"•" firms stand Behind their products and guarantees 5 5 4

Section II In this section, I would like to ask you about your experience in handling problems and complaints tf-e'-n^q automobile repair.

1. nave you, in the last six (6), months contacted any repair shop/manufactjrer -egardTng jn, jr^j>,_ ;,;, ,, poor quality work, bad product, etc.?

Yes No

If yes, how often? aoout •. or 2 times

iDout 3 to 6 t"»es about ' to 12 ties

more than 12 t'mes 2. How often do you talk to your friends and relatives regarding oroo'ems and complai-^ts .ou nue :once'"'"g

automobile repair (circle appropriate number)?

3.33(1.28) Often s« »r

3. Have you in the last six (6) months contacted a consume' ;'• ojolic agency, sjcn as tne Better Business B^'eiu regarding any complaint?

'es 10

If yes, roughly how many times in tne last six months? times iporo«'iiate'/

4. Have /ou ever taken legal action against a repair shop or nanuf acturer regardi'g iny of your lompamts'

'es

If yes, roughly now many times? 10

;imes iporo«imateiy

Section U I Next, I would like for you to think of a problea that you remmbtr most clearly concerning your experience with autoaoOilt repair. For exampit, you may have been unhappy with the quality of work done on your car. tne behavior of shop aaployeti or with the repair shop policies. Keeping in mind your particular problem, please answer the following questions.

1. Please describe briefly your problem or dissatisfaction:

Page 266: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

256

M e « n ( s t d ) 2. i<me and location of tne rett^r snop - . o ' . e d :

3. '.' a product was mvol/ed, wnat was t 'roduct Brand '^ute of tne item ti' appliciP'e

At this point I would'like to ask you a few questions aeout tne oroo -m a";! wnom og '•• s -.sj-'s 3'- '-- •• .- ! ! ? _ - ' ! ! • I^" '? '••*'* •*•* '^ "•• 'bllowing statements and then noicate tne extent :' /ou' i:ree~»-t ;r jisl *9'"ee*ent by circling the appropriate ni*oer.

2.28(0.74), rs . - "" " '^" — -». ihe repair shop's policies ire to blime for the above 3roo>n j

^ . 3 3 ( 0 . 8 1 ) 5. The repair shop personnel are simply careless !

1.88(0.78) 6. The repair shoo didn't mean for tne problem to occjr, out mstj.es "apoen 1 .'

. U J V U . 5 1 ; 7 I expected too much of the repair shop 3 ; ;

2 . 0 6 ( 0 . 4 9 ) 8. It is proBaOly my *ault, since I Should be more careful 'n -,J ---q •— ' . p j -shop 3 , .

2 . 0 1 ( 0 . 8 8 ) 9. The manufacturer of the product is responsio!* for tne sad oroduct ( f t o':3jt IS involved j j

10. Overall how dissatisfied were you Before you did anything aoout tne I'-z'o'^ s.:n as ;o ;i:» to t-e 'epa -Shop etc.). Please circle the numoer that Best represents /our 'eeiiig.

lOOS 90S 30S 70S 60S SOS 40S 30S 20S i:x )X Completely Oissatisfied lot : ssitisfi«3 it 4' '

11. Which of the following action (s) did you take after you eoer?nced f e iBo<e orooiem' i c * -i- -ne answer IS OK).

After I experienced tne above problem I:

Forgot about the incident and Oid notni-ig Complained to the repair shop 'imediately or on iy -if. t-o

Went Back or called the repair snop immediately ana isteo tnem to tj«« cir? ;f :>•( oroo Decided never to use that repair snop

Told -ny friends and relatives about my Bad experience Complained to a consumer agency, sucn is tne Better Sjsmess B--?ij

Took some legal action against the '•eoair shop •na'iji'i:t.,-»r Other, please specify

12. Now please tell us how you felt aoouC tne whole incident after you nad tuen ;"e iso-e ic: o" s). 'lease circle the number that best represents your feeling.

lOOS 90S aOS 70S SOS SOS 40S 30S 20S \'X :t Completely Satisfied 'lot Sifsr'^d at Ai:

13. What do you think auto repair snops ought to do to solve ~'> tyoe of orooiem you nad?

Section IV Next, Imagine that sometime in the future you had gone to your most frequently visited repair shop and tn inci­dent similar to the one you Indicated above occured again. Now read each of the 'ollowing statements and -ndi. cate the extent to which you think they are likely to happen. The categories are:

(6) Very Likely (4) Somewhat Likely (2)'Jnliket/ (S) Likely (3) Somewhat Unlikely (1) Very jniuely

1. Assjue that you reported the incident to tne repair shoo, now 'sely is it that tne repair shop would:

Very .fy '-'<e'/ .m >eiy

4.08(1.92) a. apologitt but do nothing 6 5 * 3 4.36(1.72) b. take appropriate action to take care of your problem ('e'j-d. etc.)... 6 5 4 3 1 1L(\ (sl\ ' '"'*• ><""• problem and give better service to you in tne '..tjre 6 5 4 3 i.l'^Kl.ol) J oe more careful in future and everyone would benefit s 5 4 3 3.48(1.69)

Page 267: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

Mean(std)

257

2. Assume tnat /ou -nent'oned tne orooiem to /our '-• tne same --epair snop. now Uxely is it that tie/ 3s ana 'e

j'3:

3 .70(1 .57) 4 . 6 4 ( 1 . 2 6 ) 3 .05(1 .63) 2 .77 (1 .60 ) ,

3.

2 . 9 5 ( 1 . 6 6 ) 2 .76 (1 .56 ) 2 . 4 6 ( 1 . 3 6 ) 2 . 2 7 ( 1 . 3 3 )

a. 0.

c. d.

Assume Better

b. c.

go on using the i-epair shop as jsual ^ Be more careful »ntn jsing that ^eoir shoo i stop using that repair shop altogetner ^ help you solve your problem '!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!...! 5 that you recforted the incident to a consumer agency, sucn is t-» Business Bureau, how likely is is tnat tney wouM:

not Believe you until many others nave similir :o<"o'i"'ts 5 take no action 5 make the reoair shoo take tare of your problem 5 solve your problem and ensure that the 'epair shop is care'-' in future..., 5

5.50(1 5.21(1, 4.i4(l 5.25(1, '•s74(l, 3.70(1, 3.31(1, 1.91(1, 3.00(1, 2.11(1,

04) 34) 68) 35) 50) 76) 88) 38) 86) 58)

Now I would like to ask you about what you might actually do in case the iBo you had gone to your most frequently visited repair shop.

4. If a similar problem occurred again, how lilcely is it that you would:

ve inc- :ei'. o::.'*<3 i-ji'

.,ar y

'. < e < '±J.

t 4 4

4 4 4

i 4 4 4

3 ) 3 3 3

3 3 3 1

2.55(1, 5.44(0, 5.52(0, 5.38(0,

64) 98) 78) 84)

a. forget the incident and do nothing 5 • b. definitely complain to the store manager on your next trip 5 : c. decide not to use that repair shop again 5 : d. 30 oacK or call tne repair shop immediately and ask tnem to ti<e

care of your problem S : e. speak to your friends and relatives aoout your bad experience 5 ! f. convince your friends and relatives not to use that repair snop 6 : g. complain to a consumer agency ind ask them to naxe the repair snop

taxe care of your problem S : n. write a letter to the local newspaper about your oad experience 5 i. report to a consumer agency so tnat they can warn otner considers 6 : J. take some legal action against the repair shop/manufacturer 6

As the last part of this section, I am going to ask you to think about how likely is 't tnat y:j aou d take any actlon(s) if you were pretty sure about the response you tre going to get. ^or example, many co^sjuers •ou'd complain only if they were confident that someone would take care of their concern, w m l e tn«r. are iiany others who would complain regardless of the response they get. Now read each of the following statements and tnen indicate the extent to which you tre likely to take that action.

Very _. ,ery

-'»ei/ 'J"''»?'/ 5. How likely is it that you would report the incident to the repair shop,

if you were pretty Sure that the repair shop would:

a. apologize but do nothing 5 0. taxe care of your proolem to your satisfaction 6 c. solve your proolem and give you Better service • -e future 6 d. Be more careful in future and everyone would be'-^-'t 6 '.

3.26(1.73) 4.85(1.18) 4.32(1.63) 4.40(1.64)

2.22(1.55) 1.83(1.28) 5.22(0.85) 5.33(0.86)

4 1 4 4

3 3 3 3

-•ow litely IS it that you would mention the incident to relatives if you were pretty sure that they would:

Our friends and

1. go on usiig that repair shop as usual 6 0. Be more careful onen using that repair shop 5 c. stop jsmg that repair shop altogetner 5 d. nelp you solve your prpblem 6

7. How likely is it that you would report the incident to a consumer agency, as tne Better Business Bureau, if you were pretty sure that they would:

Such

not Believe you until many others have similar compliints 6 take no action S make tne repair shop take care of your oroblem 6 solve the problem and ensure that the repair shop is careful m tne future *

5 5 5 5

4 4 4 4

3 3 3 3

5

J

> J

1

5 3 0

4 4 4

3 3 3

5

2 2

3 2

Page 268: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

Section V

In this last section, I would like to ask you a few oackground questions:

1. Which repair shop do /ou use iwst frequently (please 3i<e "am* ind :3cit ;<•

2. How many timet did you use this repair snop in tne last six o<ie /ear?

times ipproxmats /

3. How long have you been using this repair shop?

less than 6 months ' ess than 2 /ears But no'e tnan 5 nontns

4. What other repair shops do you use for auto repair?

5. How frequently do you use these other repair snops. say m tne last one /ear'

258

6. Are you:

7.

3.

Please check

Are you:

the

15

Male i-emaie

category that repi

to 21

20 years to 25 years 26 to 30 years

Single

31 to 35 years 36 to 40 years

— 41 to 45 years

Warned Divorced

9. What is your occupation (job title):

46 to 50 years 51 to 55 years

56 to 60 /ears Over 60 yrs.

Separated widowed

10. What is tne last year of formal education that you comp eted? (check one)

High school or less I'rade school College Graduate scnool

11. With wnlch ethnic or racial group do you identify yourself?

White Black

Hispanic ~ Other, please specify

12. PI east check the CJtegory that represents your total nousenold mcome Ho 'n t • ' married) in 1984?

less than $10,000 $10,000 to $20,000

— $20,001 to $30,300

$30,001 to $50,000 $50,301 to $70,300

— $70,001 to $90,000

$90,001 to $110,000 Over $110,001

Thank you for your cooperation

Page 269: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

259

Medical Care Survey

This questionnaire Is designed to determine the spec"': complaints tnat peoo'e -)>• medical tare 'rom a physician and/or hospital. Tnere are no -'gnt or wrong i-s«»-s nions are important. Please read the instructions carefully and answer i ' g^estions.

'O.r oersonj ;pi.

Section 1 First I'm going to ask you some general questions about your opinions regara'-g o^rst i' United States. I would like you to read each statement as if appears, ''len -rtite tn< or disagreeaent by circling the n\aib*r that Best describes your react'on to the state**-'

(6) Strongly Agree (5) Agree

(4) Agree Somewhat 2) isi;-(3) Disagree Somewhat ('.) Qisag'

s 1": -:so' ^ i ? - t .' .

. •-• .i-.iz

ee i t-; -g

Mean(std)

4 . 6 5 ( 1 . 3 0 ) I. Complaining to hospitals/physicians is usually done by people "itn little •ise

to do 5

2 . 6 6 ( 1 . 4 1 ) 2. Most hospitals care nothing at all aoout the patient 5

• ^^ ^ 3. Most physicians care nothing at all about the patient 5

3 . 7 3 ( 1 . 5 5 ) 4. It Bothers me quite a bit if I do not complain about poor medical t-eit-ent 5

2 22(1 25) ^ ' ' 5. The medical industry has Been a large influence in raising our count'/ s

standard of nealth 5 t*. ID(1. £.0) g Hospitals ire getting so big that they don't treat the patient oersonal'/ 5 5 . 5 2 ( 0 . 7 8 ) 7. Peoole have a responsibility to tell hospital/pny$ic1ans wnen the treatment

or service they receive is poor 6

3 . 7 3 ( 1 . 4 9 ) 8. Getting satisfactory medical care is a real problem 6

3 . 2 9 ( 1 . 4 8 ; q [J sometimes feels good to get my oissatisfactton and frustration with medical care off my chest oy comolaining 5

3 . 5 6 ( 1 . 5 9 ) Q_ Patients do not have any influence on the medicines that are prescrioed to them *

3 . 7 0 ( 1 . 5 1 ) 11. All hospitals really want to do is to make the most money they can 6

3 . 6 5 ( 1 . 3 7 ) 12. All physicians really want to do is to make the most money tney can 5

4 . 7 6 ( 1 . 4 4 ) jj_ People are bound to end up with unsatisfactory medical tr»it-,?nt once in a while, so tney should not complain S

3 . 9 4 ( 1 . 2 2 ) 14 Physicians prescribe more medicines than the patient rei / -eeds S 3 . 7 2 ( 1 . 3 5 ) 15. Complaining has to be done to xeep hospitals from becomi-g "esoonsible 6

3 . 6 7 ( 1 . 4 2 ) j5_ Complaining has to Be done to keep physicians from oecon-g irrespons'Ole 6

i 4 l 7 5 ( 1 . 2 3 ) 17. It is hard to jnderstand why some physicians are twice as expensive as otners.. i

3 . 3 9 ( 1 . 5 2 ) ,j J fjg„ comolain when I'm dissatisfied with medical care Because ! feel it IS my duty to do so

2 . 9 2 ( 1 . 3 9 ) j5 T g patient is usually the least important consideration to most nospitals 6

2 . 6 2 ( 1 . 2 8 ) 2 0 . The patient is usually the least important consideration to most pnysicuns.... 6

4 . 3 5 ( 1 . 4 9 ) ^ 1 AJ „,„ J, they discharge a patient most hospitals forget aoout tne patent.... 6

4 . 3 7 ( 1 . 3 1 ) 2 2 . By making complaints about unsatisfactory medical care, m the long rjn tne quality of health service will improve *

1 . 7 6 ( 1 .^'48) 23. One must be willing to tolerate poor medical service *

5 . 3 7 ( 1 . 0 7 ) 2*- f^^ces of medical treatment is going jp faster than the incomes of ordinary ^ consiiners

2 . 6 4 ( 1 . 0 9 ) ^ 5 ,|jjj hospitals are willing to accept reasonable complaints S

:ilS " tn»

.' i;-««<«««t

:' »s are:

• " ; <

2

2

>

2

2

2

2

2

-

'

1

1

1

'

1

2 1

Page 270: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

260

M««n (std) ;;"

2 . 9 6 ( 1 . 2 4 ) 2 6 . Most pn^sic'ins ,re -ilHn, tj accept reasonaol* compia.nts $

2 . 9 5 ( 1 . 0 9 ) 2 7 . Hospitals generally offer the services that the patent re«|!, ,,,..5 ,

4 . 2 8 ( 1 , 4 3 ) 2 8 . •hospital profits tre mgn yet they xeep on raismq their prices 5

4 ' 1 6 n ' A 9 1 '' ''''*'''*" "'•''"" *••• '"9»» yet they keep on raising tneir jnces

30. I feel a sense of accomplishment ^ntn I manage to get r-onolimt '0 1

hospital taken care of !.......!..! •

2 j A 0 ( l . 3 8 ) 3 1 . In general hospitals ire plain dishonest m tneir Jeilmgs .un t-e pifen:.... i

32. By complaining about poor medical service. I may prevent T^^r ^.ool* ''om

experiencing the same prpBlon .\_..' ^

. H I . 5 0 ) 33. I j^ gf5g„ dissatisfied with the medical care I receive i

4 . 5 5 ( 1 . 2 2 ) 3 4 . I don't like people who complain to physicians, because JS,.I11/ -neir complaints ire unreasonable ! 5

2 . 8 0 ( 1 . 3 1 ) 3 5 . A large variety of physicians allows people to choose the one tne/ 'ei'ly want

4 . 5 6 ( 1 . 2 1 ) 3 6 . The medical industry trys to influence government for tneir own oenefit 5

; 1

» J

» I

3

: )

i I

4 ]

1 3

Section 11 In this section, I would like to ask you about your experience in handling problems and cowlamts toncermna medical care. '

1. Have you, in the last year contacted any physician/hospital regarding any oroo em. such as mco-o.••'•:• waiting time, charges, etc.?

res 10

If yes, how often? about 1 or 2 times

aoojt 3 to 6 times about 7 to 12 t -nes

Tiore than 12 times

3.38(1.49)

2. How often do you talk to your fnends and relatives regarding proolems and compii -ts you -i»e toncemiq medical care (circle appropriate numOer)?

6 5 4 3 2 1 Often Never

3. -lave you m the last year contacted a third party, such is --e "Medical Association or tne Better Business Bureau regarding an^ medical complaint?

'es No

If yes, roughly how many times in the last yetrl times approximately

4. Have you ever taken legal action against a physician ana or nospital regarding any of /our comp'amts?

'es

If yes, roughly now many times? I0

times approximately

Section III Next, I would like for you to think of a problem that you remaaber most clearly concerning your experience with medical cart. For exaaplt, you may have been unhappy with the competence of the doctor ana/or nospital staff, tht waiting tiat at tht doctor's offlct/hospltal or tht manner in which you were treated. Keeping m mind your particular problaa, please answer the following questions.

I. Please describe briefly your problem or dissatisfaction:

Page 271: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

261

Mean(8td) , 2. When did tms proplem occur:

•*""" ""• '«»' 8 •«>"'« iK»re than 6 »onths ago «jr. -,r , ,, ,,. but within the last I year

?*JlIIl!l ??l!* ' •*•'* ""• *• "* '^ • '*• "»•«''»"« *"»«»«« w e xoPla* and whom you feel s 'esoonsBie 'or -.-!?^..! .* *?• " •"••* ••«* »' "•• follo«ln, statements and then indicate the extent of your igreeme-t :r lil •VetMnt by elrelint tht appropriaU ni»Ptr. '^ ' i- - . »

3. Tht phytlcian i$ to blaaw for tht above problem (If applicable) 3

2 . 2 3 ( 0 . 8 3 ) 4. Tht physicians or hospltaPs staff is simply careless ; 1.93(0.77). ,^ , .

5. rht physician/hospital didn't mean for the prpblem to occur, out mistaxes nappe"... 3 : ^ . 1 7 ( 0 . 5 8 ) g, I expected too much of the physician/hospital 1 ;

2 . 0 9 ( 0 . 5 4 ) 7. It is probably my fault, since I should be leore careful m selecting -ne physician/hospital j ; ;

2 . 2 9 ( 0 . 8 4 ) 8. The hospital is responsible for tht problem (If a hospital is involved) 3 2

9. Overall how dissatisfied were you before you did anything about tne prpoiem. "ease ;•-:*« tne ---loer -..m best represents your feeling.

lOOS gOS SOS 70S 60S SOS 40S 30S 20S lOS 3S Completely Dissatisfied Not Dissafsf'ed it All

10. Which of the following actlon(s) did you take after you experienced tne apove problem? more tnan on* mswer is OK).

After I experienced the above problem I:

Forgot about the incident and did nothing Complained to the hospital and/or physician inwiediately

Went back or called the hospital and/or physician aoout the problem Otcldtd never to go to that hospital and/or physician

Told my friends and relatives about my bad experience Complained to a third party, sucn as the Medical Association or tne tetter Business Bureau

Took some legal action against tne hospital and/or pnysicin Other, please specify

11. Now please tell us now satisfied you were with the whole incident after you had taxe" :nt aao<e action (si. Please circle tht nwotr that otst represents your feeling.

lOOS 90S BOS 70S 60S SOS 401 30S 20S lOS 7S Completely Satisfied tot Satisfied at A M

12. What do you think physicians and/or nospitals ought to do " solve the type pf problem you had?

Section IV Dtit, imagint that ^owtimt in tht futurt you had gont to yotr t t t frtquftly visittd phyticien/hospital and an incidtnt similar to tht ont you indleattd aPovt occurrtd again. No* rted tacn of tht fpllaving stateatnts and indlcatt tht eittnt ta which you thinfc thty are Hktlr to happtt. Tht eattgpritt art:

(•) Vtry Llktly (4) Somtwhat Liktiy (2) Unllktiy (S) LIktIy (3) Somtwhat Unliktiy (I) Vtry Unliktiy

Very Very Likely ^niutly

1. AssMt that you reported tht incidtnt to tht hospital and/or physician, how llktiy is it that tnt physician and/or hospital would:

3.06(1.99) a. apologllt but do nothing 6 5 4 3 2 1 3.45(1.88) b. takt appropriate action to takt cart of your prooleai 6 5 4 3 2 1 •> 1 1 / 1 a n \ c. solvt your proPlea and givt bttttr strvlct to you in tnt futurt 6 5 4 3 2 1 J.ll^l.oUj d. ot mort cartful in future and everyone would btntfit 6 S 4 3 2 1 3.07(1.76)

Page 272: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

262

Hean(std)

3.07(1.72) 4.77(1.32) 2.75(1.74) 2.79(1.63)

2.61(1.56) 2.13(1.45) 2:47(1.53) 2.14(1.40)

- ' / Assume that you mentioned the problem to your friends and r.ij'.w.s ">-the same nospital and/or pnysidan. how likely is it that they wou'd:

a. continue to go to the same hospital md/or physican 5 0. be more careful when they go to that physician and/pr lospiti' 5 c. stop going to that hospital and/or physician altogether 6 d. help you solve your problem ^

AssiMt that you reported m e incidtnt to a third party, such as tne Med'cai Association or the Setter Business Bureau, how Hkely is is that they •;. ::

i. not believe you until many others have similar coiwliints 5 b. take no action 5 c. make the physician/hospital take care of your proolem 6 d. solve your problem and ensure that the pnysidan and/or nospital

is careful in future 4

Now I would like to ask you about what you might actually do in case the above incident occ' you had gont to your most frequently visited hospital and/or pnysidan.

iga-

-?'/

A.92(1 4.88(1 4.09(1 4.46(1 4.83(1 3.60(1 2.80(1 2.17(1 2.82(1 2.04(1 2.70(1

.61)

.71)

.94)

.73)

.53)

.93)

.84)

.59)

.80)

.64)

4. If a similar problem occurred again, how likely is It that you would:

2.69(1 5.32(1 5.39(1 5:44(1

a. forget the incident and do nothing 5 5 « 3 b. definitely complain to the hospital and/or physician on your next

trip 6 decide not to go to that hospital and/or physician again 6 go back or call the nospital and/or pnysician i:mmediately and as< them to take care of your prpblem 6 speak to your friends and relatives about your pad experience 6 convince your friends and relatives not to go to that nospital and/or physician.. 5

g. comolain to a consumer agency and ask them to make the hospital and/or physician take care of your problem 6

h. write a letter to the local newspaper about your bad experience 6 i. report to a third party so that they can warn other consumers 6 j. take some legal action against the hospital and/or physician 6 5 4 3 2 1

.69)At tht last part of this section, I am going to ask you to think about how likely Is It that you would take any action(t) if you were pretty sure about the response you art going to gtt. For exawle. aany people would complain only If they wtrt confidtnt that soewont would takt cart of thtir conctrn, wnilt there ire «any others who would complain regardless of the response they gtt. Now rtad each of the following statements and then indicate tht extent to which you are likely to take that action.

Very lery L'xely Jn' <e^t

5. How likely is it that you would report the incident to the '-spital and/or physician if you were pretty Sure that tne hospital i-d/or physician would:

c. d.

e. f.

.69)

.09)

.12)

.07)

3.73(1.80) 5.08(1.18) 4.06(1.80) 4.37(1.60)

2.60(1.68) 1.86(1.35) 4.62(1.55) 5.00(1.36)

a. apologize but do nothing 6 b. take care of your proolem to your satisfaction 6 c. solve your problem and give you better service •' tne future 6 d. be more careful in future and everyone would be^^'it 6

How likely Is it that you would mention the incident to /our friends and relatives if you were pretty sure that they would:

a. continue to go to that hospital and/or physician 6 b. be more careful when they go to tnat hospital and/or physician 6 c. stop going to that hospital and/or physician altogether 6 d. help you solve your problem 6

HOW likely if it that you would report the incident to a third oarty, sucn as the Medical Association or tht Better Business Bureau, if you were pretty sure that thty would:

a. not Dtlitvt you until many othtrs nave similar complaints < 6 b. takt no action 6 c. makt tht hospital and/or physician taxe care of your problem 6 d. solve the problem and ensure that the nospital and/or pnysidan

is cartful in tht future 8

5 5 5 5

4 4 4 4

3 3 3 3

t

1

2 2

I 1 I I

5 5 5 5

4 4 4 4

3 3 3 3

2 > 2 I

i

1 1 i

5 5 5

4 4 4

3 3 3

2 2 2

1 I i

Page 273: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

263

Stctlon V

In this last stctlon, I would likt to ask you a few background questions:

. I. How frequently have you visited any physician for medical care m the last /eir'

less than once more than once but less than 5 times

fi re •"!

'•iii t-a

•no'e t"l" .J t

2. How frequently have you visited any hospital for iiedlcal care m the last /ear?

less than once more than once but less than 5 times

lore ••'1" 0 • --i 0

'ess tnan ;: t ••a

more than 13 fm«s

3. What do you think physician and/or hospitals can do to improve service to patients?

4.

S.

«.

7.

Are you: Male

Female

Please check the category that represents

15 to 20 years 21 to 25 years

26 to 30 years

Are you: Single

What is your occupation (job title)?

your age:

31 to 35 years 36 to 40 years

41 to 45 years

Married Divorced

46 to 53 years 51 to 55 years

36 to CO years Over 60 yrs

Separated Widowed

8. What Is the last year of formal education that you cafflo:'?tj:? (check one)

High school or less Trade school College Graduate school

9. With which ethnic or racial group do you identify yourself? -

White Black

Hispanic Other, please specify

10. Please check the category that represents your total household income (joint if married) in 1984.

less than $10,000 $10,000 to $20,000

~~ $20,001 to $30,000

$30,301 to $50,000 $50,001 to S70,000

"~ $70,001 to $90,000

$90,001 to $110,000 Over $110,301

Thank you for your cooperation

Page 274: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

264

financial Services Sjr»«/

This questionnaire Is designed to determine t»e specific to-oiai^ts i q or-oems tnat o-^o a variety of financial institutions; Including banks, savings and 'oars, ced't jn^ois, et people tre not happy with the service charges banks put on cnecxmg accounts, tne oa'i-c-manner In which loan applications are handled by many Banxs. 'nere tre no rijn; or .r-,-g personal opinions tre important. Please read the instructons care'uily and answer a ;.

Section I First I'm going to ask you some general questions about your opinions reqaro'ig ajnis (.n savings and loans, and credit unions) in the United States. I would l'«« /ou to reaj eic appears. Then Indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement oy Q-':'."-q tn« ^jy^t your reaction to the statement. The categories tre:

(6) Strongly Agree (5) Agree

«) Agree Somewhat (3) Disagree Somewhat

Mean(std)

4.96(1.07) 2.93(1.26)

3.77(1.40) 3.06(1.37)

4.40(1.34)

5.45(0.80)

3.25(1.33) 3.37(1.40)

3.24(1.63)

4.40(1.31) 4.54(1.22)

2.91(1.28)

3.64(1.44) 4.88(1.21)

3.60(1.39)

2.82(1.32)

3.20(1.38)

4.01(1.32)

2.11(l.'>4e)

4.38(1.26)

2,61(1.06) 2.71(1.01)

4.25(1.22)

2) 5'sagr (I) Disagr

iqr.e

1. Complaining to Banks is usually done By people with little else to do 5

2. Most banks really care about their customers 5

3. It bothers me quite a bit if I do not complain about poor service from a oam.. 6

4. The banking Industry has Been a large influence in improving our country's financial position S

5. Banks are getting so Big that they don't treat the customer perspnally 6

6. People have a responsoility to tell banks when a service they recewe IS not satisfactory 6

7. Getting satisfactory service from Banks is a real problem 6

8. It sometimes feels good to get my dissatisfaction and frustration in dealing with banks off my chest by complaining 6

9. People do not have any influence on the manner m «hlch the Banks conduct

their Business 6

0. All Banks really want to do is to make the most money they can 6

1. People are bound to end up witn unsatisfactory service once >n a wmle.

so they Should not complain 6

2. Banks encourage people to spend more than they really shoJd 6

3. Complaining nas to be done to keep Banks from Becoming "-esoonsiBle 6

4. It is hard to understand why financial services (such as :-ecxing accounts) at some banks are twice as expensive as others 6

5. I often complain when I'm dissatisfied with bank services Because I 'eel it is my duty to do so 5

6. The customer is usually the least important consideration to most banks 6

7. As soon as they open your account, most banks forget about the customer S

8. By making complaints apout unsatisfactory service, m tne long run tne quality of banks will improve 5

9. One must be willing to tolerate poor service from most oanxs 6

0. Costs of financial services are going up faster than the -"omes of many people *

1. Most banks tre willing to adjust reasonable complaints 6

2. Banks generally offer what people want 6

3. Banks profits are high, yet they keep on raising the cost of their services.... 6

• " t . « " : t < -q • ' t n • : - •ttitot. - i ' ,

3' s t a t e " « " t s tn<] ••<# "Sw«rs. - - . » , e r , : . , r S t i p - S .

s 1 1 : , ? • • -• - I t : e $

4

4

*

*

4

4

4

4

4

O l - i S .

IS t .",-.' oes

D ' i i r ' t

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

2

1

I

1

1

I

I

Page 275: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

265

»qree '•'•"•'i /.

M8an(3td)

4 : 4 3 ( 1 . 1 6 ) 2 4 . I feel a sense of accompl ishme'-t when ! nanage to je' i compli'^' -o a r>, taken care of ,

2.01(1.21) ^ , ' 25. In general banxs tre plain dishonest in tneir dealings .itn -re jeop e 5

4 . 0 9 ( 1 . 3 2 ) 2 6 . By complaining about unsatisfactory service. I may pre«»nr o'-*' o-oo'e --om

experiencing the same problem ] , , ' ^

2 . 5 7 ( 1 . 3 0 ) 2 7 . I am often dissatisfied with the service I receive n most oanns ^

4 : 6 3 ( 1 . 2 3 ) 2 8 . I don't like people who complain to oanxs. Because usually t-e- :omo i "ts

are unreasonable ,;

2 . 2 8 ( 1 . 1 2 ) 2 9 . A large number of Banks allows people to Choose the one tney -ea'l/ .i-t 5

4 . 0 7 ( 1 . 2 5 ) " 8*"'" ''•y to influence government Just to Better tnemseUes s 2 . 0 7 ( 1 . 2 2 ) 31. Banks firmly stand Behind their deposits m d guarantees 5

Section H In this section, I would like to ask you about your experience in handling problems and complaiits m ;oti! various financial services from a bank (such as cheeking accounts, ioans. etc.).

1. Have you, in the last six (6), months contacted any Bank regarding m y oroO'e^. sjcn as errors m you-account, poor service, etc.?

->

1

1

J

1

3

3

-l_i

•r,

If yes, how often? No

about 1 or 2 times iDout 3 to 6 •. mes

iBout 7 to 12 f-nes more tnan 12 t "nes

2. How often do you talk to your friends m d relatives regarding oroolems m d c»iplai"ts /OJ ^i.» :o'':e''"'-g financial service (circle appropriate numoer)?

6 5 4 3 2 1 2.39(1.29) 21-SH v.er

3. Have you in the last six (6) months contacted a consumer or puolic agency, sucn as tne Bctt*- 3jsiness Bureau regarding any complaint?

Tes No

If yes, roughly how many times in the last six months? times approximately

4. Have you ever taken legal action against i bank regarding i-y of your comoiaints?

'es

If yes, roughly how many times? •<o

times approximately

Section ::i Next, I would like for you to think of a problem that you remember most clearly concerning your experience with banks. For exa«ple. you may have oeen unhappy with the way your account was handled or balanced, the jnfair treatment of your loan application, the behavior of bank employees or the various service charges put on your account. Keeping in mind your particular problem, please answer the following questions.

1. Please describe Briefly your problem or dissatisfaction:

2. When did this problem occur:

within the last 6 months more than 6 montns But less than 1 year

more tnan a yetr ago

3. N M O and location of the bank involved:

Page 276: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

266

M«an (std)

2.24(0.80)

1.88(0.75) 1.67(0.79)

2.20(0.58) 1.76(0.55)

At this point I would like to ask you a few questions about the proB'tm irj ^^ ^^ - „ , -taon^ •> . I would like you to read each of the following statements and tn-- ind-ite :ne ..-ent of ,->.r ,a ,-• agreement By circling the appropriate number. ^

^:'^f : i;rje

4. The bank policies'are to Blame for tne aoove proolem 3

5. The bank personnel tre simply careless ]

6. The bank didn't mean for the problem to occur, out mistakes happen ;

7. I expected too much of the Bank j

8. It is prooably my fault, since I should Be more careful m select ig -e oi''« 1

9. Overall how dissatisfied were you Before you did anything loct tie or-o^-i - eis,? ; -: •-•? -.-:•>-best represents your feeling.

lOOS 90S 30S 70S 60S SOS 40S 30S ITt ITt OS Completely Dissatisfied Not Tssat'sf'^J it J''

10. Which of the following action (s) did you take after you experienced -• ipove o-oo -'-'' n-:-» f i " is Ok).

After I experienced the above problem I:

Forgot about the incident and did nothing Comolained to tne Bank immediately

Went Back or called tne Bank aoout the proolem Decided never to use that Bank

Told •ny friends and reJati/es about my oad e'oerie^ce Complained to a consjner agency, such as tne 3ett»r 3jsi^ess 3.'-?i-

Took some legal action against tne amx Other, please specify

11. Now please tell us how satisfied you were about the wnole incident atte-- /ou •iio tnen --e )Oo<e act Please circle tne numoer that Best represents your feeling.

lOOS 90S 30S 70S SOS SOS 40S 30S 20S U t » Completely Satisfied 'Jot Sat 'sf'ed at 4' i

12. What do you think banks should do to solve the type of problem you nad?

•o' t. -"• 3 1 s -

on '$).

Section !V Next, imagine that sometime in the future you had gone to yo.' ••ost f ^ ' ^ n y H / f!*^^*^ **"* *"* *" '"eident similar to the one you indicated above occurred again. Now -jad each of the following statements and 'noic the extent to which you think they are likely to happen. The categories are:

cate

(6) Ury Likely (5) Likely

(4) Somewhat .lely (3) Somewhat .nlikaly

( 2 ) J n l ' i i e l y f l ) very Unluely

3,70(2.10) 4.30(1.88) 3.85(1.90) 3.46(1.86)

1.90(1.22) 4.23(1.59) 1.77(1.22) 2.92(1.82)

Very

Assume tnat you reported the incident to the Bank, now lUely is it that the otnK would:

a. apologlte but do nothing S b. take appropriate action to take care of your proolem 6 c. solve your proolem and give Better service to you in tne futjr* 6 d. oe more careful in future and everyone would Benefit 6

Very

Assune that you mentioned the problem to your friends and relatives «no jse the same bank, how likely is u that they would:

a. go on using that bank as usual 6 b. be more careful when using tnat bank 6 c. stop using that bank altogether 6 d. help you solve your proolem 6

/•ry

.ery .n' - n i y

Page 277: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

Mean(std)

267

3.19(1.72) 2.77(1.80) 2.65(1.70) 2.20(1.61)

5.17(1.40) 4:83(1.59) 3.23(1.98) ^371(1.81) 4s00(1.80) 2.53(1.58) 2.25(1.54) 1.73(1.37) 2.28(1.55) 1.58(1.22)

3.50(2.07) 5.27(1.35) 5.43(1.19) 5.33(1.28)

3.83(1.94) 4.57(1.54) 3.52U.93) 4.20(1.80)

2.15(].5.S) 1.61(1.21) 3.80(1.87) 3.93(2.00)

3. Assume that you reported the incident to a consuner agency, such as tne Setter Business Bureau, how likely is it that thty would:

4. not btlitve you until many pthtrs havt similar complaints b. take no action .-c. make the bank takt care of your problem d. solvt your problem and ensure that the bank is careful m future.

tmry . ' i ' y

How I would likt to ask you about what you might actually dp in case the above incident occ-'-e; yot had gont to your most frpqutntly visittd bank.

ierf

4. If a similar problem occurred again, how likely is it that you would:

4. forget tht incident and do nothing g b. definitely complain to the manager $ c. dtcidt not to ust that bank again g d. go back or call tht bank and ask them to take care of your proolem... 5 e. speak to your friends and relatives about your bad experience 6 f. convince your friends and relatives not to use that bank 6 g. complain to a consuner agency and ask them to make the bank taxe

care of your proolem $ h. write a letter to the local newspaper aoout your bad exoenence 6 I. report to a consumer agency so that they can warn other cons.4Mrs 6 j. take some legal action against the oank 6

'Jit

igam wnile

As tht last part of this stctlon. I am going to ask you to think about how likely is it that you would take any action(s) if you were pretty sure about the response you are going to gtt. For example, many people would complain only If thty wtrt confidtnt that someone would take cart of thtir conctrn, while there are many others who would complain regardless of the response they get. Now read each of the following statamtnts and tnen Indicate tht extent to which you are likely to take that action.

Very Very Lixely 'Jniuelf

5. How likely is It that you would report the incident to the bank, if you were pretty sure that the bank would:

a. apologize but do nothing 6 b. take care of your problem to your satisfaction 6 c. solve your oroblem and give you better service in the future 6 d. be iwre careful in the future and everyone would benefit 6

6. How likely is it that you would mention the incident to your fritnds i'-relatlves If you were pretty sure that thty would:

a. go on using that bank as usual 6 b. bt more careful when using that bank 5 c. stop using that Bank altogether i d. help you solve your problem 6

7. How likely is It that you would report the incident to a consumer agency, as the Better Business Bureau, if you were pretty sure tnat thty would:

Such

a. not believe you until many others have similar complaints 6 0. take no action 6 c. make the bank take care of your problem 6 d. solve tne problem and ensure that the bank is careful in the future... 6

5 5 5 5

4 4 4 4

3 3 3 3

2 2 2 2

I 1 I I

Stctlon V

In this last stctlon. I would likt to ask you a few background qutstions:

1. Which is your primary bank? (pleast give nme and location)?

Page 278: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

268

2. How long nave you Been using this bank?

'•»« '141 5 months less tnan 2 years o.t -nore -:-• :-)o > ,,ars than 6 months ^

3. What other banks do you use for financial services?

4. What do you think Banks can do to improve service to customers?

5. Oo you use automatic teller machines (ATM) for depositing or witndri«ing money'

yes No

6. Oo you think the automatic te l le r machines (that jse Bank cards) ntie -iioe aanic-g fasier^

agree don't know disagree

7. All things considered, what is your opinion about the automatic teller macm-es^

8. Are you:

Male Female

9. Please check the category that represents your age:

1: to 20 years 31 to 35 years H to :•? /ears 21 to 25 years 36 to 40 years 51 to 55 /ears

26 ta 30 years 41 to 45 years S6 to SO .ears Over 60 <'S.

10. Are you: Single Married Separated

Divorced wiiowed

11. What is your occupation? (joo title):

12. What 1$ the last year of formal education that you cono'-^ted? (check one)

Hign school or less ''ride school College graduate sfooi

13. With which ethnic or racial group do you identify yourself?

rfhite Black

Misaanic Otner. please specify

14. Please check the category that represents your total household income (Joint if narr.o) in '.984.

less than $10,000 $30,001 to $50,000 590.001 to $110,000 $10,000 to $20,000 $50,301 to $70,000 Over S'tlO.OOl

$20,001 to $30,000 $70,001 to J90.300

Thank you for your cooperafon

Page 279: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

269

APPENDIX E: SELECTED DISCONTENT AND ALIENATION ITEMS

ITEM hfUMBER iTEM DESCRIPTION

DiKOBtoot #1

#2

#3

#4

#6

#7

#8

#0

#10

#11

#12

Industry hM an obligation to cleui ap the wMt« th«y have b««a damping bat they w«i't doing It.

Chain ptorap are gvtting PO big that th«y do not treat the consomer peraonally.

Stores advertM 'special d«ais* jast to get the alMppnr into the store to boy something else.

All business really wants to do is to maks ths most money it can.

Companies encourage the consnmer to b«y more than he/she really nseds.

It is hard to onderstan-: why sooie brands are twice as expensive as others.

As soon as they make a sale, most forget abont the bvyer.

PriceB of prodncts are going up (aster than the incomes of ordinary consumers.

Business profits are high yet they keep on raising their prices.

An attractive package sometimes influences a purchase that isn't necessary

Companies 'jaii up* a product with oo real improvement, just to get a higher prxe or sell more.

Companies try to influence the govemment jnst to better themselves.

Page 280: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …

2 7 0

ITEM NUMBER ITEM DESCRIPTION

ABenntioo #1 Most companies care nothing at all about the

#2

# 3

#4

#5

#7

#8

# 9

#10

Shopping is usually an nnpleasaat experience.

People are unable to determine what products wiU be sold ia the store.

The small businessman has to do what the big business says or else!

The consumer is usually the least important ronsifiersiion to most companies.

One must be willing to tolerate poor service from most stores.

Companies generally offer what the consumer wants (R).

In geasral companies are plain dishonest in their dealing with the consumer.

I am often dissatisfied with a recent purchase.

Business firms stand behind their products and guarantees (R).

Page 281: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …
Page 282: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION USING ITEM RESPONSE …