Alliance Brief

download Alliance Brief

of 22

Transcript of Alliance Brief

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    1/22

    Levels and Pat terns of the  Therapeutic

    Alliance

     in

      Brief Psychotherapy

    CHRISTOPHER L. STEVENS, Ph.D.*

    J. CHRISTOPHER MURAN, Ph.D.*

    JEREMY D. SAFRAN, Ph.D.

    BERNARD S. GORMAN, Ph.D.f .. •

    ARNOLD WINSTON, M.D.*

    We examined  the  relevance of the  level  and  pattern  of the  therapeutic

    alliance in 44  cases of  three different manualized 30-session treatments

    using patient

      r tings

      of the Working Alliance Inventory after

     e ch

     session. It

    was hypothesized that bo th high-alliance level and either  linear incre se in

    alliance rating or a  series of  hrief rupture-and-repair episodes would  be

    found

      in

      successful treatments.

      We

      also hypothesized that

      a

      more global

    high-low-high pattern predicted

      in tbe

      literature would

      not be

     present.

    Consistent with the literature higher  lli nce levels were found to be related

    to improved

     outcome. As

     predicted,

     we

     did not find

     

    glohal,  high-low-high

    pattern. Local rupture-and-repair patterns were found  in 30 of the cases;

    linear trends were found  in 66 of the cases. There was no relationship

    between outcome

     and

      either pattern.

      We

     found

      no

      differences among

     the

    treatments.

    LEVEL  PATTERN  OF THE  THER APEUT IC ALLL^NCE  IN  BRIEF

    PSYCHOTHERAPY

    Although findings that a strong, positive therapeutic alliance is related

    to positive treatment outcome has been quite consistent, (Horvath and

    Symonds, 1991; Martin, Garske, & Davis, 2000) the exact nature of this

    relationship has remained less clear. As a result, clinical theorists and

    researchers have increasingly turned their attention to determining how

    the alliance functions to foster change. For example, some (e.g., Raue &

    *Beth Israel Medical Center

      and

      Alben Einstein College

      of

      Med icine, New School

      for

      Social

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    2/22

    AMERICAN JOURNAL O F PSYCH OTHER APY

    Goldfrie d, 1994; Beck, 1995) believe that the alliance serves prim arily as a

    static foundation for the application of curative techniques, while others

    (e.g., Bo rdin, 1979, 1994; Safran M ura n, 2000) believe that the alliance

    is an active agent of the change process in its own right. Many of these

    authors have theorized that the way in which the alliance develops over

    time can give important information about how this takes place. More

    specifically, they hypothesized that working through conflicts or strains in

    the alliance leads to positive change and that the variability in the alliance

    ratings, indicating these periods of strain, could be a pre dictor of impro ved

    outcome.

    Much of this research can be characterized as a search for global shifts

    in the alliance over the course of treatme nt. Researchers, influenced in pa rt

    by Mann (1973), suggested that alliance developm ent goes throu gh a series

    of changes as patients react to the shifting techniques of the therapist, (e.g.

    Ge lso Ca rter 1985, 1994; H orv ath Lub orsky, 1993, Kivlighan

    Shaughnessy, 1995, 200 0). Specifically, resea rchers posit that th e alliance

    will proceed through three distinct phases. According to this model, the

    alliance is characterized by an initial period of high hope fostered by

    supportive alliance-building activity by the therapist. During the next

    phase of treatm ent, the patient realizes that all expectations will not be m et

    and increasingly challenges therapist activity, which leads to growing

    ambivalence, resistance, and greater strain on the alliance. Finally, in

    successful treatment, patients gain a more realistic understanding of the

    therapist's role and accept change and the limitations of therapy, which is

    brought about (at least in part) by the therapist's focus on working through

    and termination. This process may be perceived as following a high-low-

    high pattern of alliance development.

    A number of studies tried to demonstrate this global high-low-high

    pa ttern with mixed results (Bernard, Schw artz, Ocaltis Stiner, 1980;

    G olden Rob bins, 1990; H orvath M arx,  1991; Joyce an d P iper 1990;

    Kivlighan Shaughnessy, 2000; Pa tton , Kivlighan M ulton , 1997;

    Schwartz and B ernard,

      1981;

     and Tracey, 1989). Th e most com pelling case

    for a high-low-high pattern comes from KivHghan and Shaughnessy

    (2000). They noted that as a result of the statistical procedures used, both

    their 1995 study and the study by Patton, Kivlighan, and Multon (1997)

    were unable to detect possible subgroups of alliance development. As a

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    3/22

    Alliance Level and Pattem

    come. They used novice counselors and recruited subjects for a 4-session,

    short-term dynamic therapy. Analysis of their initial sample of 38 clients

    showed three distinct patterns of alliance development, which they iden-

    tified as stable alliance, linear alliance growth, and quadratic alliance

    growth. A subsequent replication sample of 41 clients identified the same

    three p attern s. In add ition, this analysis of the results of the second sample

    showed that clients reporting a quadratic pattern of alliance development

    had greater improvement on outcome measures compared to other pat-

    terns of development.

    Although the results offer the strongest support to date of a rupture-

    and-repair process, there are several significant limitations to this study.

    The stated goal of the study was to examine patterns of global alliance

    development, but such a brief treatment is a significant shortcoming. First,

    it seems unlikely that any of the stages that Mann described had time to

    develop. Similarly, Horvath and Luborsky's (1993) model also suggested

    that the alliance-building phase of treatment would take place over the

    first five sessions, peaking at session three. With only four sessions of

    treatment, it is impossible to determine if the changes observed were

    related to those that might be seen in longer treatment. Second, with only

    four sessions, there are otily 16 potential patterns that could develop,

    several of which could be interpreted as high-low-high, stable, or linear

    improvement. Since Kivlighan and Shaugnessy found three different pat-

    terns, it might be more important to ask which patterns did not emerge.

    And finally, it is impossible to distinguish between discrete or transient

    changes in alliance and more global dynamics.

    Although, taken as a group, these studies seem to provide support for

    the notion of a global high-low-high process, all suffered from significant

    limitations. Sample sizes were small or not representative of clinical

    pop ulation s (Tracy, 1989; G old en Robb ins, 1990). The training and

    exp erien ce of the therapis ts involved was limited (Kivlighan Shaugh-

    nessy, 1995, 2000 ; Pa tton et al., 1997). Tre atm ent length was inconsistent

    (Bernard et al. 1980; Bernard and Schwartz, 1981) or extremely brief

    (Kivlighan Shaughnessy, 2000). Add itionally, the statistical m ethod s

    used were not appropriate for detecting patterns that might exist among

    subg roup s of patien ts (Tracy, 1989; G old en Robb ins, 1990; Kivlighan

    Shaughnessy, 1995; Patton et al., 1997). These limitations raise the ques-

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    4/22

    A M E R IC A N J O U R N A L O F P S Y C H O T H E R A P Y

    A review of the literature to date suggests that there is moderate

    support for the notion that variability increases in the midphase of

    treatment (Bernard, Schwartz, Ocaltis and Stiner, 1980; Schwartz and

    Bernard, 1981). Similarly, there is some initial support for the notion that

    undergoing a rupture and repair process is related to improved outcome

    (Form an M arm ar, 1985; Lansford, 1986; G olde n Robb ins, 1990;

    H orva th M arx, 1991; Kivlighan Shaughnessy, 2000 ; Pa tton , Kivlighan

      M ulton, 1997). How ever, Ge lso and Carter, as well as H orva th and

    Luborsky, point out that the strains in the alliance experienced during the

    middle phase of treatment often are the result of more focal strains on the

    relationship, which are brought about by momentary lapses or empathic

    failures on the part of the therapist, disagreements on tasks or goals, or

    strains on the bo nd. Both empirical (Forman M armar, 1965; Rho des,

    H ill, Th om pson Elliott, 1994; Safran, Cro cker, M cM ain M urray,

    1990; M uran , 2002; Safran M uran 1996; Safran, M uran Samstag,

    1994; Safran, M uran , Samstag Stevens, 2002 in press) and theoretical

    (Bord in, 1994; Safran M ura n, 2000) work suggest that when strains in

    the alliance are not addressed, outcome is negatively impacted and drop

    out may occur.

    If this is true, then the middle phase of treatment might be better

    characterized by a series of brief ruptures and repairs in which the

    alliance is strained. In successful treatment, the strain is addressed and

    the alhance returns to previously high levels, not during the final stage

    of treatment, but within a few sessions. Rather than thinking of alliance

    development in terms of curves, with global decrease in patient satis-

    faction and general weakening of the alliance in the middle phase, it

    could be envisioned as a series of dips and spikes representing, not

    global shifts in satisfaction, but a kind of session-by-session negotia-

    tion proces s (Safran M ura n, 200 0).

    The present study attempts to address many of the methodological

    problems that limited the conclusions drawn from previous studies and to

    test the idea that the rupture-and-repair process is better described as a

    local, rather than global, phenomenon. Consistent with the Bordin's

    (1994) assertion that different patients work differently in the alliance, two

    patterns of alliance development were predicted. The first form is a linear

    increase in Working Alliance Inventory (WAI) scores across the course of

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    5/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    to five sessions. A global high-low-high pattern was not expected to

    appear. Additionally, it was predicted that treatments demonstrating a

    linear increase or rupture-and-repair episodes would evidence better

    outcome than those that did not.

    METHODS

    PARTICIPANTS ., . . -

     her pists

    This study was conducted as part of a psychotherapy research project

    at a major medical center in New York City (see Muran, 2002, for an

    overview). The therapists were drawn from the department of psychiatry

    and included attending psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, social workers,

    psychiatry residents, psychology interns and externs. To assure consistency

    of treatment, all of the therapists, regardless of their background and

    experience, received similar training and evaluation in the therapeutic

    model they practice. To assure treatment adherence, trained observers

    rated videotapes of a randomly selected session in each treatment third

    (beginning, middle and end), using an instrument developed by the

    project, which has demonstrated adequate psychometric properties (Pat-

    ton, M uran, Safran, W ach tel, W insto n, 1996). Licensed clinicians

    participated in one-hour weekly supervision until they met acceptable

    standards for treatment adherence (1-2 cases), while unlicensed therapists

    continued to receive individual supervision for all of their cases. All

    therapists participated in a weekly 90 -m in ut e g roup supervision.

    Patients were randomly assigned to one of three manualized, 3 0 -

    session, on ce- pe r-w eek treatment conditions designed to treat person ality-

    disordered patients. All the therapy sessions were videotaped. The three

    different conditions were Brief Relational Therapy ([BRT] Safran, 2002;

    Safran M uran , 2000; M uran Safran, 2002 ), Co gnitive-Behavioral

    Therapy ([CBT ] Tu rner M uran, 1992) and Brief Ad aptive Psychother-

    apy ([B AP]; Pollack, Flegen heirmer Kaufman, 1988). Brief Relational

    Therapy is an integrative model that combines the principles of relational

    psychoanalysis and humanistic psychotherapy. It focuses on detection and

    resolution of alliance ruptures and has an explicit emphasis on process

    rather than content. The CBT condition is a fairly traditional, schema-

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    6/22

    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHO THERA PY

    to the environment in a more adaptive fashion. Of the 128 cases reviewed

    for inclusion in this the project, 44 fit the specific inclusion criteria

    (presented below). Of the 44 patients, eighteen were seen in CBT, 14 in

    BRT and 12 in BAP.

    Patients

    Patients were recruited using advertisements in local newspapers,

    inviting adults suffering from long-standing depression, anxiety, or

    interpersonal problems to participate in a program to investigate

    short-term psychotherapy. The principal inclusion criterion was that

    they met criteria for diagnosis of Personality Disorder (PD) cluster C or

    NOS on axis II. Exclusion Criteria was evidence of schizophrenia or

    other psychoses, organic brain syndrome or mental retardation, mania

    or bipolar disorder, severe obsessive compulsive disorder or serious

    eating disorder, serious dissociative disorders, a current substance use

    disorder, active para-suicidal or suicidal behavior, a history of violent

    behavior or severe impulse control problems, psychotropic medication

    use within the past six months.

    Patient

     demographics.

     Th ere were an equal num ber of male and female

    patients. They ranged in age from 25 to 63 years (M   =  40, SD =10). All

    w ere edu cated at or above the high school level, and 7 5 had a college

    degree or at least some gradu ate level education. M ore than 7 5 were

    employed. Eighty percent were Caucasian.

    Patient diagnostic characteristics.

      All of the patients in this study m e

    the criteria for an Axis II diagnosis, and all but two met criteria for an Axis

    I diagnosis in the

      DSM IV

      (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). All

    patients with an Axis I diagnosis were in the mood or anxiety disorder

    spectrum. All 44 patients met criteria for an Axis II diagnosis, and 14 also

    met criteria for a secondary Axis II diagnosis based on the SCID-II. More

    than half (23) had a personality disorder NOS, with the remainder

    diagnosed as falling into Cluster C (14 avoidant, 7 obsessive compulsive,

    aggressive).

    Coders

    We had four coders classify alliance development curves and identify

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    7/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    MEASURES

     lliance

    Alliance was assessed using the short form of the WAI (Horvath

    G reenb erg, 1989, Tracey Kokotovic, 1989).The W AI was adm inistered

    at the end of each session as part of a postsession questionnaire (PSQ)

    designed to assess the therapeutic relationship (Muran et. al. 1991).

    The WAI (Horvath, 1989) was created to assess the working alliance

    independent of a therapist's theoretical orientation while simultaneously

    providing a clear description of what constitutes a working alliance and

    how the alliance functions to promote positive change. Items for the WAI

    were generated in a unique multi-step process to insure content validity.

    Items were created initially to reflect the three different dimensions (tasks,

    goals and bond) of Bordin's model (Bordin 1974, 1979, 1980) and were

    rated by psychologists from different theoretical backgrounds. Experts on

    the working alliance reviewed each item, rating it for relevance. The

    remaining items were reviewed a second time from randomly selected

    practicing psychologists and separated into clusters corresponding to each

    of the three dimensions (tasks, goals and bo nd ). Th e to p- rat ed 12 items in

    each group were chosen to form the WAI. There has been good empirical

    support both for the overall scale and the subscales.

    Tracey and Kokotovic (1989) used a hierarchical bilevel model to

    represent the factor structure of the WAI, and they concluded that the

    WAI assesses both the individual aspects of the alliance represented by the

    subscales and the overall alliance dimension. Based on their findings, they

    selected the four items that best defined each of the unique aspects of the

    alliance (i.e. the three subscales) and constructed a 12-item short form of

    the WAI. It is this shortened version that is used as a part of the PSQ and

    provides the alliance ratings used in this study.

      utcome

     me sures

    Outcome for this study was measured using two factors established by

    calculating the standardized residual gains for each of six outcome mea-

    sures,

      the Global Assessment Scale ([GAS] Endicott, Spitzer, Fleiss,

    Cohen 1976), Global Symptom Index (GSI) of the Symptom Checklist

    90-Revised ([SCL—90—R] Derogatis, 1983), Inventory of Interpersonal

    Problems

      ([IIP] ,

      H oro w itz, Ro senberg, Baer, U reno Villasefior, 1988),

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    8/22

    AMERICAN

      J O U R N A L

      OF

      P S Y C H O T H E R A P Y

    analysis with Varimax rotation.

     Two

     factors were ex tracted with eigenval-

    ues exceeding

      1.00 and

      with

      5 9 . 1 1 %  of the

      variance accounted.

      Two

    outcome composites were calculated

     by

     averaging

     the

      standardized scores

    of

      the

      measures that loaded

      > .45 on the

      respective factors

      and by

    applying the  yielded factor scores as  weights. Factor one, which is consid-

    ered

      a

     m easure

     of

      symptom reduction,

      is a

      composite that included

     GAS

    (factor loading

     =

      -.78, factor score

     =

      - .36) ,

     PTC

     (.83, .41),

     TRC

     (.81,

     .40)

    and

      the GSI {.45, .12).

     Factor

      two, a

      measure

      of

      patients interpersonal

    functioning,

      is a

      composite that included

     the IIP

      (.82,), WIPSI

      .89, .61)

    and

      GSI .50, .23).

    STATISTICAL METHODOLOGY

      ata selection

    Since patterns

      of

      alliance development were

      the

      focus

      of

      this study,

    gaps

      in PSQ

      reporting could have been problematic.

      As a

      result,

      we

    included only cases

      in

      which

      the

      patient

      had

      completed treatment,

      and

    returned

      a

     minimum

      of

     6 6 %

     of the

     PSQ s, with

     no

     gaps

     of no

      more than

    three consecutive sessions.

     Of the 128

      original cases,

     44 met all of

      these

    criteria

      and

      were included

      in the

     analysis.

     The

     largest

     gap in

      consecutive

    sessions

     was

     three , w ith

     an

     average

     of

      one missed session.

     To

     allow

     for the

    consistent analysis

     of

     patterns across

     the

     length

     of

     therapy,

     all

     gaps

     in PSQ

    scores were filled

     in

     with

     the

     average

     of

     scores from

      the

     sessions be fore

     and

    after

      the

      skipped session

      s). For

     example,

     if a PSQ

      from session

      14

     were

    missing,

      the

     average

     WAI

     values

     for

      sessions

      13 and 15

      were substituted

    for session

     14.

    As

     it

      seemed possible that large gaps

      in the PSQ

      reporting might

      be

    indicative

      of

      ruptures,

      and

      because cases with large numbers

      of

      missing

    PSQ s could

     not be

     included

     in the

     analysis,

     it is

     possible that

     the

     rem aining

    cases might

      be

      representative

      of

      only good cases .

     To

      help control

      for

    this, the

     data

     was

     analyzed

      in

     several ways. First,

     we

     conducted

      a

     t-test

      to

    compare  the  percentage  of  PSQs returned  for  clients  who  completed

    treatment with those

     who

     dropped

     out of

      treatment.

     The

      test results were

    not statistically significant

      / =

      .423,

     p

    .673). Next,

     all

     cases

     of

      patients

    who completed treatment were examined

      for a

      correlation between

      the

    percentage

     of

      PSQs returned

     and

     outcome. Outcom e

     for

      symptom reduc-

    tion and  interpersonal functioning both at  termination  and follow-up was

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    9/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    Table 1. OVERA LL

    W AI

    Mean

    Standard

    Deviation

    Ilanjje

    Ont-

    5.24

    0.75

    3/>-6.7

    WAI MEAN

    Phase

    Two

    5.58

    0.72

    3.7-7.0

    AND STANDARD

    Three

    5.95

    0.69

    4 . ^ 7 . 0

    Three

    5.39

    0.86

    3.0-7.0

    DEVIATION SCORES

    Session

    Fifteen

    5.55

    0,85

    3.5-7.0

    Thirty

    6.0

    0.75

    4.3-7.0

    Overall

    Treatment

    5.59

    0.66

    4.1-7.0

    relationship was found for either symptom reduction or interpersonal

    functioning at termination or follow-up. Although the precise meaning of

    missing PSQs cannot be determined, the percentage of PSQs returned

    does not appear to be predictive either of length of treatment or of

    outcome.

    Data nalysts

    Before analyzing the impact of different  patterns  of alliance develop-

    ment on outcome, we needed to determine first if alliance

      level

      for this

    sample correlated with outcome. The treatment was first divided into

    phases, as suggested by the literature (eg., Mann 1973; Gelso and Carter,

    1994;

      and Horvath and Luborsky, 1993). Using bivariate Pearson corre-

    lations with a Spearman two-tailed test of significance, we compared

    • aUiance levels at the third , fifteenth, thi rtieth session;

    • average alliance levels for p hase on e (sessions on e to five),

    • pha se two (sessions six to twen ty-five), and ph ase thre e (twenty six

    through thirty); and

    • overall W A I mean -.

      ^

      -

    with outcomes for symptom reduction (factor one) and interpersonal

    functioning (factor two) at term ination (see Table 1 for des crip tion of

    statistics). As can be seen in Table 2, the mean WAI ratings for both

    factors were correlated significantly with outcome. With the exception of

    the ratings in phase one for interpersonal functioning, all of the mean

    ratings for the phases were correlated significantly to outcome. Ratings

    taken from individual sessions showed a weaker relationship with out-

    come, with only sessions 15 and 30 being significant. Interestingly, corre-

    lations were highest for phase two and session fifteen, indicating the closest

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    10/22

    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTHERAPY

    Table

    2 .

    WORKING

    ALLIANCE

    O ne

    BY OUTCOME

    Phase

    Two Three

    CORRELATIONS

    Session

    Three Fifteen Thirty

    Treatment

    Mean

    Factor Pearson

    One Correlation

    Sig. (2-tailed)

    Factor Pearson

    Two Correlation

    Sig. {2-tailed)

    - . 4 4 4 - . 5 1 6 - . 3 6 4 - . 2 7 7 - . 4 7 9 - . 3 2 7 - . 5 2 4

    .006 .000 .024 .092 .002 .044 .000

    - . 1 6 7 - . 3 2 9 - . 3 2 5 - . 0 6 3 - . 2 8 1 - . 2 5 5 - . 3 2 5

    .280 .030 .032 .682 .064 .094 .032

    range, while all of the correlations for factor two, interpersonal function-

    ing, were somewhat lower. Low scores for either factor indicate improve-

    ment, so that a negative correlation indicates that higher WAI scores are

    related to decreased patient symptoms or decreased interpersonal prob-

    lems.

    Identification of urves

    To test the hypothesis that both a linear trend and a series of ruptures

    and repairs are predictive of improved outcome, we examined each of the

    44 cases to determine if either of these two definitions described the

    pattern of alliance development, or if they were better described as

    high-low-high or other patterns. Testing was done in several ways. First,

    we used the mean WAI scores for each of the 44 cases to construct a

    Chance-corrected Coefficient of Proportionality Matrix. This correlation

    matrix allows cases to be examined for shifts across a series of cases,

    regardless of their abso lute value. This m eans that the sha pe of the alliance

    development may be examined independently of the level of the alliance

    scores. W e used W ard s m ethod to see if the cases wou ld cluster by pa ttern

    of developm ent. W ard s m etho d is a fairly conservative m eth od , which

    constructs fairly compact (minimum variance) clusters.

    Large breaks in similarity occurred to produce two dusters, each

    consisting of four highly similar cases. Examination of the two clusters

    indicated that both were forms of linear increase, with the first (r

     

    .46 to

    .87) cluster characterized by a steady, but gradual, increase across the

    length of treatment with little variation, and we called this pattem linear-

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    11/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    stable scores, and we labeled this late-linear. No other patterns of alliance

    development were identified using the cluster analytic technique.

     dentific tion

      of Alliance  Curves

    W e then employed a second m ethod to determine if linear or qu adratic

    patterns were present. Both linear and quadratic patterns can be detected

    using ANOVAs to describe regression curves. ANOVAs generated for

    each model were examined to determine if adding a quadratic term better

    described the shape of the alliance over time compared to a simple linear

    relationship.

    Since many potential curves could be associated with a positive finding

    for a quadratic relationship, the coders were asked to examine each of the

    curves and to determine if the curves were best described by a high-low-

    high, linear, or stable pattern. Four coders examined each of the curves

    and classified each as being high-low-high, linear increase, or other

    {Kappa = .90). To determine if a case could be categorized as having

    undergone a local period of rupture and repair as measured by the WAI,

    the same four coders were given graphs of each of the 44 cases and asked

    to identify rupture and repair cycles.

    A total of 29 cases (66%) were identified as having a linear trend by

    regression analysis with ANOVA (significant at the .05 level or better),

    indicating that WAI scores improved over the course of treatment. After

    examining each of the regression curves, coders identified 13 cases (30%)

    as being best categorized as having a linear trend. All of the cases identified

    by the coders, and all of the cases in each of the two clusters, were

    identified as having a linear trend in the ANOVA analysis. 

    dentific tion

     of Rupture and Repair Events

    Rupture-and-repair patterns were identified differently from more

    global trends (e.g., linear, high-low-high, etc.). Cases that demonstrated

    micro rupture-and-repair events are not easily detected by statistical

    procedures. Although these patterns have a distinctive shape, consisting of

    a dro p in alliance scores with a subsequ ent return , the length, severity, and

    timing of these episodes may be different in each case. It was, therefore,

    necessary to use coders to identify these rupture-and-repair episodes.

    If we define ruptu res in the therapeutic alliance as disagreements abou t

    tasks or goals or prob lems with the relational bond (Safran et al., 2002, and

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    12/22

    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTHERAPY

    F i g u r e 1

    LINE GRAPH OF WORKING ALLIANCE RATINGS (MEAN) BY SESSION.

    D   2 3 4  S  8 7 3 9  t O I 1 1 2 1 3 1 4 1 5 1 8 1 7 1 8 1 8 2 2 1 2 2 2 3 2 4 2 5 2 6 2 7 2 8 2 9 3 D 3 1

    S E S S IO N N U M B E R

    and-repair event as a downward shift in WAI ratings from stable (consistent

    or dropping no more than  5  point, or an increase in the previous session),

    with a drop of one WAI point in one session (moderate) or more than one

    point (serious) in one or more consecutive sessions (starting with an initial

    drop of  or more points), with a subsequent return to within .25 point of the

    predrop level or higher within 3 to 5 sessions.

    The same four coders who classified regression curves also identified

    rupture events. They were able to reliably identify cases as being rupture

    free, having only m ode rate rup ture s, or serious rup ture s (K appa ^ .91). A

    total of 22 cases (50 ) were cod ed as having rup ture s. Th irteen (3 0 ) of

    these were classified as having serious events. See Figure

     

    for an example

    of a case with a single rupture-repair episode, which starts in session 16

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    13/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    for which a linear trend was detected by the initial ANOVAs were

    categorized as significant lin ear . Tho se for which a linear trend was

    identified by the coders were categorized as co de r-r ate d linear . Because

    coders agreed about only two cases as being best described by a global

    high-low-high pattern, this category was not included in the final analysis.

    Cases identified as having local ru ptu re patte rns were categorized as any

    ruptu res . Tho se with only m odera te ruptures were categorized as only

    mo derate ruptu res and those with patterns of only serious ruptures were

    classified as only serious ru ptures .

    We ran ANOVAs, using the General Linear Model of SPSS (9.0), for

    each category to test the degree to which they related to outcome for factor

    one or factor two. Treatments categorized as linear by ANOVA and coders

    were not found to be related significantly to outcome on factor one.

    However, cluster 1 (the stable linear group) showed a relationship to

    improved outcome on factor one (symptom reduction) with a modest

    effect size

      {F

     (2, 44) ^ 4.497,

     p

    .045,

     r

    .31). Similarly, there was no

    significant relationship between linear development patterns as defined by

    ANOVA, coder or cluster analysis and factor two (interpersonal function-

    ing).  There also was no relationship between the presence of rupture-and-

    repair episodes and outcome on factor two.

    Since rupture-and-repair cycles, as operationalized here, were not

    significantly related to outcome, a secondary analysis was run to determine

    if variability in the alliance was predictive of outcome. To do this, the

    standard deviation for each patient's WAI scores was correlated with

    outc om e for each factor. A m oderately strong relationship was found

    between the standard deviation of the WAI scores and factor one

      ir

    .345,

      p —

      .042), indicating that a patie nt's im prove m ent on factor o ne

    (symptom reduction) is negatively correlated with variability on alliance.

    Factor two (interpersonal functioning) was not found to be significantly

    correlated with variability in WAI scores

      [r

    .028). Additionally, to

    explore the different findings between this study and Kivlighan and

    Shaugnessy (2000), an additional analysis was conducted. The Kivlighan

    and Shaughnessy (2000) study found that treatments with brief high-low-

    high patterns were positively correlated with outcome. This study found

    no such relationship. We conjectured that this discrepancy might be

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    14/22

    AMERICAN JOURNA L OF PSYCH OTHERA PY

    and Shaughnessy study. To determine if this difference in treatment length

    had an effect on the relationship between pattern development and

    outcome, the WAI scores for session 3 were subtracted from those of

    session 30. Eighteen of the 22 cases containing rupture-and-repair events

    had higher scores at session thirty than at session three, indicating that a

    majority of cases (81 ) with ru pt ur e- an d- re pa ir episodes ended w ith

    alliance scores as high or higher than hose with which they began.

    Treattnent Conditions

    In addition to looking for an overall relationship among alliance level,

    pattern of alliance development, and outcome, the data was also analyzed

    for relationships among the three treatment conditions and patterns of

    alliance development. No significant relationship was found between

    alliance levels and treatment group. A series of Chi-Square tests were run

    to test for relationships between type of alliance pattern developed with

    treatment group. The percentage of cases with ruptures was higher in the

    BRT gro up (a total of 10 rup tures in a total of 6 6 of the cases) than in

    either CB T or BAP condition (with nine rup ture s in 5 3 of the cases and

    3 ruptures in 3 3 of the cases respectively). Th e relationship app roach ed

    significance

      x^ -

      4.725,

     p =

      .094).

    There also was no significant relationship found between linear in-

    crease and treatment condition

      {)^ —

      .625,

      p =

      .732). There was a

    moderately significant relationship

      {r

      (2, 44) = .38,

     p =

      .042) between

    treatment condition and variability. The BRT condition was found to have

    significantly greater variability (M = .66) than BAP (M

      =

      .51) and CBT

    (M = .49). One additional finding is that the linear stable cluster of four

    cases found to be related to improved outco m e was m ade u p of three C BT

    and one BAP case.

    DISCUSSION

    The hypothesis that alliance development would be characterized by a

    linear increasing pattern and treatments demonstrating localized (three to

    five session) rup tu re -an d- re pa ir episodes was sup por ted. As predicted ,

    there was no evidence of a global high-low-high pattern of alliance

    development and decay. Rather, alliance development was characterized in

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    15/22

    Alliance Level and Pattem

    seen in BRT than in BAP or CBT, indicating that rupture-and-repair

    events may occur differently in different types of treatment.

    The hypothesis that treatments demonstrating a linear trend or rup-

    ture-and-repair episodes would be related to improved outcome, received

    limited support. No relationship between the presence of rupture-and-

    repair episodes and outcome was found. There also was no relationship

    found between a statistically significant linear trend or coder-rated linear

    trend and outcome, but one cluster of four cases classified as stable-linear

    was found to have a significant relationship to improved ou tcom e on factor

    one (symptomatology). Finally, variance in WAI ratings was found to be

    negatively correlated with outcome on factor one (symptomatology).

    There are a number of possible explanations for the failure to find any

    significant relationship between the presence of rupture-and-repair epi-

    sodes and outcome. First, the way in which ruptures in the alliance were

    operationalized in this study may not accurately reflect actual rupture

    events. Working from a definition that ruptures in the aUiance are char-

    acterized by disagreements about tasks or goals, or problems with the

    relational bond, we hypothesized the WAI scores, which dropped and

    then returned to pr e -d ro p levels, wo uld reflect this process. Although the

    classifications of moderate and serious rupture events were made to

    discriminate levels of disagreement, we did not consider the number or

    severity of rupture events in this analysis. Currently, there has been no

    theoretical prediction about the pattern alliance ruptures make. Safran and

    M uran (2000) state that the process of resolving rup ture s m ay, in fact, lead

    to further rup ture s. In describing the BRT m odel, Safran w rote, In this

    approach, the treatment process is conceptualized as ongoing cycles of

    therapeutic enactment, disembedding and understanding, enactment and

    disem bed ding (Safran, 2002, p. 171).

    Nevertheless, there is no clear notion of how many (or few) rupture

    events lead to imp rovem ent. It is possible to imagine cases in which patient

    and therapist encounter, and work through, a significant rupture around a

    core maladaptive interpersonal issue and then move on to other topics.

    Pe rha ps b eing able to survive this one conflict, o r feel understood, just this

    once,  is enough to lead to significant improvement. It is just as easy to

    imagine, however, that a patient may need to work through a particular

    conflict several times with a therapist before being able to experience it in

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    16/22

    A M E RICA N J O U R N A L O F P S Y C H O T H E R A P Y

    timing, and number of these events, based on a more detailed model,

    might yield a better understanding of how the pattern of ruptures may be

    related to outcome.

    The additional findings, variance in WAI ratings was negatively corre-

    lated with outcome, and only the most stable cluster of cases displaying a

    linear pattern was correlated with improved outcome, make interpretation

    more difficult. Regardless of the specific pattern that alliance development

    had been expected to take, be it the global high-low-high pattern or the

    more local, rupture-and-repair events predicted here, there is broad

    consensus among theorists that fluctuations in the therapeutic relationship

    are both unavoidable and form an important component of the change

    process. These theorists (e.g. Bordin, 1994; Forman & Marmar, 1965;

    H orvath and Luborsky, 1993; M uran 2002; Rhod es, Hill, Th om pson &

    Elliott, 1994; Safran, Crocker, McMain & Murray, 1990; Safran & Muran

    1994, 1998, 2000; Tracey, 1989) suggest that it is through these disagree-

    ments, misunderstandings, and conflicts, that patients learn critical lessons

    about how to balance the needs of the self and the needs of others. Tracey

    (1989),

      and Horvath and Luborsky (1993) suggest that when therapists do

      t

      notice periods of tension or disagreement, they should begin to

    question if the treatment needs to be changed, noting that it is important

    to be aware that during periods when therapy goes too sm oothly might

    represent a problem in the therapeutic work.

    The belief that having the experience of working through conflicts with

    an empathic and responsive listener is an essential part of therapeutic

    change, reflects years of clinical theory and experience backed by compel-

    ling anecdotal evidence. This has lead researchers to attempt to discover

    the way therapeutic technique interacts with the alliance and shapes its

    development. Finding patterns of alliance development consistent with the

    belief that a degree of conflict is an essential element of change has proven

    however, to be a difficult task. Studies that have found these patterns (e.g.

    Berna rd et al., 1980; Berna rd & Schw artz, 1981; Pa tton et al., 1997;

    Kivlighan & Shaughnessy, 2000) have been hampered by methodological

    prob lem s. Conversely, the finding that a strong, stable alliance is related to

    improved outcome (Joyce & Piper, 1990; Kivlighan & Shaughnessy, 1995;

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    17/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    little between-session variation is more conducive to improvement than a

    more dramatically fluctuating alliance.

    There are several possible explanations for this contradiction between

    the theory and the findings. First, the shape of the alliance may mean

    different things for different pa tien ts. As Bordin 1994) suggests, for som e

    patien ts, good outc om e will be associated with a stable alliance. Fo r oth ers,

    as Tracey 1989) suggests, an overly stable alliance repres ents collusion

    between patient and therapist to avoid difficult topics or over compliance

    by the patient. Similarly, the presence of rupture-and-repair events may

    mean different things in different treatments. For some patients, these

    events may represent the uncovering and working through of maladaptive

    interpersonal cycles, for others it may be more indicative of an inability to

    form or maintain a consistent relationship or engage in therapeutic work.

    If this is true, than rather than looking for globally generalizable patterns,

    investigators need to focus on factors that might cause particular dyads to

    work in the alliance in a particular way.

    Just as alliance patterns vary depending on the particular treatment

    dyad, there is some evidence in this study that the shape the alliance takes

    relates to treatment condition. There were marginally more cases with

    rupture events in the BRT than in BAP and CBT conditions. There was

    greater variability in BRT than CBT or BAP cases, and three of the four

    members of the stable, high-alliance cluster, which was found to be

    significantly related to im proved ou tcom e on factor on e symptom redu c-

    tion), were seen in the CBT condition. While these findings are certainly

    suggestive, the effect sizes were small and the relationships marginal.

    Cau tion should be taken in interpre ting these results. Bo rdin 1994)

    contends that different treatments will produce different forms of alliance,

    and presumably, different pattems of alliance development. It may be that

    BRT therapists are taught to focus on rupture events while CBT therapists

    are more likely to smooth over misunderstandings that may alter the

    pattern of alliance development. While the fmdings of this study indicate

    an area that should be investigated in further detail, there is not currently

    enough information to draw any real conclusions.

    It is possible that the way alliance and outcome were measured in this

    study was not sensitive to the kind of changes that come from working

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    18/22

    AMERICAN JOURNA L O F PSYCH OTHER APY

    within the session. Since the WAI can only look at shifts between sessions,

    rather than within them, it is possible that it is missing a critical part of the

    negotiation and change process. Researchers, such as Form an and M armar

    (1985), Lansford (1986), and Safran, Muran and Samstag (1994), who

    found that directly addressing strains, misunderstandings, and ruptures led

    to improvement in alliance scores and predicted improved outcome,

    looked at events that took place and we re address ed w ithin a given session.

    The WAI may not measure the kinds of relational factors central to the

    negotiating process. The WAI focuses on

      greement

      between patient and

    therapist. It is clearly vital for patient and therapist to agree on what they

    are trying to accomplish and how. This basic agreement on the central

    goals of a treatment—and the methods used to achieve them—maybe a

    fairly stable core of the therapeutic relationship. Shifts in this basic

    agreem ent may reflect an inability to establish a firm foun dation for

    treatment. What may be more important for some patients, however, is to

    learn that they can  dis gree with their therap ists without disrup ting the

    relationship. The elements of the therapeutic relationship that may be

    central to change may have much more to do with patients feeling their

    therapist is willing to listen, understand and respect them, even when

    expressing ang er, anxiety, disa ppo intm ent, hopelessness, etc. It may, there-

    fore, be less important that the patient and therapist work through

    conflicts about a particular task, than it is that the patient feel free to

    express his or her doubt, discomfort, or skepticism and believe that the

    therapist is taking him or her seriously. In these instances, a therapist's

    willingness to explain a task, or change it, may be the curative factor.

    Negotiations can take place in very brief moments, and may never be

    explicitly stated, and thus, could not be reflected by an instrument that

    measures problems lasting more than one session.

    Similarly, the outcome measures, which reflect standard measures of

    symptomatology and interpersonal functioning, may not reflect the kind of

    changes that would be brought about by working through alliance rup-

    tures. Safran and Muran (2000) suggest that working through ruptures,

    w hethe r explicitly or implicitly, . . . can have an impact on the fundamen-

    tal way in which patients con strue self-o the r intera ctions (p. 16). M uran

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    19/22

    Alliance Level and Pattem

    treatment, but provides opportunities for a unique kind of growth that is

    not reflected by standard measures.

    Results from this study seem to suggest that the level of the alliance, as

    measured by the WAI, rather than the shape of its development, is

    predictive of outcome. Given the limitations of the WAI in tracking

    interpersonal process, however, it may be more a function of the way in

    which alliance is defined. The WAI has clearly demonstrated that the

    alliance, defined by agreement on tasks, goals and bond, is a powerful

    component of change in therapy. It may be necessary to develop other

    tools to determine, with greater precision, how that change takes place.

    Acknowledgments:  The research presented in this article was supported in pan by Grsuit

    MH50246 from the National Institute of Mental Health.

    REFERENCES

    Alexander, L.B.. & Luborsky, L. (1986). The Penn Helping Alliance Scales. In G reen berg , L.S., &

      insof

    W.M. (Eds.),

     The psychotherapeutic process: A  research  handbook

      (pp. 325-366). New

    York: The Guilford Press.

    American Psychiatric Association (1994).  Diagnostic Systems Manual Fourth Edition.  Washington:

    Author.

    Beck A.T., Freeman, A.. & Associates (1990).   Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York;

    Guilford.

    Beck, J.S. (1995).  Cognitive therapy:  asics  and heyond. New York: Guilford Press.

    Bernard, H.S., Schwartz, A.J., Ocaltis, K.A,, & Stincr, A. (1980). The relationship between patients'

    in-process evaluations of therapy and psychotherapy outcome. Journal of Clinical  Psychology

    36 259-2664 .

    Bordin, E. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance.

    Psychotherapy: Theory Research and Practice 16  ( 3 ) , 2 5 2 - 2 6 0 .

    Bordin, E. (1994). Theory and research on the therapeutic working alliance: New directions. In

    Horvath, A.O., Greenberg. L.S., (Ed.), The Working Alliance: Theory research and practice (pp.

    13-37). New York: Joh n Wiley & Sons.

    Derogatis, L.R. (1983).  The Symptom Checklist -90 Revised: Adm inistration scoring and procedures

    manual U.  Baltimore: Clinical Psychometric Research.

    Derogatis. L.R., & Lazarus, L. (1994). SCL-90—R, Brief Symptom Inventory, and matching ciinica

    scales. In Maruish, M.E. (Ed.),  The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and

    outcome assessment  (pp. 217-248). Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence, Erlbaum.

    Derogatis, L.R., & Spencer, P.M. (1982),   Brief Symptom Inventory M anual. Baltimore: John Hop kins

    School of Medicine.

    DeRubies RJ., & Feeley, M. (1990). Determinants of change in cognitive therapy for depression.

    Cognitive Therapy and Research 14. 4 6 9 - 4 8 2 .

    En dico tt, J., Spitzer, R., Fleiss, J., & Coh en, J. (1976). Th e G lobal Assessment S cale.  Achieves of

    General Psychiatry H 166-171.

    First, M.B., Gibbon. M., Spitzer R.L., Williams, J.B.W., & Benjamin, L. (1997).

      Structured Clinical

    Interview for DSM-AV Axis   1 1   Personality Disorders. W ashington, D C.: American Psychiatric

    Publishing.

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    20/22

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    21/22

    Alliance Level and Pattern

    Horvaih (Ed.),  The working alliance: Theo ry, Research and Practice  (pp. 131-152). New York:

    John Wiley Sons.

    Rhodes, R.H., Hill,  ;.E., Th om pso n, B.J., Elliott, R. (1994). Client ret ro sp ea ive recall of resolved

    and unresolved misunderstanding events. Journal of  ounseling Psychology,  41  (4), 473 -483 .

    Safran. J.D. (2002). Brief Relational Psychoanalytic Treatment.  Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 12  (2),

    171-195.

    Safran, J.D ., Cro cker , P., McM ain, S.. Mu rray, P. (1990). Th erap euti c alliance rup ture as a therapy

    event for empirical investigation.  Psychotherapy,  27,  154-165.

    Safran, J.D., Mu ran, J.C. (1996). Th e resolution of ruptu res in the therapeu tic alliance.

     Journal of

    Consulting Clinical Psychology,  64, 4 4 7 -4 5 8 .

    Safran, J.D ., M uran, J.C . (2000).  Negotiating the therapeutic alliance: A relational treatment guide.

    New York; Guildford Press.

    Safran, J.D ., M uran , J,C ., Samstag, L.W . (1994). Resolving ther ape utic alliance rup ture s: A

    task-analytic investigation. In A,O . Ho rvath L.S. Gree nberg (Eds.),  The Working AUiance:

    Theory, research  and practice  (pp. 225 -255 ). New York: John Wiley Sons.

    Safran, J.D ., M uran , J . C . Samstag, L.W ,, Stevens, C. (2002), Repairing alliance rup ture s. In J.C .

    Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work (pp . 235-2 54). New York: Oxford Univereity.

    Schwartz, A.J., Bernard, H.S. (1981). Com parison of patient and therapist evaluations of T im e-

    Limited Psychotherapy.  Psychotherapy:  Theory, research  and practice. 18, 101-107.

    Tracey, T.J., Kokotovic, A.M., (1989). Factor structure of the W orking Alliance Inventory.

    Psychological Assessment, 1989  (1), 207-210.

    Turn er, A.E., M ura n,J.C . (1992). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for personality disorders: A treatment

    manual.  San Diag o. CA: Social Behavioral Do cum ents.

    W inston, A., Laikin, M., McC ullough (1988).

     Short-term dynam ic

     therapy

     for personality disorders:

    A treatment manual.  San Diego, CA: Social and Behavioral Documents.

  • 8/16/2019 Alliance Brief

    22/22