A Multiple Deficit Model of ADHD - AIDAI · PDF fileA Multiple Deficit Model of ADHD ... The...

55
A Multiple Deficit Model of ADHD Bruce F. Pennington Supported by NIH (HD-27802 and HD-049027)

Transcript of A Multiple Deficit Model of ADHD - AIDAI · PDF fileA Multiple Deficit Model of ADHD ... The...

A Multiple Deficit Model of ADHD

Bruce F. Pennington

Supported by NIH (HD-27802 and HD-049027)

Collaborators

Colorado Learning Disability Research Center (CLDRC)

Erik Willcutt Co-PI on ADHD Project

Dick Olson PI of CLDRC

John DeFries “Father” of CLDRC

Shelley Smith My longtime genetics collaborator

Jan Keenan Co-PI of Reading and Language project

Graduate Students and Post-Docs

Michelle Shanahan

Holly Barnard

Lauren McGrath

Laura Santerre-Lemmon

Cinnamon Bidwell at CU-Boulder

Cynthia Hartung now on faculty at University of Wyoming

OutlineI Single Deficit Models of ADHD

A. Executive Inhibition

B. State Regulation

C. Delay Aversion

II Why Single Deficit Models Fail

A. Review of Executive Inhibition Model: Inhibition deficit is associated, coheritable, but not pervasive in ADHD

B. Tests of Motivational Effects

1. Motivational Inhibition Tasks

2. Manipulating Incentives on the Stop Task

3. Delay Aversion

4. Orbital Frontal Tasks: Object Reversal and Gambling

III What is the Multiple Deficit Model?

IV Multiple Deficit Model Applied to Comorbidity of ADHD and RD

A. ADHD and RD have a genetic overlap

B. ADHD and RD have a cognitive overlap

V. Conclusions

Cognitive Mechanisms in ADHD

Distinctive cognitive Profile Douglas (1988)Impairments in vigilance,

systematic search, motor

inhibition, tasks without extrinsic rewards.

Not impaired on basic verbal

and nonverbal memory tasks.

Executive Deficits Pennington &

Consistent for MFFT errors Ozonoff (1996)and Motor Inhibition Tasks,

like Go No-Go, Stopping.

Not consistent for WCST, Fluency tasks

The Frontal Hypothesis in ADHD

Frontal Lobe Dysfunction

Pontius (1973)

Rosenthal & Allen (1978)

Stamm & Kreder (1979)

Gualtieri & Hicks (1985)

Zametkin & Rapaport (1986)

Mattes (1989)

Executive Dysfunction

Douglas (1983)

Conners & Wells (1986)

Pennington (1991)

Schachar, Tannock, & Logan (1993)

Pennington & Ozonoff (1996)

Barkley (1997)

Nigg (2000)

Executive Inhibition Hypothesis

Key Idea: The PFC-mediated process of voluntary motor inhibition is impaired in ADHD.

Marker task: Stopping task (Logan, Cowan & Davis, 1984)

Fundamental Questions:

1) Inhibition deficits are found in other disorders.

2) If someone fails to inhibit, is it because top-down control is too weak, or because bottom-up impulses are too strong, or both?

State Regulation Models of ADHDSergeant & van der Meere (1990)

Douglas (1989)

Key Idea: In contrast to executive inhibition model, holds that core problem is in maintaining optimal state for task. For instance, manipulation of event rate can lessen inhibition deficits.

Marker tasks: Reaction time (RT) and RT variability (SDRT)

Fundamental Questions:

1) Slower and more variable RTs are pervasive in developmental disabilities.

2) Neuroimaging studies of RT and SDRT implicate PFC.

Delay Aversion (DA)Sonuga-Barke (2005)

Sagvolden et al. (2004)

Key Idea: Prefer immediate small rewards to delayed larger rewards. Fall-off in reinforcement gradient is steep.

Related Constructs: Delay of gratification (Mischel, et al. 1989)Delay discounting (Green & Myerson, 2004)

Fundamental Questions:

1) Is DA more related to CD than ADHD? 2) How does DA deficit relate to state regulation and inhibition deficits?

Delay Discounting(Green & Myerson, 2004)

Five criteria that must be met for EF weaknesses to be considered

the core deficit of ADHD

I. Groups with ADHD must exhibit weaknesses on EF measures.

II. The group deficit must remain significant after controlling for IQ

and symptoms of other disorders.

III. The group EF deficit must explain a large proportion of the variance

in ADHD symptoms.

IV. EF weaknesses must be present in most individuals with ADHD,

and absent in most individuals without ADHD.

V. EF weaknesses must be due to the same genes as ADHD.

The EF theory of ADHD

A core EF deficit is necessary and sufficient to cause ADHD

(usually referring to the combined type)

Meta-analysis of EF and ADHD (Willcutt, Brodsky, et al., 2005; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005)

• One key measure of each core EF domain was identified

� Inhibition: Stop-signal Reaction Time (25 studies)

� Set shifting: Wisconsin Card Sort perseverative errors (25 studies)

� Verbal working memory: sentence span (3 studies) and digits backward (6)

� Planning: Tower of Hanoi / London (12 studies)

� Interference Control: Clinical Stroop (9 studies)

� Executive Processing Speed: Trailmaking Test Part B (13 studies)

• 100 new studies of EF measures have been published since the

review by Pennington and Ozonoff (1996)

• 65 studies that administered at least one of these six measures

were included (most studies combined type only)

• Total N = 3,374 with ADHD and 2,969 without ADHD

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Five Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

I. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Percentage of studies finding a significant difference

between ADHD and control groups(After Willcutt et al., 2005)

0

20

40

60

80

100

Inhibition Verbal

WM

Spatial

WM

Planning Shifting Executive

Speed

Pe

rce

nt

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

III. The EF deficit must be large enough to be a core

deficit.

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Mean effect size of the group difference on each EF

measure(After Willcutt et al., 2005)

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Inhibition Verbal WM Spatial

WM

Planning Shifting Executive

Speed

We

igh

ted

me

an

effe

ct siz

e (

d)

Comparison of the effect sizes for the hypothesized “core

deficit” in Reading Disability and ADHD

(Willcutt et al., 200128)

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

3.5

4

4.5

ADHD

symptoms

Inhibition

Eff

ect

Siz

e B

etw

ee

n G

roup

s

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

3.5

4

4.5

ADHD

symptoms

Inhibition Reading

Achievement

Phoneme

Awareness

Eff

ect

Siz

e B

etw

ee

n G

roup

s

Comparison of the effect sizes for the hypothesized “core

deficit” in Reading Disability and ADHD

(Willcutt et al., 2001)

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

Not SupportedIII. The EF deficit must be large enough to be a core

deficit.

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

IV. EF deficits must be present in most individuals with

ADHD and absent in most individuals without

ADHD.

Not SupportedIII. The EF deficit must be large enough to be a core

deficit.

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Percentage of individuals with scores above the 95th percentile

on the response inhibition factor(after Nigg, Willcutt, et al., in press12)

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

Pe

rce

nta

ge

of

ind

ivid

ua

ls

Comparison

ADHD

5%

48%

Few False Positives

Many False Negatives

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

Not supported

Supported

IV. EF deficits must be present in most with ADHD and

absent in most without ADHD.

Not SupportedIII. The EF deficit must be large enough to be a core

deficit.

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

V. EF deficits must be co-heritable with ADHD

Not supported

Supported

IV. EF deficits must be present in most with ADHD and

absent in most without ADHD.

Not SupportedIII. The EF deficit must be large enough to be a core

deficit.

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%

Mean across all studies

Willcutt et al., 1995

Waldman et al., 2001

Thapar et al. 2001

Sherman et al., 1997

Rietveld et al., 2003

Neuman et al., 2001

Nadder et al., 1998

Martin et al., 2002

Levy et al., 1997

Kuntsi et al., 2001

Hudziak et al., 2000

Goodman & Stevenson, 1989

Gjone et al., 1996

Edelbrock et al., 1995

Eaves et al., 1997

Variance Explained

heritability shared environment nonshared environment

Twin studies of individual differences in ADHD symptoms(N > 10,000 twin pairs)

* Significant after covarying ADHD symptoms

Familiality of Deficits

(Bidwell et al., in press)

Method: Compare unaffected DZ cotwins (n = 228) to control DZ twins (n = 332)

Eff

ect

siz

e (

d)

*

*

**

* *

*

Inhibition Verbal WM Shifting Processing Speed

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

SSRT CPT Sent Count Trails B WCST Cod CPS SDRT

Heritability of EF deficits in the Colorado Learning Disabilities

Research Center twin study(Willcutt et al., under review)

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

SSRT CPT Sent Count SOP WCST Coding Trails

He

rita

bili

ty

Inhibition Verbal Working Memory

Executive Speed

“Shifting”

*

*

***

*

Bivariate heritability of ADHD symptoms and EF factor scores

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

Inhibition Verbal WM Shifting Executive Speed

Biv

ari

ate

he

rita

bili

ty

DSM-IV inattention DSM-IV hyperactivity-impulsivity

**

*

*

Testing the EF model of ADHD:

Criteria for a core deficit

ResultCriterion

Supported, but

small common

genetic effect

V. EF deficits must be co-heritable with ADHD

Not supported

Supported

IV. EF deficits must be present in most with ADHD and

absent in most without ADHD.

Not SupportedIII. The EF deficit must be large enough to be a core

deficit.

SupportedII. EF weaknesses must not be explained by group

differences in IQ or comorbid symptoms.

SupportedI. ADHD must be associated with EF weaknesses.

Did diagnostic heterogeneity attenuate the effect?

Why didn’t we find a core EF deficit?

Predicted Double Dissociation between the Combined and

Inattentive Subtypes

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

1.2

Response Inhibition Processing Speed

Combined Type

Inattentive Type

Imp

airm

en

t

Performance of the DSM-IV Inattentive and Combined types on

measures of neuropsychological functioning(after Chhabildas et al., 20011; Willcutt et al., in press30)

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

1.2

Inhibition Verbal

Working

Memory

Processing

Speed

Vigilance Rapid

Naming

Mean e

ffect

Siz

e v

s.

Contr

ol G

roup

Combined Type

Inattentive Type

Meta-analysis of performance of the DSM-IV subtypes on measures of

neuropsychological functioning(10 studies; Willcutt et al., under review25)

-0.2

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

Inhibition Verbal

Working

Memory

Processing

Speed

Vigilance Rapid

Naming

Planning Motor

Mean e

ffect

Siz

e v

s.

Contr

ol G

roup

CombinedType

InattentiveType

Hyp/ImpType

Figure 3: Estimated Marginal Means of "Pure" ADD vs. Combined Type

-1.5

-1.25

-1

-0.75

-0.5

-0.25

0

0.25

0.5

0.75

1

1.25

1.5

Response Inhibition Working Memory

Z S

core

ADD Combined Type

A paradigm shift:

The need for a multifactorial model

• ADHD is not attributable to a single core deficit in EF or anything else.

• EF deficits are one important part of a model that includes several other

weaknesses.

• Other possible weaknesses:

– Delay Aversion: hypersensitivity to delay expressed as behaviors devoted to

minimizing the experience of delay (Sonuga-Barke, 2003)

– Arousal (“state”) regulation: fluctuations in arousal/activation lead to suboptimal

performance (Sergeant et al., 2003)

– Cognitive Speed

• Naming speed (Rucklidge & Tannock, 2002)

• Processing speed (Willcutt et al., in press)

– Temporal processing

• Durations > 2 seconds (Barkley et al., 2001)

• Durations < 1 second (Castellanos & Tannock)

• Some weaknesses may be shared with comorbid disorders and some may be

specific to ADHD.

Logistic regression predicting ADHD simultaneously

with all EF/neurocognitive composites(Willcutt et al., in preparation)

Composite Wald P

Inhibition 6.54 .01

Working Memory 6.31 .01

Vigilance 4.49 .03

Processing Speed 21.8 <.001

Number of neurocognitive deficits exhibited by

children with and without ADHD

(Domains assessed: inhibition, set shifting, processing speed, vigilance)

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

0 1 2 3 4

Number of Deficits (> 1 SD)

Pe

rce

nta

ge

of

ind

ivid

ua

ls

Comparison

ADHD

Tests of Motivational Effects in CLDRC

A. Motivational Inhibition (Hartung, in preparation)

1. Newman’s Go No Go Commissionsd= .38*

2. Doors task d= .15, ns

B. Manipulating Incentives on Stop Task(Shanahan et al., in revision)No group x incentives interaction

C. Delay Aversion : Effects in younger age group

D. Orbital Frontal Tasks (Object Reversal and Gambling)Evidence for multiple deficits

Effects of Incentives on Stop Task: SSRT (Shanahan et al., in review)

Main effect of Group (F (1, 54) = 5.12, p < .05

No Main effect of Reward

No Group by Reward Interaction

300

0

50

100

150

200

250

ADHD Controls

No Reward

Reward

530

550

570

590

610

630

650

ADHD Controls

Group

no-reward

reward

p<.05 p<.05

Effects of Incentives on Stop Task: Go RT(Shanahan et al, in review)

Group by Incentives InteractionG

oR

T

Delay Aversion Results in CLDRC: Age Differences

8-11 years

Controls (n=129) 31.9 (5.4)ADHD (n=65) 30.5 (5.1)

d= .26, p<.10

12-18 years

Controls (n=70) 32.9 (5.7)ADHD (n=29) 35.2 (5.2)

d= -0.43, p=.06

Group Differences in Executive and Motivational TasksTask F (1,70) p

value

Inhibition

SSRT 11.45 p<.001

GDS-Commission Errors 3.27 p=.07

Motivational

CGT- Quality of Decision Making 13.5 p<.001

CGT- Deliberation Time 3.89 p=.05

Object Reversal - Total Errors 5.2 p<.05

Object Reversal - Total Points 3.9 p=.05

Object Reversal - Total Trials 5.79 p<.05

Note. Children with ADHD performed significantly worse on all of these variables.

Note. When Full Scale IQ was covaried from the analyses, the same overall

pattern remained.

Single versus Multiple Deficits

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

45

ADHD Control

Group

Perc

en

t o

f G

rou

p w

ith

Defi

cit

Inhibition Only

Orbital Only

Double Deficit

Figure 2. Multiple Deficit Model

Pennington, 2006

Level of Analysis

Etiologic Risk and

Protective Factors

Neural Systems

Cognitive Processes

Complex Behavioral

Disorders

Non-Independence

at each Level

Gx E Interaction &

G-E Correlation

Pleiotropy

Interactive

Development

Comorbidity

G1 E1 G2 E2 G3

N1 N2 N3

C1 C2 C3

D1 D2 D3

KEY

G = genetic risk or protective factor, E = environmental risk or protective factor,

N= neural system, C= cognitive process, D= disorder

RD and ADHD are Comorbid: Why?

Rejected Hypotheses

• Not a selection artifact: Comorbidity found in population samples (eg Willcutt &

Pennington, 2000)

• Not a secondary phenocopy: Comorbid subjects have both EF and PA deficits(Willcutt et al, 2001), contrary to Pennington et al (1993)

• Not cross-assortment (Friedman et al, 2003)

Supported Hypothesis: Shared Etiological Influences

• Bivariate h2g for RD and ADHD (Stevenson et al, 1993; Light et al, 1995)

• Bivariate h2g for RD and Inatt is about .40, whereas NS for RD and HI(Willcutt et al, 2000)

• QTL for RD on 6p21.3 is also linked to ADHD and shows bivariate linkage

with RD phenotypes (Willcutt et al, 2002)

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

3.5

4

t

D6S

291

D6S

439

P = .001

D6S

461

D6S

276

D6S

105

D6S

306

D6S

258

D6S

1019

6pter 6cen5 cM

Linkage of ADHD to markers on chromosome 6

-2

-1

0

1

2

3

4

t

Orthographic Choice

Phonoloogical Decoding

Phoneme Awareness

Word Recognition

D6

S291

D6

S439

P = .01D

6S

461

D6

S276

D6

S105

D6

S306

D6

S258

D6

S101

9

Bivariate linkage of RD and ADHD to markers

on chromosome 6p

6pter 6cen5 cM

Genome Scans for Shared Risk Loci For RD and ADHD

Start with ADHD Sample, Test Linkage to RD

Lou et al. (2004)

Start with RD Sample, Test Linkage to ADHD

Gayan et al. (2005)

Possible Locations of Genes That InfluenceRD, ADHD, or both RD and ADHD

RD

R+A

R+A

RD

RD

RDRD

RD

R+A

RD

AD R+A

AD

AD

AD

R+A R+A

AD

R+A

R+A

RD

RD RDR+A

AD

MeasuresLatent Variable Measures Used to Predict Latent Variable_____________________________________________________________________________Reading Ability Time limited word recognition task, PIAT Reading Recognition,

& PIAT Spelling

Inattention Symptoms Mother, Father, Teacher, & Examiner Ratings

Hyperactive/Impulsive Symptoms Mother, Father, Teacher, & Examiner Ratings

PA Phoneme Deletion (% correct, blocks 1 & 2), Pig Latin test, & the Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization task

VR Information, Similarities, Vocabulary, & Comprehension fromthe WISC-R

WM Nonword Repetition, Digit Span (Forward & Backward), Sentence Span & Counting Span

Inhibition Gordon Diagnostic System commission errors (Vigilance & Distractibility), & Stop Signal Reaction Time from the Stop Task

PS WISC-R Coding, WISC-III Symbol Search, Colorado Perceptual Speed Task,Identical Pictures, Trailmaking Test, Rapid Automatized Naming Task (Colors, Numbers, Letters, & Pictures) & Stroop Task (Word Naming & Color Naming)

_____________________________________________________________________________________Note. For ADHD, mean severity ratings from each rater were used as the indicators. This strategy allows for more variance than the more typical

strategy of defining ADHD using symptom counts.

Note. Errors from the same instrument (e.g., WISC Coding and Symbol Search) were allowed to correlate in both measurement models.

ResultsMeasurement Model

The best fitting measurement model was one which created separate latent variables for the continuous symptoms of

inattention and symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity (χ2 /df= 2.303, CFI= 0.986, RMSEA=0.045). The measurement model for the latent variables of the the cognitive constructs was also a good fitting model (χ2 /df=

3.187, CFI= 0.915, RMSEA=0.059).

Full SEM ModelThe full SEM model was also a good fit (χ2 /df= 2.63, CFI= 0.918, RMSEA=0.05

PA

VR

WM

PS

Inhibition

Reading

Ability

Symptoms off

Inattention

Symptoms

of Hyperactivity/ Impulsivity

0.33**

0.28**

0.59*

-0.28*

0.46*

0.44**

Summary of Results and Discussion

� This model accounted for 82% of the variance in RD, 39% of the variance in

symptoms of inattention, and 22% of the variance in symptoms of

hyperactivity/impulsivity.

� Furthermore, it reduced the relationship between RD and inattention symptoms from

a zero-order correlation of -0.425 to a non-significant partial correlation of -0.08, which

implies that PS explains a significant amount of their overlap.

� It also reduced the relationship between RD and hyperactivity/impulsivity from -

0.215 to -0.12, suggesting that PS partially explains this relationship.

� Contrary to prediction, WM did not contribute uniquely to either RD or ADHD

symptoms.

� These results indicate that PS is a shared cognitive risk factor for RD and ADHD,

especially between RD and Inattention symptoms.

� Moreover, by using latent traits of symptoms of inattention and

hyperactivity/impulsivity and their neuropsychological predictors, we were able to

explain a much greater amount of the variance (24-39%) of the symptoms of ADHD

than is typically found in the literature (10-12%).

Correlations Among Constructs(N=444)

Inhibition Processing Speed SDRT

Delay Aversion .05 .05 .03

SDRT .46 .43Processing Speed .37

Bold = p<.01

Predicting ADHD Symptoms(N=444)

Adjusted R2 R2 Change p

Processing Speed .135 .137 .000Inhibition .174 .041 .000SDRT .187 .015 .006Delay Aversion .191 .006 <.10

Conclusions

1. No single cognitive deficit model of ADHD appears adequate.

2. DSM-IV subtypes are not cognitively distinct, nor is pure Inattentive subtype.

3. Some combinations of executive and motivational deficits appearpromising, but more work is needed.

4. A multiple cognitive deficit model helps explain ADHD’s comorbidity

with dyslexia.

5. More work is needed on relations among key constructs: executiveinhibition, state regulation, and delay aversion.