10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

download 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

of 13

Transcript of 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    1/13

    Eitig~a 14 (1966) l-13, Q

    No& i-H otEamS Pubzishing Co., Amsterdam

    Mot to be reproduced

    y photo@nt or microfflm without written permkion from the publisher

    THE ASSIMILATED LOW TONE IN YORUBA

    AYC) BAMGBC@E

    In the phonological analysis of vowel-initial disyllabic nouns in

    Yoruba, it is usual to have a two-tone contrast in the first syllable

    and a three-tone contrast in the second, i.e. a contrast between a

    -mid tone and a low tone in the first syllable; eg.

    $$ vehicle

    #&3 spear l)

    and a contrast between a high, a mid and a low tone in the second

    syllable, e.g.

    pkfi hoe

    pkp

    husband

    pkQ

    vehicle

    @d _enemy

    fita champion

    $2 place name.

    In a disyllabic noun having an initial I-: tone, some scholars2)

    j Tones

    are indicated as follows: high /I, lc;w 1.1, mid, (unmarked), and

    the assimilated low tone {see later) is indicated by a dot. The letter N after

    a vowel letter indicates that the vowel is nasalised. The letters Q, 9, Q stand

    for /3/, /E/, 1/j respectively.

    R. C. Abraham has an irLtia1 high tone in the worg @a equality. See

    Dectionary

    of Modern Yoruba, 1950,

    pp. xi; 507. In fact this word is

    pgba.

    This would have been considered a typing error wcx-e it not for a compound

    formation from this word cited as

    Z& b & ba

    equally (which should have

    been

    Z&bppgba). No

    vowel-initial disyllabic noun has a high tone in the first

    syllable. It is also often said that no vowel-initial noun has an initial high

    tone. Cf. Tda Ward,

    An l& o&u& on to the Yoruba Language, 1952, p. 26.

    Qa the cm&my,

    there is

    a

    tonal assimilation which results in an initial

    high tone in some nouns e.g. e&g& -9 tMg&rr nasquerade, o&o --f d&o

    boil,

    Btta + ta

    three.

    2) Ward ibid,

    p. 35,

    Abraham ibid, p. xi, and E. W. Stevick,

    Yovuba Basic

    ihum,

    1963 pp. xvii-xviii.

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    2/13

    AYO BA&tGBO$E

    set up a low-rising tone in the second syllable instead of the high

    tone. For example, they have $tdf instead of Gtd. In fact, the recog-

    nition of

    this additional

    tone is unnecessary, because there is no

    phonological contrast between this tone and a high tone: the

    low-rising tone only occurs immediately preceded by an initial

    BOW tone and the high tone only occurs immediately preceded by

    an

    initial mid tone. The difference in pitch between the two tones

    car be, and usually is, considered a phonetic difference. The

    exponent of a high tone which immediately follows a mid tone

    is a high level pitch and the exponent of a high tone which im-

    mediately follows a low tone is a low-rising pitch. For example,

    igbd calabash

    kgbagarden egg.

    Differences in the pitch exponents of the tones are not, however,

    limited to the high tone. Carnochan has pointed out that a mid

    tone immediately preceded by a low tone is regularly lower in

    pitch than a mid tone immediately preceded by another mid

    tone?) What is left to be pointed out is that it is not only the high

    tone and the mid tone that have different pitch exponents, but the

    low tone too has two different pitch exponents. For each tone, the

    pitch is conditioned by the immediately preceding tone. The fol-

    lowing minimal pairs of nouns exemplify these differences:

    1. (a) igbd calabash

    (b) &gbd garden egg

    2. (a) agbo circle

    (b)

    dgbo

    infusion

    3. (a) pgbd fence

    (b) &bd equal.

    An investigation of the pitch differences in these pairs of nouns

    was done on a spectrograph.*) The narrow band spectrograms of all

    the informants confirm that in the second syllable, 1 (a) has a level

    high pitch, and 1 (b) has a low-rising pitch; both 2 (a) and 2 (b)

    have a level pitch which is just a little lower than the pitch of

    3) J. Carnochan, Pitch, Tone and Intonation in Yoruba See I n HONOW

    of

    an i e l J ones

    (Edited Abercrombie and othkrs) 1964 pp. 402-403.

    41) The examples used in this investigation were spoken by Mr. AjimQkQ,

    Mr. Wfns&l&, Mr. AdGkun, all of the University of Ibadan, and myself.

    The s_pectrograms

    were

    made by Mr. John Kelly.

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    3/13

    THE ASSIMILATED LOW TONE IN YORUBA

    3

    1 (a). Contrary to expectation, there does not seem to be any

    noticeable difference in pitch between 2 (a) and 2 (b). On the

    other hand, Carnochans investigation on a pitch metre shows

    that

    2 (b) is lower in

    forms, to which

    a contraction, 2

    syllable of 3 (a),

    pitch than 2 (a). The spectrograms of contracted

    reference will be made later, also show that in

    (b) is strikingly lower than 2 (a). In the second

    all the spectrograms show a falling pitch starting

    at a low point; but in 3 (b), the pitch is low and level. In the first

    syllable, the low tone appears as a level low pitch or a falling pitch

    starting at a low point; but the mid tone appears as a level pitch

    jsimilax to 2 (a)) in all the spectrograms.

    In a grammatical structure where a vowel-initial disyllabic

    noun is preceded by a verb having a high tone, there is usually,

    except in deliberately slow speech, a contraction resulting in the

    replacement of the initial mid tone of the noun by the high tone

    nc +h

    b

    v1 cr*e veru

    ) e.g .

    d e ig b d d b a

    he wants a calabash

    d ft agbo -+ d fdgbo he wants a circle

    d f g b d+ 0

    &bd

    he wants a fence?)

    The tonal behaviour of the above contraction is agreed on by

    all scholars and confirmed by instrumental investigatlon.6) When

    the noun has an initial low tone, however, there is a difference

    of opinion among scholars as to whether there is a contraction,

    or jest an assimilation of one or the other of the two vowels in

    contact.

    Abraham, alone of all major scholars of Yoruba, recognises

    only an assimilation

    :

    When a high-to ed verb is followed by a

    noun-object, the latter having initial low-toned vowel, then verb

    and noun are linked together by means of a high-low falling tone

    which glides unbrokenly from high to low?)

    d

    f e

    gbd --+ d f&gbd

    he wants a garden egg

    d f# dgbo -+ d fadgbo he wants an infusion

    d fJ dgbb + d fddgbd he wants a ram

    6) For the rules governing the elision of vowels in these s~ruztures, See

    A. Bamgboge Assimilation and Contraction in Yoruba Jourrinl

    of est

    African Langtiages Vol. 2, no. 1, 1965, pp. 23-27.

    *) Scs Camochan ibid, p, 403 and

    the

    spectrographic investigation already

    referred to and further discussed below,

    7) Abraham ibid. p. xv.

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    4/13

    4

    AYQ BAMGBOSE

    In the spectrograms made for the above examples, only one out

    of four shows a high-falling pitch for the vowels in contact?)

    All

    the others show a high level pitch (indicating a contraction).

    Auditory impression of the pitch also indicates that it is pro-

    nounced level high by most informants. Practically all other

    scholars of Yoruba analyse the structure as a contraction resulting

    in the replacement of the low tone of the noun by the high tone

    of the verb. Even Abraham inadvertently recognises this con-

    traction in some cases. For example, I& instead of *Z&U from,

    jdkii instead of *jdM %it k&ii instead of *ldb,$ squat?) In the

    numeral series: m&j) two, m&z three,, . . . rrabw& ten etc.,lo)

    all scholars, including Abraham, are agreed that the first syllable

    has a high tone. But these words are contractions of the nouns

    & two,

    & a

    three

    . . . +wti

    ten etc. plus a preceding item

    hating a high fone

    (probably the verb m4 take). According to

    __ -1

    Abra%rns rules, the series should have been *~&jh, *m#&,

    . .

    %n#td

    etc.11)

    The implication of the contraction of the verb-nominal structure

    in the case of a low-tone initial noun is that a different phonological

    analysis must be made for this contraction in order to reflect the

    contrast between it and a contraction involving a mid-tone initial

    noun. In the pair 1 (a) and 1 (b) i.e. the nouns igbd calabash

    and igbli garden egg,

    we saw earlier on that the pitch difference

    in the second.syllable is phonetic and not tonemic i.e. the phonetic

    exponent of a high tone immediately preceded by a mid tone is a

    high level pitch whereas the exponent of the high tone immediately

    preceded by a low tone is a rising pitch. Since in the verb-nominal

    contraction involving both nouns, the initial tone of the noun is

    replaced by the high tone of the verb, the pitch difference in the

    second syllable of the nouns becomes the only contrastive feature

    of the contraction. This means that the rising pitch which is

    phonetic in the phonological structure of the noun now becomes

    8) Further investigation is needed to show whether this is a dialectal

    variation.

    *) See Abraham ibid. pp. 403, CL, CAL 110 respectively.

    10) See Abraham ibid. p. xxxii.

    11) A starred example is one that does not occur in Standard Yoruba.

    The starred numeral series are, however, found in some dialects of the

    language. For example, in the

    Ikale dialect.

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    5/13

    THE ASSIMILATED LOW TONE IN YORUBA

    5

    the exponent of the low-rising tone in the contraction e.g.

    b /6gM he wants a calabash

    d j+gM he wants a garden egg.

    The only scholars who do not have to introduce this new tone are

    those that already have it in their analysis of the nouns (i.e. igba

    calabash and 4gb& garden egg).

    The above solution is so obvious that it has been adopted by

    all scholars who have hitherto written on the language. The only

    difference of opinion is between those who have a low-rising tone

    on this syllable, and those who have a sequence of two syllables:

    a low followed by a high (i.e. 6 f&M as opposed to d ftgbh&),.

    In the phonological analysis of these contractions, most scholars

    have been concerned only with the contractions involving

    1 (a)

    and 1 (b) nouns. Rut the mn+ra=+c in

    . .

    ---.-. zLvwQ asa th CCdXM XE iilVOhiE~

    the other pairs of nouns are no less important. Ward was the first

    scholar to point out that the contractions involving 2 (a) and 2 (b)

    are contrastive, although she does not give a contrastive phono-

    logical analysis for the pair.*) Two scholar@) have since suggested

    that the phonetic contrast of the second syllable of 2 (a) and 2 (b)

    should be made phonological in order to account for the lexical

    contrast between the two nouns. This means that the mid tone in

    the second syllable of the contraction will contrast with a new

    tone, the lowered mid tone. For example,

    b /dgbo he wants a circle

    d tdgbd he wants an infusion 14)

    This analysis accounts for the contrast in this pair. But there

    is still the third pair. So far, no scholar has yet suggested a phono-

    logical analysis which will reflect the formal contrast in the pair.

    But any native speaker of Yoruba makes a distinction between

    a contraction involving 3 (a) such as in he wants a fence and a

    contraction involving 3 (b) such as in he wants an equal. To be

    consistent, therefore, our phonological arAysis should make the

    phonetic difference in the second syllable of 3 (a) and #3 b) phono-

    logical. This then means the introductioyl of a new tone - a low-

    12)Ward ibid, pp. 33-34.

    ,

    13 Stevick ibid. p. xvii;

    Carnochan ibid.

    pp.

    403-404.

    14) In this &ample, the lowered mid tone is indicated in the same way as

    Stevick does in his book (ibid. p. xvii).

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    6/13

    6

    AYQ BAMGBOSE

    falling tone in the second syllable of the contraction of 3 (a) con-

    trasting with a low tone

    in 3

    (b)

    or, to

    bring the analysis in line with

    that of

    the other two pairs, the low tone on 3

    (a) and a new tone on

    3 (b) e.g.

    d f&$d he wants a fence

    6

    fdgbd he wants an equaS.i~)

    We can now summarise the system of tones set up for the second

    syllable

    in these structures. In the nouns, there arc thl-ee tones:

    high, mid, low.

    In the verb-nominal contraction. there are six

    tone:

    :

    high, mid, low, low-rising, lowered mid, and new low. In

    the verb-nominal contraction, therefore, three tew tones axe

    introduced. The

    pitch exponents of these three tones are similar

    to the pitch exponents of the high, mid and low tones respectively

    when they are preceded by a low tone.16) The inference that we

    can draw from this is that in spite of the replacement of the low

    tone of the noun by the high tone of the verb in a verb-nominal

    contraction, the tonal behaviour of the second syllable continues

    to be as if it was still preceded by the low tone. Ward was aware

    of this when, in talking about the lowered mid tone of the con-

    traction, she wrote, The explanation of this would seem that the

    suppressed low tone of Me has pulled down the mid tone syllable

    of the second syllable . . . The second mid tone, therefore, is not

    an independent tone but is conditioned, ;;ts shown above, by the

    elision 01 a preceding low-tone syllable. Although Wards obser-

    vation 011 this feature iq limited to the lowered mid tone, the same

    observaton is, of course, also true of the other two tones of the

    contraction: they are also conditioned by the elision of a preceding

    low tone. Looking at the phonological structure of the contraction

    from this point of view, we can set up a completely new structure

    for the contraction involving a noun having an init5.l low tone

    syllable. A prosodyl7) to be called an assimiiated lo-~ tone would

    15) The symbol used here is an arbitrary one to indicate the new tone

    required in the phonological analysis,

    16) B. Siertsema in Stress and Tone in Vxuba Vord Composition

    Lingua 8, 1959 mentions the three pitch oxponents of the second syllable

    of the contraction, but she does not consider two of them phonological.

    See p. 398, note 16.

    17) See J. T. Bendor-Samuel, The VwbaZ Piece in J& two (Word, Mono-

    graph No. 4) 1961, p. 16:

    Prosodic features are phonological elements

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    7/13

    THE SSIMIL TED LOW TONE IN YORUB

    7

    be set up for this contraction. This prosody will operate at the

    point where a low tone syllable has been elided; and the syllable

    immediately following it will have the same pitch exponents as a

    syllable immediately following a low tone syllable.I*) For example,

    using a dot to indicate the assimilated low tone, the three pairs of

    nouns given earlier will be contrasted in contractions a follows:

    ((5 igb8) 0 /&bd

    he wants a &abash

    (b feJgbb) d f&gbd

    he wants a garden egg

    (6 f4 agbo) d frigbo

    he wants a circle

    (Q t dgbo) d fkgbo he wants an infusion

    (d

    f; pgba)d ffigbd

    he wants a fence

    (b fe $gbd) 6 f#.gbb

    he wants an equal?)

    The advantages of this phonological analyzes are obvious.

    Instead of three additional tones, there is only one prosodic feature;

    and the differences in the pitch exponents of the three tones are

    phonetic both in the nouns and in the contractions?)

    It has already been pointed out that in a verb-nominal conti,c.-

    tion, the low tone is replaced by a high tone. This feature is also

    true, for the following:

    having phonetic exponents which either extend over more than one place in

    the syllable or have implications over more than one place in the sjrllable.

    The prosodic approach to phonology was introduced by J. R. Firth. See

    Sounds and Prosodies in his

    Plapevs in

    Linguistics, 1957,

    pp.

    121-l 38.

    18) The idea of this kind of phonological analysis was first mentioned by

    the writer at the Second West African Languages Longress

    held in Daltar

    in A?I+ , 1962. See

    Actes du Second Colloque

    Intelvrtational de Linguistique

    Nkgvo-A,iric&e 1963, p* 123.

    19) When whistled,

    the pitch of the high tone in the first syllable of a

    contraction having an assimilated low tone appears higher than that of the

    corresponding high tone in the other contraction. One set of spectrograms

    of the whistling by one of the informants shows this consistently; but there

    is no such consistency in the others. The low tone in the second syllable of

    both contractions is shown qn th@ spectrograms as a falling pitch (cf. Stevick

    ibid. p. 128), but the fall is much sharper in the case of a mid-tone initial

    noun.

    20) Fro- the spelling point of view, an additional advantage is that the

    awkward sequence of three vowel letters in some words would be reduced

    to two e.g.

    kdddv~ (< kz2

    ddrb) good morning wil be written as kd.dY6;

    gbi5bhn (< gbb

    &.5&n

    lit.

    hear smell) smell will be written as gbb.dYh

    For

    those scholars who in fact set up four syllables for these words, the

    reduction to three syllables is also a more economical phonological analysis.

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    8/13

  • 7/24/2019 10.1016@0024-38416690014-3

    9/13

    THE ASSIMILATED LOW TONE IN YORUBA

    9

    e.g. (dgbd ram) hgbii kt z.gbd any ram

    (M plan)

    Wke. tb

    I*

    any plan

    (iii) The ordinal numeral series (cf. (i) immediately above)

    e.g.

    Lke.ji/ike. qi

    the second

    (< %kd t $ taking of two)

    kg.tu/ikp.ta

    the third (