1-s2.0-S0304395914003765-main

download 1-s2.0-S0304395914003765-main

of 2

description

sdcmdjkmdcnc

Transcript of 1-s2.0-S0304395914003765-main

  • an

    ences in cortical and subcortical activity. In general, widespreaddeactivations were observed in a number of classical pain-related

    pattern of neurobiological activity in areas involved in pain per-ception and modulation. More importantly, the study adds tothe discussion about the methods and implications of different

    regions reported in recent imaging studies (ie, PAG, Anterior Cin-gulate Cortex [22,24]). Thus, depending on the presentation of the

    r in termxperimen

    antagonism [18], a nding that contrasts with OA, which appearsto be resistant to these manipulations [14,19,18]. Recently, similardifferences were observed with tapentadol [20], a dual l-opioidreceptor agonist and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor. Severalweeks of treatment restored the inhibitory capacity assessed byCPM in patients with diabetic polyneuropathy but not affect OA,suggesting differential neuromodulatory tone underlying eachparadigm.qDOI of original article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.008

    PAIN155 (2014) 244dependent on the spatial (CPM) and temporal (OA) presentationof nociceptive information, which in turn results in a differential

    pharmacological manipulations, CPM appears to be sensitive toopioids [9,21,26] and the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptorareas during CPM, which was contrasted by activation in a num-ber of areas including the brain stem region associated with theperiaqueductal gray (PAG) during the OA paradigm. Based onthe current study, differences in the modulation of pain are

    engaged.In addition, CPM and OA paradigms can diffe

    underlying neurotransmitter system. Based on ehttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.08.0170304-3959/ 2014 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.s of thets withtesting and conditioning stimuli, different mechanisms may beCommentary

    Conditioned pain modulation and offsetinhibit pain

    The ability of the body to modulate sensory information hasbeen a focus of a number of studies over the past several years.Experimentally, pain modulation can be assessed by a number ofmethods including conditioned pain modulation (CPM) and offsetanalgesia (OA) as highlighted in the current study by Nahman-Averbuch et al. [15]. CPM is frequently demonstrated through thereductions of a primary painful stimulus (ie, focal heat) by a secondremote conditioning stimulus (ie, cold water immersion[6,9,17,22,23]). In contrast, OA is demonstrated by using a pro-longed thermal pulse in which a slight reduction in the thermodetemperature (ie, 1C) is associated with a transient (10 s) reduc-tion in pain intensity [7,15,16,18,29,30]. Although only a smallnumber of studies have evaluated CPM and OA in the same cohort[8,17,18,20], the combined use of these inhibitory models may pro-vide a glimpse into different characteristics underlying an individ-uals inhibitory phenotype, which could have implications forfuture experimental and clinical research.

    The study by Nahman-Averbuch et al. [15] was designed toinvestigate differences between the psychophysical and neuralcorrelates of CPM and OA. Using the same thermal stimulationparadigm (ie, 30 s prolonged pulse at 49C applied to the leg),the studies demonstrated reductions in the subjective ratings ofheat pain during both concurrent immersion of the contralateralfoot into moderately painful cold water bath (CPM induction)and following a 1C temperature drop (OA induction)observa-tions that are in agreement with those of previous studies[6,7,9,10,15,16,23,30]. However, the magnitude of inhibitionobserved in the 2 models did not correlate suggesting differentmechanisms underlying inhibition. Conrming the psychophysi-cal data, fMRI assessment of each paradigm demonstrated differ-algesia: Different avenues to

    inhibitory mechanisms (CPM vs OA) and within different inhibi-tory paradigms (variations of CPM).

    What does the current study tell us about pain modulation?

    The ndings of the study suggest that experimental manipula-tions, which result in comparable magnitudes of subjective inhibi-tion, may be a consequence of different neurobiologicalmechanisms. This concept is neither surprising nor unexpected,but the current study demonstrates the complexity of nervoussystem in regulating sensory information through multipleneurological structures and neurotransmitters depending on thepresentation of the stimulus.

    Interestingly, the authors comment that inhibition during CPMappears to be due to spinal inhibition rather than descendingmodulation. CPM reects the psychophysical representation [28]of the previously described phenomenon termed diffuse noxiousinhibitory control, referring to observations by Le Bars and col-leagues in which activity within the lower aspect of the brainstem suppresses activity within the spinal cord [11,12], whichcan be indirectly modulated by activity within the PAG [2] andthe rostral ventromedial medulla [1]. In contrast, OA is associatedwith increased activity within the PAG and rostral ventromedialmedulla [4,29], although a peripheral component may also exist[16]. Thus, CPM represents a subjective response to a range ofpossible mechanisms, which may include the originally proposedspino-bulbo-spinal loop described by Le Bars and colleagues, aspinal-mediated mechanism proposed by Nahman-Averbuchet al. [15], as well as brain stem, and/or higher-order cortical

    www.e l sev ie r . com/ loca te /pa in

    42445

  • What are the clinical implications of multiple pain modulatorymechanisms?

    The current study also has clinical implications for the evalua-tion of pain modulation models. Although a reduced capacity toinhibit pain during CPM is commonly observed in clinical pain

    including CPM (ie, different conditioning stimuli, temporal and

    [8] Honigman L, Yarnitsky D, Sprecher E, Weissman-Fogel I. Psychophysicaltesting of spatial and temporal dimensions of endogenous analgesia:conditioned pain modulation and offset analgesia. Exp Brain Res 2013;228:493501.

    [9] King CD, Goodin B, Kindler LL, Caudle RM, Edwards RR, Gravenstein N, Riley3rd JL, Fillingim RB. Reduction of conditioned pain modulation in humans bynaltrexone: an exploratory study of the effects of pain catastrophizing. J Behav

    Commentary / PAIN155 (2014) 24442445 2445spatial presentation of stimuli) alter these neurological proles inaddition to how these mechanisms differ in individuals withchronic pain. I anticipate that the current study will encourageadditional investigation into the neurobiological mechanismsunderlying inhibition.

    Conict of interest

    The author is aware of no conicts of interest regarding thiscommentary.

    References

    [1] Bouhassira D, Chitour D, Villanueva L, Le Bars D. Morphine and diffuse noxiousinhibitory controls in the rat: effects of lesions of the rostral ventromedialmedulla. Eur J Pharmacol 1993;232:20715.

    [2] Bouhassira D, Villanueva L, Le Bars D. Effects of systemic morphine on diffusenoxious inhibitory controls: role of the periaqueductal grey. Eur J Pharmacol1992;216:14956.

    [3] Cruz-Almeida Y, Fillingim RB. Can quantitative sensory testing move us closerto mechanism-based pain management? Pain Med 2014;15:6172.

    [4] Derbyshire SW, Osborn J. Offset analgesia is mediated by activation in theregion of the periaqueductal grey and rostral ventromedial medulla.Neuroimage 2009;47:10026.

    [5] Garrett PH, Sarlani E, Grace EG, Greenspan JD. Chronic temporomandibulardisorders are not necessarily associated with a compromised endogenousanalgesic system. J Orofac Pain 2013;27:14250.

    [6] Goodin BR, Kroni T, King CD, Glover TL, Sibille K, Fillingim RB. Testing therelation between dispositional optimism and conditioned pain modulation:does ethnicity matter? J Behav Med 2013;36:16574.

    [7] Grill JD, Coghill RC. Transient analgesia evoked by noxious stimulus offset. JNeurophysiol 2002;87:22058.populations (for review, see [27]), only 1 study has reported areduction in OA in neuropathic pain patients [19]. Thus, it isunclear whether a reduction in OA is also observed in other paincohorts, or if one or both methods exhibit comparable inhibitorydecits. Considering recent evidence that the efciency of CPM[5,13] and other modulatory mechanisms [25] appears to be intactin patients, the evaluation of multiple paradigms may provideadditional information about the inhibitory phenotype of the par-ticipant [3]. For example, a proportion of patients may have anintact descending pain system as assessed by CPM, but they mayfail to engage neurological mechanisms underlying OA. Whilebalancing the burden to the participant, studies could incorporatea multiple step evaluation of several inhibitory paradigms, whichcould include incorporating assessments of both CPM and OAparadigms within a testing protocol. The use of CPM and OA mayindicate a selective or widespread dysfunction of inhibitorymechanism, and imaging will assist in this investigation.

    In sum, the study by Nahman-Averbuch et al. [15] adds to thecurrent literature regarding the neurobiological mechanismsinvolved in pain inhibition. In order to understand these mecha-nisms better, additional research is needed to determine if meth-odological factors including variations of testing paradigmsMed 2013;36:31527.[10] King CD, Wong F, Currie T, Mauderli AP, Fillingim RB, Riley 3rd JL. Deciency in

    endogenous modulation of prolonged heat pain in patients with irritablebowel syndrome and temporomandibular disorder. PAIN 2009;143:1728.

    [11] Le Bars D. The whole body receptive eld of dorsal horn multireceptiveneurones. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2002;40:2944.

    [12] Le Bars D, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls(DNIC). I. Effects on dorsal horn convergent neurones in the rat. PAIN 1979;6:283304.

    [13] Martel MO, Wasan AD, Edwards RR. Sex differences in the stability ofconditioned pain modulation (CPM) among patients with chronic pain. PainMed 2013;14:175768.

    [14] Martucci KT, Eisenach JC, Tong C, Coghill RC. Opioid-independent mechanismssupporting offset analgesia and temporal sharpening of nociceptiveinformation. PAIN 2012;153:123243.

    [15] Nahman-Averbuch H, Martucci KT, Granovsky Y, Weissman-Fogel I, YarnitskyD, Coghill RC. Distinct brain mechanisms support spatial vs temporal lteringof nociceptive information. PAIN 2014;155:2491501.

    [16] Naugle KM, Cruz-Almeida Y, Fillingim RB, Riley 3rd JL. Offset analgesia isreduced in older adults. PAIN 2013;154:23817.

    [17] Naugle KM, Riley 3rd JL. Self-reported physical activity predicts pain inhibitoryand facilitatory function. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2014;46:6229.

    [18] Niesters M, Dahan A, Swartjes M, Noppers I, Fillingim RB, Aarts L, Sarton EY.Effect of ketamine on endogenous pain modulation in healthy volunteers.PAIN 2011;152:65663.

    [19] Niesters M, Hoitsma E, Sarton E, Aarts L, Dahan A. Offset analgesia inneuropathic pain patients and effect of treatment with morphine andketamine. Anesthesiology 2011;115:106371.

    [20] Niesters M, Proto PL, Aarts L, Sarton EY, Drewes AM, Dahan A. Tapentadolpotentiates descending pain inhibition in chronic pain patients with diabeticpolyneuropathy. Br J Anaesth 2014;113:14856.

    [21] Pertovaara A, Kemppainen P, Johansson G, Karonen SL. Ischemic pain non-segmentally produces a predominant reduction of pain and thermal sensitivityin mana selective role for endogenous opioids. Brain Res 1982;251:8392.

    [22] Piche M, Arsenault M, Rainville P. Cerebral and cerebrospinal processesunderlying counterirritation analgesia. J Neurosci 2009;29:1423646.

    [23] Riley 3rd JL, King CD, Wong F, Fillingim RB, Mauderli AP. Lack of endogenousmodulation and reduced decay of prolonged heat pain in older adults. PAIN

    2010;150:15360.[24] Sprenger C, Bingel U, Buchel C. Treating pain with pain: supraspinal

    mechanisms of endogenous analgesia elicited by heterotopic noxiousconditioning stimulation. PAIN 2011;152:42839.

    [25] Staud R, Robinson ME, Goldman CT, Price DD. Attenuation of experimentalpain by vibro-tactile stimulation in patients with chronic local or widespreadmusculoskeletal pain. Eur J Pain 2011;15:83642.

    [26] Willer JC, Le Bars D, De Broucker T. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in man:involvement of an opioidergic link. Eur J Pharmacol 1990;182:34755.

    [27] Yarnitsky D. Conditioned pain modulation (the diffuse noxious inhibitorycontrol-like effect): its relevance for acute and chronic pain states. Curr OpinAnaesthesiol 2010;23:6115.

    [28] Yarnitsky D, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bouhassira D, Edwards RR, Fillingim RB, GranotM, Hansson P, Lautenbacher S, Marchand S, Wilder-Smith O. Recommendationson terminology and practice of psychophysical DNIC testing. Eur J Pain2010;14:339.

    [29] Yelle MD, Oshiro Y, Kraft RA, Coghill RC. Temporal ltering of nociceptiveinformation by dynamic activation of endogenous pain modulatory systems. JNeurosci 2009;29:1026471.

    [30] Yelle MD, Rogers JM, Coghill RC. Offset analgesia: a temporal contrastmechanism for nociceptive information. PAIN 2008;134:17486.

    Christopher D. KingUniversity of Florida Pain Research and Intervention Center of Excellence

    (PRICE), Gainesville, FL 32610, USAE-mail address: [email protected]

    Conditioned pain modulation and offset analgesia: Different avenues to inhibit painWhat does the current study tell us about pain modulation?What are the clinical implications of multiple pain modulatory mechanisms?Conflict of interestReferences